192 results on '"Visual Neuroscience"'
Search Results
2. Visual Object Segmentation Improvement Using Deep Convolutional Neural Networks
- Author
-
Kanithan, S., Vignesh, N. Arun, Karthick SA, Hamdan, Allam, Editorial Board Member, Al Madhoun, Wesam, Editorial Board Member, Alareeni, Bahaaeddin, Editor-in-Chief, Baalousha, Mohammed, Editorial Board Member, Elgedawy, Islam, Editorial Board Member, Hussainey, Khaled, Editorial Board Member, Eleyan, Derar, Editorial Board Member, Hamdan, Reem, Editorial Board Member, Salem, Mohammed, Editorial Board Member, Jallouli, Rim, Editorial Board Member, Assaidi, Abdelouahid, Editorial Board Member, Nawi, Noorshella Binti Che, Editorial Board Member, AL-Kayid, Kholoud, Editorial Board Member, Wolf, Martin, Editorial Board Member, El Khoury, Rim, Editorial Board Member, Kumar, Ashish, editor, Jain, Rachna, editor, Vairamani, Ajantha Devi, editor, and Nayyar, Anand, editor
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
3. State-of-the-art image and video quality assessment with a metric based on an intrinsically non-linear neural summation model.
- Author
-
Luna, Raúl, Zabaleta, Itziar, and Bertalmío, Marcelo
- Subjects
VISUAL perception ,COMPUTER vision ,IMAGE databases ,IMAGE processing ,VIDEOS - Abstract
The development of automatic methods for image and video quality assessment that correlate well with the perception of human observers is a very challenging open problem in vision science, with numerous practical applications in disciplines such as image processing and computer vision, as well as in the media industry. In the past two decades, the goal of image quality research has been to improve upon classical metrics by developing models that emulate some aspects of the visual system, and while the progress has been considerable, state-of-the-art quality assessment methods still share a number of shortcomings, like their performance dropping considerably when they are tested on a database that is quite different from the one used to train them, or their significant limitations in predicting observer scores for high framerate videos. In this work we propose a novel objective method for image and video quality assessment that is based on the recently introduced Intrinsically Non-linear Receptive Field (INRF) formulation, a neural summation model that has been shown to be better at predicting neural activity and visual perception phenomena than the classical linear receptive field. Here we start by optimizing, on a classic image quality database, the four parameters of a very simple INRF-based metric, and proceed to test this metric on three other databases, showing that its performance equals or surpasses that of the state-of-the-art methods, some of them having millions of parameters. Next, we extend to the temporal domain this INRF image quality metric, and test it on several popular video quality datasets; again, the results of our proposed INRF-based video quality metric are shown to be very competitive. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
4. Artificial psychophysics questions classical hue cancellation experiments.
- Author
-
Vila-Tomás, Jorge, Hernández-Cámara, Pablo, and Malo, Jesús
- Subjects
COLOR space ,SPECTRAL sensitivity ,PSYCHOPHYSICS ,PHOTORECEPTORS ,COLOR codes - Abstract
We showthat classical hue cancellation experiments lead to human-like opponent curves even if the task is done by trivial (identity) artificial networks. Specifically, human-like opponent spectral sensitivities always emerge in artificial networks as long as (i) the retina converts the input radiation into any tristimulus-like representation, and (ii) the post-retinal network solves the standard hue cancellation task, e.g. the network looks for the weights of the cancelling lights so that every monochromatic stimulus plus the weighted cancelling lights match a grey reference in the (arbitrary) color representation used by the network. In fact, the specific cancellation lights (and not the network architecture) are key to obtain human-like curves: results show that the classical choice of the lights is the one that leads to the best (more human-like) result, and any other choices lead to progressively different spectral sensitivities. We show this in two ways: through artificial psychophysics using a range of networks with different architectures and a range of cancellation lights, and through a change-of-basis theoretical analogy of the experiments. This suggests that the opponent curves of the classical experiment are just a by-product of the front-end photoreceptors and of a very specific experimental choice but they do not inform about the downstream color representation. In fact, the architecture of the post-retinal network (signal recombination or internal color space) seems irrelevant for the emergence of the curves in the classical experiment. This result in artificial networks questions the conventional interpretation of the classical result in humans by Jameson and Hurvich. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
5. State-of-the-art image and video quality assessment with a metric based on an intrinsically non-linear neural summation model
- Author
-
Raúl Luna, Itziar Zabaleta, and Marcelo Bertalmío
- Subjects
visual perception ,visual neuroscience ,receptive field ,INRF ,computational modeling ,image quality assessment ,Neurosciences. Biological psychiatry. Neuropsychiatry ,RC321-571 - Abstract
The development of automatic methods for image and video quality assessment that correlate well with the perception of human observers is a very challenging open problem in vision science, with numerous practical applications in disciplines such as image processing and computer vision, as well as in the media industry. In the past two decades, the goal of image quality research has been to improve upon classical metrics by developing models that emulate some aspects of the visual system, and while the progress has been considerable, state-of-the-art quality assessment methods still share a number of shortcomings, like their performance dropping considerably when they are tested on a database that is quite different from the one used to train them, or their significant limitations in predicting observer scores for high framerate videos. In this work we propose a novel objective method for image and video quality assessment that is based on the recently introduced Intrinsically Non-linear Receptive Field (INRF) formulation, a neural summation model that has been shown to be better at predicting neural activity and visual perception phenomena than the classical linear receptive field. Here we start by optimizing, on a classic image quality database, the four parameters of a very simple INRF-based metric, and proceed to test this metric on three other databases, showing that its performance equals or surpasses that of the state-of-the-art methods, some of them having millions of parameters. Next, we extend to the temporal domain this INRF image quality metric, and test it on several popular video quality datasets; again, the results of our proposed INRF-based video quality metric are shown to be very competitive.
