Back to Search Start Over

Dispositional optimism and optimistic bias: Associations with cessation motivation, confidence, and attitudes.

Authors :
Everson, Nicole Senft
Klein, William M. P.
Lee, Scott S.
Selove, Rebecca
Sanderson, Maureen
Blot, William J.
Tyndale, Rachel F.
King, Stephen
Gilliam, Karen
Kundu, Suman
Steinwandel, Mark
Sternlieb, Sarah J.
Andersen, Shaneda Warren
Friedman, Debra L.
Connors, Erin
Fadden, Mary Kay
Freiberg, Matthew S.
Wells, Quinn S.
Canedo, Juan
Young, Robert P.
Source :
Health Psychology; Sep2022, Vol. 41 Issue 9, p621-629, 9p
Publication Year :
2022

Abstract

Objective: To test whether 2 conceptually overlapping constructs, dispositional optimism (generalized positive expectations) and optimistic bias (inaccurately low risk perceptions), may have different implications for smoking treatment engagement. Method: Predominantly Black, low-income Southern Community Cohort study smokers (n = 880) self-reported dispositional optimism and pessimism (Life Orientation Test "Revised subscales: 0 = neutral, 12 = high optimism/pessimism), comparative lung cancer risk (Low/Average/High), and information to calculate objective lung cancer risk (Low/Med/High). Perceived risk was categorized as accurate (perceived = objective), optimistically-biased (perceived< objective), or pessimistically-biased (perceived >objective). One-way ANOVAs tested associations between dispositional optimism/pessimism and perceived risk accuracy. Multivariable logistic regressions tested independent associations of optimism/pessimism and perceived risk accuracy with cessation motivation (Low/High), confidence (Low/High), and precision treatment attitudes (Favorable/Unfavorable), controlling for sociodemographics and nicotine dependence. Results: Mean dispositional optimism/pessimism scores were 8.41 (SD = 2.59) and 5.65 (SD = 3.02), respectively. Perceived lung cancer risk was 38% accurate, 27% optimistically-biased, and 35% pessimistically-biased. Accuracy was unrelated to dispositional optimism (F(2, 641) = 1.23, p = .29), though optimistically-biased (vs. pessimistically-biased) smokers had higher dispositional pessimism (F(2, 628) = 3.17, p = .043). Dispositional optimism was associated with higher confidence (Adjusted odds ratio [AOR] = 1.71, 95% CI [1.42, 2.06], p < .001) and favorable precision treatment attitudes (AOR = 1.66, 95% CI [1.37, 2.01], p < .001). Optimistically-biased (vs. accurate) risk perception was associated with lower motivation (AOR = .64, 95% CI [.42, .98], p = .041) and less favorable precision treatment attitudes (AOR = .59, 95% CI [.38, .94], p = .029). Conclusions: Dispositional optimism and lung cancer risk perception accuracy were unrelated. Dispositional optimism was associated with favorable engagement-related outcomes and optimistically-biased risk perception with unfavorable outcomes, reinforcing the distinctiveness of these constructs and their implications for smoking treatment. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]

Details

Language :
English
ISSN :
02786133
Volume :
41
Issue :
9
Database :
Supplemental Index
Journal :
Health Psychology
Publication Type :
Academic Journal
Accession number :
158761054
Full Text :
https://doi.org/10.1037/hea0001184