Back to Search
Start Over
Brightness and Darkness as Perceptual Dimensions
- Source :
- PLoS Computational Biology, PLoS Computational Biology, 3(10):e0030179, 1849-1858. PUBLIC LIBRARY SCIENCE, PLoS Computational Biology, Vol 3, Iss 10, p e179 (2007)
- Publication Year :
- 2007
- Publisher :
- Public Library of Science (PLoS), 2007.
-
Abstract
- A common-sense assumption concerning visual perception states that brightness and darkness cannot coexist at a given spatial location. One corollary of this assumption is that achromatic colors, or perceived grey shades, are contained in a one-dimensional (1-D) space varying from bright to dark. The results of many previous psychophysical studies suggest, by contrast, that achromatic colors are represented as points in a color space composed of two or more perceptual dimensions. The nature of these perceptual dimensions, however, presently remains unclear. Here we provide direct evidence that brightness and darkness form the dimensions of a two-dimensional (2-D) achromatic color space. This color space may play a role in the representation of object surfaces viewed against natural backgrounds, which simultaneously induce both brightness and darkness signals. Our 2-D model generalizes to the chromatic dimensions of color perception, indicating that redness and greenness (blueness and yellowness) also form perceptual dimensions. Collectively, these findings suggest that human color space is composed of six dimensions, rather than the conventional three.<br />Author Summary Vision scientists have long adhered to the classic opponent-coding theory of vision, which states that bright–dark, red–green, and blue–yellow form mutually exclusive color pairs. According to this theory, it is not possible to see both brightness and darkness at a single spatial location, or an extended set of locations, such as a uniform surface. One corollary of this statement is that all perceivable grey shades vary along a continuum from bright to dark. At first glance, the notion that brightness and darkness cannot coexist on a single surface accords with our common-sense notion that a given grey shade cannot be simultaneously both brighter and darker than any other grey shade. The results presented here suggest that this common-sense notion is not supported by experimental data. Our results imply that a given grey shade can indeed be simultaneously brighter and darker than another grey shade. This seemingly paradoxical conclusion arises naturally if one assumes that brightness and darkness constitute the dimensions of a two-dimensional perceptual space in which points represent grey shades. Our results may encourage scientists working in related fields to question the assumption that perceptual variables, rather than sensory variables, are encoded in opponent pairs.
- Subjects :
- Male
Brightness
Light
0302 clinical medicine
COLOR APPEARANCE
Perceptual Closure
Homo (Human)
Task Performance and Analysis
FILLING-IN
Contrast (vision)
Computer vision
Biology (General)
Mathematics
media_common
REAL OBJECTS
Ecology
CONTRAST ASYNCHRONIES
05 social sciences
Darkness
LUMINANCE
EDGE INTEGRATION
PRIMARY VISUAL-CORTEX
Color model
Computational Theory and Mathematics
Modeling and Simulation
Visual Perception
Female
Research Article
Adult
Lightness
QH301-705.5
Color vision
media_common.quotation_subject
Models, Neurological
Astrophysics::Cosmology and Extragalactic Astrophysics
Color space
050105 experimental psychology
Contrast Sensitivity
03 medical and health sciences
Cellular and Molecular Neuroscience
LIGHTNESS PERCEPTION
Genetics
Humans
0501 psychology and cognitive sciences
Molecular Biology
Ecology, Evolution, Behavior and Systematics
business.industry
ANCHORING THEORY
Ophthalmology
Artificial intelligence
business
Photic Stimulation
PERCEIVED BRIGHTNESS
030217 neurology & neurosurgery
Neuroscience
Subjects
Details
- ISSN :
- 15537358 and 1553734X
- Volume :
- 3
- Database :
- OpenAIRE
- Journal :
- PLoS Computational Biology
- Accession number :
- edsair.doi.dedup.....69718f2e4e39cfd7a75cd0eea9a8e723