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
6. Artificial psychophysics questions classical hue cancellation experiments
- Author
-
Jorge Vila-Tomás, Pablo Hernández-Cámara, and Jesús Malo
- Subjects
artificial psychophysics ,visual neuroscience ,hue cancellation experiments ,opponent color coding ,spectral sensitivity of artificial networks ,Neurosciences. Biological psychiatry. Neuropsychiatry ,RC321-571 - Abstract
We show that classical hue cancellation experiments lead to human-like opponent curves even if the task is done by trivial (identity) artificial networks. Specifically, human-like opponent spectral sensitivities always emerge in artificial networks as long as (i) the retina converts the input radiation into any tristimulus-like representation, and (ii) the post-retinal network solves the standard hue cancellation task, e.g. the network looks for the weights of the cancelling lights so that every monochromatic stimulus plus the weighted cancelling lights match a grey reference in the (arbitrary) color representation used by the network. In fact, the specific cancellation lights (and not the network architecture) are key to obtain human-like curves: results show that the classical choice of the lights is the one that leads to the best (more human-like) result, and any other choices lead to progressively different spectral sensitivities. We show this in two ways: through artificial psychophysics using a range of networks with different architectures and a range of cancellation lights, and through a change-of-basis theoretical analogy of the experiments. This suggests that the opponent curves of the classical experiment are just a by-product of the front-end photoreceptors and of a very specific experimental choice but they do not inform about the downstream color representation. In fact, the architecture of the post-retinal network (signal recombination or internal color space) seems irrelevant for the emergence of the curves in the classical experiment. This result in artificial networks questions the conventional interpretation of the classical result in humans by Jameson and Hurvich.
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
7. The policy of testing hypotheses in Chilean science. The role of a hypothesis-driven research funding programme in the installation of a hypothesis-driven experimental system in visual neuroscience.
- Author
-
Garrido Wainer, Juan Manuel, Hirmas-Montecinos, Natalia, and Trujillo Osorio, Nicolás
- Subjects
- *
PHILOSOPHY of science , *RESEARCH funding , *NEUROSCIENCES , *RESOURCE allocation , *HYPOTHESIS - Abstract
O'Malley et al. (2009) and Haufe (2013) suggest that the philosophical idea of science as hypothesis testing generates a pernicious bias towards hypothesis-driven research and against exploratory research in the review process of research proposals and the allocation of resources. This paper addresses a conceptual objection to the argument by O'Malley et al. (2009) and Haufe (2013). We argue that the funding agencies' concepts of good science do not belong to epistemological or philosophical contexts but to political and institutional contexts. This means that correcting (potential) biases in research funding does not entail correcting funding agencies' (supposed) philosophies of science. To illustrate this point, we provide an in-depth historical case study: the granting of funds to neuroscientist Pedro Maldonado by the Chilean funding programme FONDECYT. This is a relevant comparison as FONDECYT's guidelines explicitly promote hypothesis-driven research and endorse a view of "good science" as hypothesis testing. However, we will see that the overall influence of the philosophical idea of science as hypothesis testing over this funding programme, the research project, and the actual practice of hypothesis testing is somewhat limited. The concept of science as hypothesis testing seems to play a crucial institutional or political (not philosophical) role in allowing the conceptual articulation of social expectations and researchers' expectations. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
8. Understanding the development and use of tools in neuroscience: the case of the tungsten micro-electrode.
- Author
-
Garrido Wainer, Juan Manuel
- Abstract
The philosophical interest in experimental practice in neuroscience has brought renewed attention to the study of the development and use of techniques and tools for data production. John Bickle has argued that the construction and progression of theories in neuroscience are entirely dependent on the development and ingenious use of research tools. In Bickle's account, theory plays a tertiary role, as it depends on what the tools allow researchers to manipulate, and the tools, in turn, are developed not in order to test theories but as solutions to engineering problems. However, Bickle's account is not entirely precise in explaining what informs researchers' decision-making in their atheoretical laboratory tinkering. Identifying the sources that guide researchers in tool development and use is crucial if one wishes to contribute to the philosophical or meta-scientific understanding of experimental practice in neuroscience. In the following paper, I claim that decision-making in tools' development and use in neuroscience is doubly guided. Pre-existing theory and concepts determine information's relevance, whereas tools' functioning in controlled situations determines information's reliability. Accordingly, experimenters' decision-making is situated both in the context of analysing, modelling or interpreting information and in the context of producing information. I study the case of the tungsten microelectrode developed by David Hubel during the 1950s. First, I show that pre-existing theory and concepts (in particular, the "neuron doctrine" and the concepts of "receptive field" and "cortical column") determine in advance what information would be relevant to obtain from the microelectrode. Second, I show that Hubel's tinkering follows the guidelines derived from the very structure of what we recognise as reliable experimentally produced information. Finally, I suggest that data-production processes allow experimenters to assess what to expect from an experimental system in terms of concept- and theory-generation and confirmation, thereby endorsing Bickle's tenet on the tertiary role of theory in neuroscience. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
9. Premio Nacional de Investigación 2023 + Premio Galego Do Ano 2023 para Felipe Criado-Boado
- Author
-
European Commission, Vuelta-Santín, Rubén [ruben.vuelta-santin@incipit.csic.es], Criado-Boado, Felipe, Vuelta-Santín, Rubén, European Commission, Vuelta-Santín, Rubén [ruben.vuelta-santin@incipit.csic.es], Criado-Boado, Felipe, and Vuelta-Santín, Rubén
- Abstract
Los trabajos experimentales en el Material Minds Lab – proyecto ERC SyG XSCAPE, del INCIPIT-CSIC, abren el vídeo oficial de la entrega de los Premios Nacionales de Investigación 2023. Felipe Criado-Boado, corresponding Principal Investigator del Proyecto ERC-2020-SyG 951631 XSCAPE, ha sido galardonado con el Premio Nacional de Investigación 2023. Ha recibido el premio por sus contribuciones pioneras e innovadoras a la arqueología, que han contribuido a comprender cómo las sociedades humanas han interactuado y modificado el paisaje en el transcurso de la historia. Sus estudios recientes examinan cómo el mundo que construimos modifica nuestra mente y nuestra forma de procesar la información, en el proyecto europeo Material Minds. También ha conseguido el Premio Galego do Ano 2023, concedido por El Correo Gallego, del grupo Prensa Ibérica. El proyecto XSCAPE utiliza un conjunto de materiales de diferentes culturas y entornos, para explorar los cambios cognitivos impulsados por la materialidad.
