This third registration aims to address potential confounds that were identified after carrying out experiments affiliated with the second registration part of this project: 'Processing at-issue content part 2: disentangling recency from clause type'. These self-paced reading experiments - experiment 3 with sentences containing an adverbial clause that starts with 'after', and experiment 4 with subject- and object-modifying relative clause constructions - were designed to investigate which factor had a greater impact on the at-issue status of the individual clauses of above-mentioned sentence types: clause type, or recency. This was tested by measuring how fast a {continuation sentence} was processed that had content that was coreferential with only one of the clauses in the sentence it followed. In experiment 3 this was done by comparing the reading times of {continuation sentences} with content that was coreferential with content in either a sentence-final adverbial clause (1a), or a sentence-early matrix clause (1b): Experiment 3 - adverbial clauses: (1a) They went to a violin concert after they had dinner at a French bistro. It was {a very small cosy restaurant.} (1b) They had dinner at a French bistro after they went to a violin concert. It was {a very small cosy restaurant.} In experiment 4 this was done by comparing the reading times of {continuation sentences} with content that was coreferential with content in a sentence-early matrix clause (2a), a sentence early relative clause (2b), or a sentence-final matrix clause (2c): Experiment 4 - relative clauses (2a) My dad opened a bottle of wine for my uncle, who was having a snack of chips and dip. It was {a dry white wine from Italy.} (2b) My dad, who opened a bottle of wine for my uncle, was having a snack of chips and dip. It was {a dry white wine from Italy.} (2c) My dad, who was having a snack of chips and dip, opened a bottle of wine for my uncle. It was {a dry white wine from Italy.} Neither experiments yielded any significant differences in reading times for the continuations, leaving the question as to which factor - recency or clause type - is more important in assigning at-issue status to clauses, open. While it is clear from the first study (see previous registrations for this project) that recency is an important factor, it is not yet clear whether clause type is as well. The current follow-up study will address this question once more, by carrying out follow-up studies for both experiments which will address potential confounds in experiments 3 and 4. There are three potential reasons we identified for our null results: 1. In experiments 3 & 4, the 'it is/was' pre-region was presented immediately before the {continuation}, at the line below the sentence with which 'it is/was' establishes coreference once the {continuation} is read. This was changed from experiments 1 & 2, where the 'it is/was' pre-region was presented immediately after the coreference sentence, after which the {continuation} was presented on the next line. While there is no immediate reason to believe that the position of this pre-target region with respect to on which line it is presented should have such an effect, it is the only notable difference between experiment 4 and its predecessor (experiment 2). Conditions (2b) and (2c) were both present in experiment 2, and significant differences were found between these two conditions then. Since this result was missing from experiment 4 where the pre-target region was on a different line, we want to make sure that this was not the reason for the absence of an effect. If we replicate experiment 4 and find no effect again, it might mean that the significant differences we found in experiment 2 were spurious differences. If clause type and/or recency are important factors influencing reading times of the {continuation}, experiment 6 (our replication of experiment 4, in line with the positioning of the pre-target region of experiment 2), should show that the continuation is read faster in (3c) than in (3b). Experiment 6 - relative clauses (3a) My dad opened a bottle of wine for my uncle, who was having a snack of chips and dip. It was {a dry white wine from Italy.} (3b) My dad, who opened a bottle of wine for my uncle, was having a snack of chips and dip. It was {a dry white wine from Italy.} (3c) My dad, who was having a snack of chips and dip, opened a bottle of wine for my uncle. It was {a dry white wine from Italy.} 2. In experiment 3, the sentence containing the adverbial clause was presented as a single [chunk] (4) rather than being broken up in three [chunks] (5) as was done in experiment 1(6): (4) [They went to a violin concert after they had dinner at a French bistro.] (5) [They went to a violin concert] [after] [they had dinner at a French bistro.] (6) [After] [they went to a violin concert,] [they had dinner at a French bistro.] This was done to avoid a reading where the subordinating adverbial clause was actually read as coordinating (7): (7) They went to a violin concert, after, they had dinner at a French bistro. While a single [chunk] might have facilitated the subordinating reading, it could be facilitated even more so by presenting the sentence in two individual [chunks]: one containing the matrix clause, and one containing the adverbial clause (8): (8) [They went to a violin concert] [after they had dinner at a French bistro.] By chunking the regions like this, we also avoid having one [chunk] that is much larger than all of the surrounding [chunks], making the experiment more cohesive throughout with no [chunks] that stand out to participants for their size. This leads to below conditions for experiment 5: Experiment 5 - adverbial clauses: (9a) [They went to a violin concert] [after they had dinner at a French bistro.] [It was] {a very small cosy restaurant.} (9b) [They had dinner at a French bistro] [after they went to a violin concert.] [It was] {a very small cosy restaurant.} Both of these changes mean that experiments 5 & 6 will be closer to the original design of experiments 1 & 2, where our findings were in line with our expectations and predictions. That said, if we replicate our null results, this could mean that the significant differences we previously found in experiments 1 & 2, were spurious differences. 3. Lastly, in experiment 3, the null-effect can be explained by the conditions themselves. The conditions were balanced so that in both of them the {continuation} attaches to a clause that has a characteristic that makes it likely to be at-issue, and one that makes it less likely to be at issue. In (1a), the {continuation} attaches to a sentence-late clause, but it is a subordinate clause. In (1b) the {continuation} attaches to a main clause, but it is sentence-early. If this is the (main) reason for our null-effect, we don't expect to find anything different for experiment 5 compared to experiment 3. However, we want to make sure that any other potential confounds are addressed before concluding this.