When learning material that differs with respect to importance, leaners tend to remember more-important over less-important material (Castel et al., 2009; 2011; 2013). For instance, when studying words that differ by how many points (e.g., 1-10) each will be worth on an upcoming test, learners differentially encode and better remember higher-value items (e.g., FENCE: 10, SWIM: 9) compared to lower-value items (e.g., APPLE: 1, WEIGHT: 2); this effect is known as value-directed remembering (VDR; see Knowlton et al., 2022 for a review). The focus of most of the work in this field has been on memory outcomes, and in only a few instances have researchers explored the impact of value on learners’ self-regulated learning. Prior work has shown that learners tend to spend more time studying high-value word pairs over lower-value word pairs (Soderstrom & McCabe, 2011, Experiment 2) and choose to restudy high-value single words more often than lower-value single words (Middlebrooks & Castel, 2018, Experiment 2). Research with basic materials is informative; however, much less is known about how value influences learners’ study choices with educationally-relevant materials. In day-to-day life, a common scenario in which one may encounter value-associated information is when using a scoring rubric. To illustrate, instructors might use a rubric to score a writing assignment for a class, and this rubric may indicate, for example, that the introduction section is worth 30 out of a possible 100 points on the assignment, whereas the method section is worth 20 points. Some prior work has found that giving learners access to the rubric by which their assignment will be scored increases learners’ self-reported, self-regulated learning. Specifically, compared to learners who do not have access to a rubric during preparation, learners who do report engaging in more monitoring, planning, and evaluating of their learning during study (Panadero & Romero, 2013). Thus, making rubrics available to learners as they prepare for an assignment may benefit them by making the expectations for that assignment clear, allowing learners to plan their approach accordingly (Panadero, Tapia, & Huertas, 2013; Reddy & Andrade, 2010). As stated by Panadero and Jonsson (2020), the majority of research on how learners use rubrics has relied on self-report measures and personal anecdotes. Moreover, there exists a wide variety of type and quality of rubrics used in prior work, with some rubrics containing subjective criteria (e.g., “all or most of the instruction involves active engagement on the part of the students”), some containing value-based information (e.g., “active engagement is worth 4 points”; Andrade & Du, 2005), and some containing script-like, scaffolded instructions (e.g., “underline key phrases in the rubric with colored ink, then, underline evidence in your draft as having met the standard with the same color ink”; Andrade, 2010; 2017). The current study, therefore, aims to systematically measure how value as provided by a rubric influences self-regulated learning. By doing so, we will apply the VDR framework to a new, realistic context: using a rubric to prepare for a writing assignment. Specifically, we will examine how rubrics that differ with respect to the information they contain regarding points, influence learners’ studying decisions as they prepare for a writing assignment about mineral formation. Participants will be randomly assigned to one of four rubric groups: 1. Varied points rubric: two high-point (12 point) concepts, one medium-point (8 points) concept, and two low-point (4 points) concepts 2. Reverse-varied points rubric: same as varied points rubric but with opposite high-point (12 points) and low-point (4 points) concepts 3. Constant points rubric: all concepts associated with medium-points (8 points) 4. No points rubric: no points provided (only concept names appear) During the task, participants will learn about the process of mineral formation by reading about 5 concepts (from Ariel et al., 2021) in anticipation of completing a writing assignment. Participants can spend as much time as they want preparing for and completing the writing assignment. While preparing for the writing assignment, participants will see a rubric, 5 concept names, and the question, “which concept would you like to study?”. Participants can choose to study any of the 5 concepts by clicking on its name. Once a concept has been selected, a brief description of the concept will appear on the screen, and participants can spend as much time reading it as they want. For example, if participants select “Compounds”, they will be directed to a page containing a paragraph about compounds (in the context of mineral formation). When participants are finished studying a concept, they will be redirected to the choice-screen. After studying at least one concept, the choice-screen will also have an option that allows participants to move onto the writing assignment. Participants can study any concept they want, in any order they want, and revisit a concept as many times as they want. Beyond the requirement that participants must study one concept, participants can also choose to skip any concept (i.e., they can skip 4 of 5). On the writing assignment, which will be self-paced, participants will be asked to write as much as they can about the process of mineral formation underneath each of the 5 concept names. Following the writing assignment, participants will answer a series of self-report questions such as, “how did you determine you were ready for the writing assignment”, as well as a series of demographic questions.