- Published
- 2024
10. Como facer experimentos de Eye-Tracking? Proxecto XSCAPE Material Minds. Versión en galego
- Author
-
Vuelta-Santín, Rubén [ruben.vuelta-santin@incipit.csic.es], Fernández-Pestonit, Alba, Zaghi, Regina, Vuelta-Santín, Rubén, Vuelta-Santín, Rubén [ruben.vuelta-santin@incipit.csic.es], Fernández-Pestonit, Alba, Zaghi, Regina, and Vuelta-Santín, Rubén
- Abstract
O proxecto XSCAPE, financiado con fondos europeos, reúne a un equipo de arqueólogos, científicos da visión e filósofos cognitivos para explorar se os mundos que construimos alteran a nosa propia mente e a forma en que procesamos a información. O equipo financiado polo Consello Europeo de Investigación estuda se os nosos nosos edificios, estradas e artefactos cambian os patróns de pensamento e atención. O equipo do proxecto, que ten previsto realizar unha serie de máis de corenta estudos de casos globais, utiliza un conxunto de materiais —de diferentes culturas e entornos antigos e novos— para explorar os cambios cognitivos impulsados pola materialidade. XSCAPE utiliza unha nova metodoloxía sinérxica que combina múltiples estudos de casos do mundo real coa neurociencia visual máis recente e simulacións simples basadas en axentes.
- Published
- 2024
11. How to do Eye-Tracking experiments? XSCAPE Material Minds Project. English versión
- Author
-
Vuelta-Santín, Rubén [ruben.vuelta-santin@incipit.csic.es], Fernández-Pestonit, Alba, Zaghi, Regina, Vuelta-Santín, Rubén, Vuelta-Santín, Rubén [ruben.vuelta-santin@incipit.csic.es], Fernández-Pestonit, Alba, Zaghi, Regina, and Vuelta-Santín, Rubén
- Abstract
The EU-funded XSCAPE project will bring together a team from the areas of archaeology, vision science and cognitive philosophy to explore whether the worlds we build alter our own minds and how we process information. The ERC-funded team will investigate whether our buildings, roads and artefacts change patterns of thought and attention. Slated to conduct a series of more than 40 global case studies, the project will use a set of materials – from different cultures and old and new settings – to explore materiality-driven cognitive change. XSCAPE will use a new synergistic methodology that combines multiple real-world case studies with the latest visual neuroscience, and simple agent-based simulations.
- Published
- 2024
12. ¿Cómo hacer experimentos de Eye-Tracking? Proyecto XSCAPE Material Minds. Versión en castellano
- Author
-
Vuelta-Santín, Rubén [ruben.vuelta-santin@incipit.csic.es], Fernández-Pestonit, Alba, Zaghi, Regina, Vuelta-Santín, Rubén, Vuelta-Santín, Rubén [ruben.vuelta-santin@incipit.csic.es], Fernández-Pestonit, Alba, Zaghi, Regina, and Vuelta-Santín, Rubén
- Abstract
El proyecto XSCAPE, financiado con fondos europeos, reúne a un equipo de arqueólogos, científicos de la visión y filósofos cognitivos para explorar si los mundos que construimos alteran nuestra propia mente y la forma en que procesamos la información. El equipo financiado por el Consejo Europeo de Investigación estudia si nuestros edificios, carreteras y artefactos cambian los patrones de pensamiento y atención. El equipo del proyecto, que tiene previsto realizar una serie de más de cuarenta estudios de casos globales, utiliza un conjunto de materiales —de diferentes culturas y entornos antiguos y nuevos— para explorar los cambios cognitivos impulsados por la materialidad. XSCAPE utiliza una nueva metodología sinérgica que combina múltiples estudios de casos del mundo real con la neurociencia visual más reciente y simulaciones simples basadas en agentes.
- Published
- 2024
13. ¿Cómo hacer experiencias de Eye-Tracking? Proyecto XSCAPE Material Minds. Versión español americano
- Author
-
Vuelta-Santín, Rubén [ruben.vuelta-santin@incipit.csic.es], Zaghi, Regina, Vuelta-Santín, Rubén, Vuelta-Santín, Rubén [ruben.vuelta-santin@incipit.csic.es], Zaghi, Regina, and Vuelta-Santín, Rubén
- Abstract
El proyecto XSCAPE, financiado con fondos europeos, reúne a un equipo de arqueólogos, científicos de la visión y filósofos cognitivos para explorar si los mundos que construimos alteran nuestra propia mente y la forma en que procesamos la información. El equipo financiado por el Consejo Europeo de Investigación estudia si nuestros edificios, carreteras y artefactos cambian los patrones de pensamiento y atención. El equipo del proyecto, que tiene previsto realizar una serie de más de cuarenta estudios de casos globales, utiliza un conjunto de materiales —de diferentes culturas y entornos antiguos y nuevos— para explorar los cambios cognitivos impulsados por la materialidad. XSCAPE utiliza una nueva metodología sinérgica que combina múltiples estudios de casos del mundo real con la neurociencia visual más reciente y simulaciones simples basadas en agentes.
- Published
- 2024
14. Editorial: Rising stars in visual neuroscience: 2022.
- Author
-
Meier, Kimberly, Chang, Dorita H. F., Reynaud, Alexandre, and Kelly, Krista R.
- Subjects
NEUROSCIENCES ,AFFERENT pathways ,RETINAL ganglion cells ,VISUAL fields ,VISION - Abstract
This document is an editorial published in Frontiers in Neuroscience in 2024. It introduces the "Rising Stars in Visual Neuroscience 2022" Research Topic, which showcases the work of emerging leaders in the field. The research presented in this topic covers a wide range of areas in visual neuroscience, including retinal degeneration, decision strategies and performance, intermodal integration, and clinical applications. The editorial highlights specific studies that investigate retinal remodeling effects, decision-making in uncertain environments, crossmodal transfer of object information, compensatory mechanisms in visual processing, and a new digital tool for assessing hand-eye coordination in individuals with ultra low vision. The authors emphasize the importance of this research in advancing our understanding of visual neuroscience and identifying promising researchers in the field. [Extracted from the article]
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
15. Perceptual category learning and visual processing: An exercise in computational cognitive neuroscience
- Author
-
Cantwell, George, Riesenhuber, Maximilian, Roeder, Jessica L, and Ashby, F Gregory
- Subjects
Information and Computing Sciences ,Machine Learning ,Neurosciences ,Behavioral and Social Science ,Eye Disease and Disorders of Vision ,Basic Behavioral and Social Science ,Mental health ,Neurological ,Basal Ganglia ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,Computer Simulation ,Humans ,Learning ,Photic Stimulation ,Random Allocation ,Visual Cortex ,Visual Perception ,Basal ganglia ,COVIS ,Categorization ,Computational cognitive neuroscience ,HMAX ,Visual neuroscience ,Artificial Intelligence & Image Processing ,Artificial intelligence ,Machine learning ,Statistics - Abstract
The field of computational cognitive neuroscience (CCN) builds and tests neurobiologically detailed computational models that account for both behavioral and neuroscience data. This article leverages a key advantage of CCN-namely, that it should be possible to interface different CCN models in a plug-and-play fashion-to produce a new and biologically detailed model of perceptual category learning. The new model was created from two existing CCN models: the HMAX model of visual object processing and the COVIS model of category learning. Using bitmap images as inputs and by adjusting only a couple of learning-rate parameters, the new HMAX/COVIS model provides impressively good fits to human category-learning data from two qualitatively different experiments that used different types of category structures and different types of visual stimuli. Overall, the model provides a comprehensive neural and behavioral account of basal ganglia-mediated learning.
- Published
- 2017
16. Effects of top-down influence suppression on behavioral and V1 neuronal contrast sensitivity functions in cats
- Author
-
Jian Ding, Zheng Ye, Fei Xu, Xiangmei Hu, Hao Yu, Shen Zhang, Yanni Tu, Qiuyu Zhang, Qingyan Sun, Tianmiao Hua, and Zhong-Lin Lu
- Subjects
Biological science ,Behavioral neuroscience ,Sensory neuroscience ,Visual neuroscience ,Science - Abstract
Summary: To explore the relative contributions of higher-order and primary visual cortex (V1) to visual perception, we compared cats' behavioral and V1 neuronal contrast sensitivity functions (CSF) and threshold versus external noise contrast (TvC) functions before and after top-down influence of area 7 (A7) was modulated with transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). We found that suppressing top-down influence of A7 with cathode-tDCS, but not sham-tDCS, reduced behavioral and neuronal contrast sensitivity in the same range of spatial frequencies and increased behavioral and neuronal contrast thresholds in the same range of external noise levels. The neuronal CSF and TvC functions were highly correlated with their behavioral counterparts both before and after the top-down suppression. Analysis of TvC functions using the Perceptual Template Model (PTM) indicated that top-down influence of A7 increased both behavioral and V1 neuronal contrast sensitivity by reducing internal additive noise and the impact of external noise.
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
17. Arche-writing and data-production in theory-oriented scientific practice: the case of free-viewing as experimental system to test the temporal correlation hypothesis.
- Author
-
Garrido Wainer, Juan Manuel, Fardella, Carla, and Espinosa Cristia, Juan Felipe
- Abstract
Data production in experimental sciences depends on localised experimental systems, but the epistemic properties of data transcend the contingencies of the processes that produce them. Philosophers often believe that experimental systems instantiate but do not produce the epistemic properties of data. In this paper, we argue that experimental systems' local functioning entails intrinsic capacities to produce the epistemic properties of data. We develop this idea by applying Derrida's model of arche-writing to study a case of theory-oriented experimental practice. Derrida's model relativises or dissolves the conceptual distinction between the moment of data production and a subsequent moment of data dissemination. It thus has consequences for understanding both data production (despite being intrinsically local, data production a priori generates transferrable and modellable information) and data dissemination (when modelling information, researchers needs to refer this information to the context of its production). We study a case of data production in a non-exploratory experimental system designed to test a pre-existing hypothesis in visual neuroscience. A case of theory-oriented experimental practice should allow us to identify the autonomous functioning of experimental systems in data production more clearly, insofar as it allows us to study the limits of pre-existing theory in the activities of these systems. We suggest that pre-existing concepts, hypotheses and theories condition the relevance but not the production of experimental data. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
18. Conceptual Change in Visual Neuroscience: The Receptive Field Concept.
- Author
-
Venturelli, A. Nicolás
- Subjects
- *
SENSORY neurons , *NEUROSCIENCES , *PHILOSOPHY , *BIOLOGY , *VISUAL cortex - Abstract
I focus on the concept of the receptive field of a sensory neuron, taking it as a prominent case to address conceptual change in the history of neuroscience. I argue for an interpretation of its role and evolution based on the idea, previously rehearsed in the philosophy of biology, of a problem agenda. I claim that the concept's main contribution to visual neuroscience was that of establishing a problem agenda for the early experimental research on visual processing, thus structuring the field's experimental practices and integration between converging scientific fields. I pay particular attention to the research programme led by Hubel and Wiesel, revolving their famous experiments on the cat's visual cortex during the 60s, when receptive field exploration blossomed. I additionally claim that the concept's more recent evolution, witnessing a loss in its definiteness and centrality, can be accommodated by my account. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
19. LED Zappelin’: An open source LED controller for arbitrary spectrum visual stimulation and optogenetics during 2-photon imaging
- Author
-
M.J.Y. Zimmermann, A. Maia Chagas, P. Bartel, S. Pop, L.L. Prieto-Godino, and T. Baden
- Subjects
2 Photon ,Visual neuroscience ,Optogenetics ,Light stimulation ,LED ,Open hardware ,Science (General) ,Q1-390 - Abstract
Two-photon (2P) microscopy is a cornerstone technique in neuroscience research. However, combining 2P imaging with spectrally arbitrary light stimulation can be challenging due to crosstalk between stimulation light and fluorescence detection. To overcome this limitation, we present a simple and low-cost electronic solution based on an ESP32 microcontroller and a TLC5947 LED driver to rapidly time-interleave stimulation and detection epochs during scans. Implemented for less than $100, our design can independently drive up to 24 arbitrary spectrum LEDs to meet user requirements. We demonstrate the utility of our stimulator for colour vision experiments on the in vivo tetrachromatic zebrafish retina and for optogenetic circuit mapping in Drosophila.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
20. Translational Vision Science & Technology
- Subjects
vision science ,visual disorders ,imaging technology ,regenerative medicine ,visual neuroscience ,physiological optics ,Medicine ,Ophthalmology ,RE1-994 - Published
- 2020
21. The foundations of visual neuroscience in Australia.
- Author
-
Vaney, David I.
- Abstract
The passing of Jack Pettigrew (University of Queensland) and Bogdan Dreher (University of Sydney) within 2 weeks of each other in May 2019 provided the catalyst for documenting the roles that they and their colleagues from the Peter Bishop School played in establishing vision research as a major strength in Australian neuroscience in the second half of the 20th century. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
22. Variational Models for Color Image Correction Inspired by Visual Perception and Neuroscience.
- Author
-
Batard, Thomas, Hertrich, Johannes, and Steidl, Gabriele
- Abstract
Reproducing the perception of a real-world scene on a display device is a very challenging task which requires the understanding of the camera processing pipeline, the display process, and the way the human visual system processes the light it captures. Mathematical models based on psychophysical and physiological laws on color vision, named Retinex, provide efficient tools to handle degradations produced during the camera processing pipeline like the reduction of the contrast. In particular, Batard and Bertalmío (in J Math Imaging Vis 60(6):849–881, 2018) described some psychophysical laws on brightness perception as covariant derivatives, included them into a variational model, and observed that the quality of the color image correction is correlated with the accuracy of the vision model it includes. Based on this observation, we postulate that this model can be improved by including more accurate data on vision with a special attention on visual neuroscience here. Then, inspired by the presence of neurons responding to different visual attributes in the area V1 of the visual cortex as orientation, color or movement, to name a few, and horizontal connections modeling the interactions between those neurons, we construct two variational models to process both local (edges, textures) and global (contrast) features. This is an improvement with respect to the model of Batard and Bertalmío as the latter cannot process local and global features independently and simultaneously. Finally, we conduct experiments on color images which corroborate the improvement provided by the new models. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
23. Expanding Theoretical Complexity
- Author
-
Hackett, Paul M. W. and Hackett, Paul M.W.
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
24. State-of-the-art image and video quality assessment with a metric based on an intrinsically non-linear neural summation model
- Author
-
European Commission, Ministerio de Ciencia e Innovación (España), #NODATA#, Luna, Raúl, Zabaleta, Itziar, Bertalmío, Marcelo, European Commission, Ministerio de Ciencia e Innovación (España), #NODATA#, Luna, Raúl, Zabaleta, Itziar, and Bertalmío, Marcelo
- Abstract
The development of automatic methods for image and video quality assessment that correlate well with the perception of human observers is a very challenging open problem in vision science, with numerous practical applications in disciplines such as image processing and computer vision, as well as in the media industry. In the past two decades, the goal of image quality research has been to improve upon classical metrics by developing models that emulate some aspects of the visual system, and while the progress has been considerable, state-of-the-art quality assessment methods still share a number of shortcomings, like their performance dropping considerably when they are tested on a database that is quite different from the one used to train them, or their significant limitations in predicting observer scores for high framerate videos. In this work we propose a novel objective method for image and video quality assessment that is based on the recently introduced Intrinsically Non-linear Receptive Field (INRF) formulation, a neural summation model that has been shown to be better at predicting neural activity and visual perception phenomena than the classical linear receptive field. Here we start by optimizing, on a classic image quality database, the four parameters of a very simple INRF-based metric, and proceed to test this metric on three other databases, showing that its performance equals or surpasses that of the state-of-the-art methods, some of them having millions of parameters. Next, we extend to the temporal domain this INRF image quality metric, and test it on several popular video quality datasets; again, the results of our proposed INRF-based video quality metric are shown to be very competitive.
- Published
- 2023
25. Model matters: Differential outcomes in traumatic optic neuropathy pathophysiology between blunt and blast-wave mediated head injuries.
- Author
-
Hetzer, S.M., O'Connell, C., Lallo, V., Robson, M., and Evanson, N.K.
- Subjects
- *
BLAST injuries , *BLUNT trauma , *HEAD injuries , *AMYLOID beta-protein precursor , *LATERAL geniculate body , *OPTIC nerve injuries , *RETINAL ganglion cells - Abstract
Over 3 million people in the United States live with long-term disability because of a traumatic brain injury (TBI). The purpose of this study was to characterize and compare two different animal models of TBI (blunt head trauma and blast TBI) to determine common and divergent characteristics of these models. With recent literature reviews noting the prevalence of visual system injury in animal models of TBI, coupled with clinical estimates of 50–75% of all TBI cases, we decided to assess commonalities, if they existed, through visual system injury. A unilateral (left directed) blast and repeat blast model injury with coup-contra-coup injury patterns were compared to a midline blunt injury. Injuries were induced in adult male mice to observe and quantify visual deficits. Retinal ganglion cell loss and axonal degeneration in the optic tract, superior colliculus, and lateral geniculate nuclei were examined to trace injury outcomes throughout major vision-associated areas. Optokinetic response, immunohistochemistry, and western blots were analyzed. Where a single blunt injury produces significant visual deficits a single blast injury appears to have less severe visual consequences. Visual deficits after repeat blasts are similar to a single blast. Single blast injury induces contralateral damage to the right optic chiasm and tract whereas bilateral injury follows a single blunt TBI. Repeat blast injuries are required to see degeneration patterns in downstream regions similar to the damage seen in a single blunt injury. This finding is further supported by amyloid precursor protein (APP) staining in injured cohorts. Blunt injured groups present with staining 1.2 mm ahead of the optic nerve, indicating axonal breakage closer to the optic chiasm. In blast groups, APP was identifiable in a bilateral pattern only in the geniculate nucleus. Evidence for unilateral neuronal degeneration in brain tissue with bilateral axonal ruptures are pivotal discoveries in this model differentiation. Analysis of the two injury models suggests that there is a significant difference in the histological outcomes dependent on injury type, though visual system injury is likely present in more cases than are currently diagnosed clinically. • Brain injury modalities lead to overlapping and unique pathological outcomes. • Optic neuropathy may be a more common feature of TBI than previously thought. • Unilateral, coup-contra-coup blast TBI leads to contralateral optic tract injury. • Yet, a midline blunt TBI leads to bilateral optic nerve injury. • Identification of conserved pathology of TBI may lead to better treatments. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
26. Optimising the Structure-Function Relationship at the Locus of Deficit in Retinal Disease
- Author
-
Jack Phu, Michael Kalloniatis, Henrietta Wang, and Sieu K. Khuu
- Subjects
glaucoma ,retinitis pigmentosa ,statokinetic dissociation ,perimetry ,visual neuroscience ,Neurosciences. Biological psychiatry. Neuropsychiatry ,RC321-571 - Abstract
Technologies such as optical coherence tomography have facilitated the visualization of anatomical tissues such as that of the retina. The availability of in vivo retinal anatomical data has led to the hypothesis that it may be able to accurately predict visual function from anatomical information. However, accurate determination of the structure-function relationship has remained elusive in part due to contributions of non-retinal sources of variability, thus imposing potential limitations in the fidelity of the relationship. Furthermore, differences in manifestation of functional loss due to different retinal loci of change (inner retina or outer retinal elements) have also been the subject of debate. Here, we assessed the application of a novel, more objective psychophysical paradigm to better characterize the relationship between functional and structural characteristics in the eye. Using ocular diseases with known loci of anatomical change (glaucoma, inner retinal loss; and retinitis pigmentosa, outer retinal loss), we compared conventional more subjective psychophysical techniques that may be contaminated by the presence of non-retinal sources of variability with our more objective approach. We show that stronger correlations between underlying retinal structure and visual function can be achieved across a breadth of anatomical change by using a more objective psychophysical paradigm. This was independent of the locus of structural loss (at the ganglion cells for glaucoma or photoreceptors for retinitis pigmentosa), highlighting the role of downstream retinal elements to serve as anatomical limiting factors for studying the structure-function relationship. By reducing the contribution of non-retinal sources of variability in psychophysical measurements, we herein provide a structure-function model with higher fidelity. This reinforces the need to carefully consider the psychophysical protocol when examining the structure-function relationship in sensory systems.
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
27. The role of alpha oscillations in visual information processing
- Author
-
Nelli, Stephanie
- Subjects
Quantitative psychology ,Neurosciences ,Computational Neuroscience ,Human Cognition ,Neural Oscillations ,Visual Neuroscience - Abstract
ABSTRACT OF THE DISSERTATIONThe role of alpha oscillations in visual information processingbyStephanie NelliDoctor of Philosophy in Neurosciences with a Specialization in Computational NeuroscienceUniversity of California San Diego, 2019Professor John Serences, ChairNeural oscillations are one the most prominent features of electrical brain recordings, involving the synchronized activity of large populations of neurons, and have been linked to a variety of important functions over the past century. Existing theories propose these oscillations allow the brain to dynamically switch between functional neural circuits, allowing computational flexibility on time scales too fast for the slow structural changes that characterize long-term cortical plasticity. In particular, alpha oscillations (~8-13 Hz) are often visible in raw scalp electroencephalography (EEG) recordings over visual and parietal cortex and seem to regulate the waxing and waning of visual attention and perception across time. However, many previous results focus specifically on the phase, power or frequency of the alpha oscillation, neglecting the common dynamical systems that simultaneously generate all these metrics and impact visual information processing. In this dissertation, I begin by reviewing relevant literature about alpha oscillations, and then aim to link together disparate measures of the alpha oscillations in both behavior and the brain to address how dynamical alpha state regulates visual information processing. First, in C1 I find that both phase locked and purely power based analyses of imprecise attentional selection display alpha rhythms, suggesting a common impact on behavior. Indeed, in C2 I show that alpha frequency, which governs phase and amplitude are linked mathematically in simple models of harmonic oscillators, which I confirm in neural recordings. Thus, the impact of alpha oscillations on perception depend on circuit interactions with top-down driving oscillators, and in C3 I find that optimal oscillatory drive for visual perception depends intimately on each subject’s particular dynamical system and resultant peak alpha frequency. Together, this thesis challenges core assumptions underlying current theories of the role that alpha oscillations play in regulating visual information processing using both mathematical models and empirical data. I then propose a more unified theoretical framework in which alpha frequency, phase and amplitude should not be viewed as independent metrics to be correlated with behavior, but instead as the result of a common dynamical system that impacts visual perception.
- Published
- 2019
28. Optimising the Structure-Function Relationship at the Locus of Deficit in Retinal Disease.
- Author
-
Phu, Jack, Kalloniatis, Michael, Wang, Henrietta, and Khuu, Sieu K.
- Subjects
RETINAL diseases ,OPTICAL coherence tomography ,GLAUCOMA ,PERIMETRY ,SENSE organs - Abstract
Technologies such as optical coherence tomography have facilitated the visualization of anatomical tissues such as that of the retina. The availability of in vivo retinal anatomical data has led to the hypothesis that it may be able to accurately predict visual function from anatomical information. However, accurate determination of the structure-function relationship has remained elusive in part due to contributions of non-retinal sources of variability, thus imposing potential limitations in the fidelity of the relationship. Furthermore, differences in manifestation of functional loss due to different retinal loci of change (inner retina or outer retinal elements) have also been the subject of debate. Here, we assessed the application of a novel, more objective psychophysical paradigm to better characterize the relationship between functional and structural characteristics in the eye. Using ocular diseases with known loci of anatomical change (glaucoma, inner retinal loss; and retinitis pigmentosa, outer retinal loss), we compared conventional more subjective psychophysical techniques that may be contaminated by the presence of non-retinal sources of variability with our more objective approach. We show that stronger correlations between underlying retinal structure and visual function can be achieved across a breadth of anatomical change by using a more objective psychophysical paradigm. This was independent of the locus of structural loss (at the ganglion cells for glaucoma or photoreceptors for retinitis pigmentosa), highlighting the role of downstream retinal elements to serve as anatomical limiting factors for studying the structure-function relationship. By reducing the contribution of non-retinal sources of variability in psychophysical measurements, we herein provide a structure-function model with higher fidelity. This reinforces the need to carefully consider the psychophysical protocol when examining the structure-function relationship in sensory systems. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
29. Location representations of objects in cluttered scenes in the human brain
- Author
-
Graumann, Monika
- Subjects
cognitive neuroscience ,100 Philosophie und Psychologie::150 Psychologie::152 Sinneswahrnehmung, Bewegung, Emotionen, Triebe ,fMRI ,EEG ,multivariate pattern analysis ,visual neuroscience ,object recognition ,100 Philosophie und Psychologie::150 Psychologie::153 Kognitive Prozesse, Intelligenz - Abstract
When we perceive a visual scene, we usually see an arrangement of multiple cluttered and partly overlapping objects, like a park with trees and people in it. Spatial attention helps us to prioritize relevant portions of such scenes to efficiently interact with our environments. In previous experiments on object recognition, objects were often presented in isolation, and these studies found that the location of objects is encoded early in time (before ∼150 ms) and in early visual cortex or in the dorsal stream. However, in real life objects rarely appear in isolation but are instead embedded in cluttered scenes. Encoding the location of an object in clutter might require fundamentally different neural computations. Therefore this dissertation addressed the question of how location representations of objects on cluttered backgrounds are encoded in the human brain. To answer this question, we investigated where in cortical space and when in neural processing time location representations emerge when objects are presented on cluttered backgrounds and which role spatial attention plays for the encoding of object location. We addressed these questions in two studies, both including fMRI and EEG experiments. The results of the first study showed that location representations of objects on cluttered backgrounds emerge along the ventral visual stream, peaking in region LOC with a temporal delay that was linked to recurrent processing. The second study showed that spatial attention modulated those location representations in mid- and high-level regions along the ventral stream and late in time (after ∼150 ms), independently of whether backgrounds were cluttered or not. These findings show that location representations emerge during late stages of processing both in cortical space and in neural processing time when objects are presented on cluttered backgrounds and that they are enhanced by spatial attention. Our results provide a new perspective on visual information processing in the ventral visual stream and on the temporal dynamics of location processing. Finally, we discuss how shared neural substrates of location and category representations in the brain might improve object recognition for real-world vision.
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
30. Bayesian analysis of retinotopic maps
- Author
-
Noah C Benson and Jonathan Winawer
- Subjects
vision ,vision science ,visual neuroscience ,retinotopy ,retinotopic map ,occipital cortex ,Medicine ,Science ,Biology (General) ,QH301-705.5 - Abstract
Human visual cortex is organized into multiple retinotopic maps. Characterizing the arrangement of these maps on the cortical surface is essential to many visual neuroscience studies. Typically, maps are obtained by voxel-wise analysis of fMRI data. This method, while useful, maps only a portion of the visual field and is limited by measurement noise and subjective assessment of boundaries. We developed a novel Bayesian mapping approach which combines observation–a subject’s retinotopic measurements from small amounts of fMRI time–with a prior–a learned retinotopic atlas. This process automatically draws areal boundaries, corrects discontinuities in the measured maps, and predicts validation data more accurately than an atlas alone or independent datasets alone. This new method can be used to improve the accuracy of retinotopic mapping, to analyze large fMRI datasets automatically, and to quantify differences in map properties as a function of health, development and natural variation between individuals.
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
31. Asymmetries of the visual system and their influence on visual performance and oculomotor dynamics.
- Author
-
Jóhannesson, Ómar I., Tagu, Jérôme, and Kristjánsson, Árni
- Subjects
- *
VISUAL fields , *RETINAL ganglion cells , *PHOTORECEPTORS , *OCULOMOTOR nerve , *SACCADIC eye movements - Abstract
Our representation of the visual field is not homogenous. There are differences in resolution not only between the fovea and regions eccentric to it, but also between the nasal and temporal hemiretinae, that can be traced to asymmetric distributions of photoreceptors and ganglion cells. We review evidence for differences in visual and attentional processing and oculomotor behaviour that can be traced to asymmetries of the visual system, mainly emphasising nasal‐temporal asymmetries. Asymmetries in the visual system manifest in various measures, in basic psychophysical tests of visual performance, attentional processing, choice behaviour, saccadic peak velocity, and latencies. Nasal‐temporal asymmetries on saccadic latency seem primarily to occur for express saccades. Neural asymmetries between the upper and lower hemifields are strong and cause corresponding differences in performance between the hemifields. There are interesting individual differences in asymmetric processing which seem to be related to the strength of eye dominance. These neurophysiological asymmetries and the corresponding asymmetries in visual performance and oculomotor behaviour can strongly influence experimental results in vision and must be considered during experimental design and the interpretation of results. We review evidence for asymmetries in vision, attention, and oculomotor behaviour. We mainly focus on processing differences that can be traced to asymmetries of the visual system, in particular nasal‐temporal asymmetries. Biological asymmetries are found in the retina, the optic nerves and some brain areas involved in visual processing, and these asymmetries manifest in various measures, basic psychophysical tests of attentional processing, choice behaviour, and saccadic parameters. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
32. Convolutional neural network-based encoding and decoding of visual object recognition in space and time.
- Author
-
Seeliger, K., Fritsche, M., Güçlü, U., Schoenmakers, S., Schoffelen, J.-M., Bosch, S.E., and van Gerven, M.A.J.
- Subjects
- *
SIGNAL convolution , *NEURAL circuitry , *DECODING algorithms , *OBJECT recognition (Computer vision) , *SPACETIME - Abstract
Abstract Representations learned by deep convolutional neural networks (CNNs) for object recognition are a widely investigated model of the processing hierarchy in the human visual system. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging, CNN representations of visual stimuli have previously been shown to correspond to processing stages in the ventral and dorsal streams of the visual system. Whether this correspondence between models and brain signals also holds for activity acquired at high temporal resolution has been explored less exhaustively. Here, we addressed this question by combining CNN-based encoding models with magnetoencephalography (MEG). Human participants passively viewed 1,000 images of objects while MEG signals were acquired. We modelled their high temporal resolution source-reconstructed cortical activity with CNNs, and observed a feed-forward sweep across the visual hierarchy between 75 and 200 ms after stimulus onset. This spatiotemporal cascade was captured by the network layer representations, where the increasingly abstract stimulus representation in the hierarchical network model was reflected in different parts of the visual cortex, following the visual ventral stream. We further validated the accuracy of our encoding model by decoding stimulus identity in a left-out validation set of viewed objects, achieving state-of-the-art decoding accuracy. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
33. Long‐term anesthetic protocol in rats: feasibility in electrophysiology studies in visual prosthesis.
- Author
-
Barriga‐Rivera, Alejandro, Tatarinoff, Veronica, Lovell, Nigel H., Morley, John W., and Suaning, Gregg J.
- Subjects
- *
ARTIFICIAL vision , *ANESTHETICS , *ELECTROPHYSIOLOGY , *VISUAL perception , *LABORATORY rats - Abstract
Abstract: Electrical stimulation of excitable cells provides therapeutic benefits for a variety of medical conditions, including restoration of partial vision to those blinded via some types of retinal degeneration. To improve visual percepts elicited by the current technology, researchers are conducting acute electrophysiology experiments, mainly in cats. However, the rat can provide a model of a range of retinal diseases and possesses a sufficiently large eye to be used in this field. This article presents a long‐term anesthetic protocol to enable electrophysiology experiments to further the development of visual prostheses. Six Long‐Evans rats (aged between 14 and 16 weeks) were included in this study. Surgical anesthesia was maintained for more than 15 h by combining constant intravenous infusion of ketamine (24.0–34.5 mg/kg/h), xylazine (0.9–1.2 mg/kg/h), and inhaled isoflurane in oxygen (<0.5%). Overall heart rate, respiratory rate, and body temperature remained between 187–233 beats/min, 45–58 breaths/min, and 36–38 °C, respectively. Neural responses to 200‐ms light pulses were recorded from the superior colliculus using a 32‐channel neural probe at the beginning and before termination of the experiment. Robust responses were recorded from distinct functional types of retinal pathways. In addition, a platinum electrode was implanted in the retrobulbar space. The retina was electrically stimulated, and the activation threshold was determined to be 5.24 ± 0.24 μC/cm2. This protocol may be used not only in the field of visual prosthesis research, but in other research areas requiring longer term acute experiments. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
34. Altered white matter structure in the visual system following early monocular enucleation.
- Author
-
Wong, Nikita A., Rafique, Sara A., Kelly, Krista R., Moro, Stefania S., Gallie, Brenda L., and Steeves, Jennifer K. E.
- Abstract
Partial visual deprivation from early monocular enucleation (the surgical removal of one eye within the first few years of life) results in a number of long-term morphological adaptations in adult cortical and subcortical visual, auditory, and multisensory brain regions. In this study, we investigated whether early monocular enucleation also results in the altered development of white matter structure. Diffusion tensor imaging and probabilistic tractography were performed to assess potential differences in visual system white matter in adult participants who had undergone early monocular enucleation compared to binocularly intact controls. To examine the microstructural properties of these tracts, mean diffusion parameters including fractional anisotropy (FA), mean diffusivity (MD), axial diffusivity (AD), and radial diffusivity (RD) were extracted bilaterally. Asymmetries opposite to those observed in controls were found for FA, MD, and RD in the optic radiations, the projections from primary visual cortex (V1) to the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN), and the interhemispheric V1 projections of early monocular enucleation participants. Early monocular enucleation was also associated with significantly lower FA bidirectionally in the interhemispheric V1 projections. These differences were consistently greater for the tracts contralateral to the enucleated eye, and are consistent with the asymmetric LGN volumes and optic tract diameters previously demonstrated in this group of participants. Overall, these results indicate that early monocular enucleation has long-term effects on white matter structure in the visual pathway that results in reduced fiber organization in tracts contralateral to the enucleated eye. Hum Brain Mapp 39:133-144, 2018. © 2017 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
35. “Not Art but Life”: 360° Vision Practised by the Egyptian Sphinxes in the Capital of Russia
- Author
-
Tillberg, Margareta and Tillberg, Margareta
- Abstract
As we all know – art is what we find in museums and – with the exception of statues of rulers and warlords in city squares – life is everything else. But wait! Not everyone viewed things in exactly the same way. The Group for Organic Culture visited different places in the city of Petersburg/ Petrograd/ Leningrad to make observations. One such place was the steps down to the water in front of the Academy of Arts. One day a conventional academy-professor saw them there and became outraged: "They sat with their backs to the river painting it." The spot however, was well chosen. What did they see? Oddly, there were (and still are) two stone sphinxes. What are Egyptian sphinxes doing in northern Europe? Why in St Petersburg? And why this location? What did they see from between the sphinxes on the steps down to the river Neva? From the well-chosen spot between the Egyptian sphinxes, they saw a panorama of tasteful palaces. They practiced vision with a 360° angle. But to be able to see what happened behind your back also meant having to be vigilant. Names mentioned in the text (in order of appearance): Albert Benois, Ivan Pavlov, Mikhail Matiushin, Ender siblings – Boris Ender, Ksenia Ender, Maria Ender, Yuri Ender, Nikolai Grinberg, Elena Guro, Napoleon, Tsar Peter the Great, Karl Marx, Lenin, Stalin, Trotsky, Empress Catherine the Great, Denis Diderot, Kazimir Malevich, "Seryi" - Grigorii Ginger, Herostratus, Evgenii Kovtun, Nikolai Punin, Rembrandt, Angelica Zander Rudenstine, Natalia Murray, Andrei Zhdanov, Putin, Russian Tsar Alexandre, Kirov, Gustav Eiffel, Tolstoy, George Costakis., As we all know – art is what we find in museums and – with the exception of statues of rulers and warlords in city squares – life is everything else. But wait! Not everyone viewed things in exactly the same way. The Group for Organic Culture visited different places in the city of Petersburg/ Petrograd/ Leningrad to make observations. One such place was the steps down to the water in front of the Academy of Arts. One day a conventional academy-professor saw them there and became outraged: "They sat with their backs to the river painting it." The spot however, was well chosen. What did they see? Oddly, there were (and still are) two stone sphinxes. What are Egyptian sphinxes doing in northern Europe? Why in St Petersburg? And why this location? What did they see from between the sphinxes on the steps down to the river Neva? From the well-chosen spot between the Egyptian sphinxes, they saw a panorama of tasteful palaces. They practiced vision with a 360° angle. But to be able to see what happened behind your back also meant having to be vigilant. Names mentioned in the text (in order of appearance): Albert Benois, Ivan Pavlov, Mikhail Matiushin, Ender siblings – Boris Ender, Ksenia Ender, Maria Ender, Yuri Ender, Nikolai Grinberg, Elena Guro, Napoleon, Tsar Peter the Great, Karl Marx, Lenin, Stalin, Trotsky, Empress Catherine the Great, Denis Diderot, Kazimir Malevich, "Seryi" - Grigorii Ginger, Herostratus, Evgenii Kovtun, Nikolai Punin, Rembrandt, Angelica Zander Rudenstine, Natalia Murray, Andrei Zhdanov, Putin, Russian Tsar Alexandre, Kirov, Gustav Eiffel, Tolstoy, George Costakis.
- Published
- 2022
36. Vision
- Subjects
visual neuroscience ,psychophysics and optometry ,immunology and microbiology of the eye and visual system ,clinical and epidemiological research ,ocular and neuronal cell biology ,supporting technologies for vision research ,Biology (General) ,QH301-705.5 - Published
- 2017
37. Visual Performance Fields
- Author
-
Benson, Noah, Kupers, Eline, Barbot, Antoine, Carrasco, Marisa, and Winawer, Jonathan
- Subjects
neuroscience ,V2 ,visual field ,striate cortex ,V1 ,vision ,cortex ,visual performance ,cortical magnification ,extrastriate cortex ,psychophysics ,visual cortex ,visual neuroscience - Abstract
This OSF page accompanies the publication "Cortical Magnification in Human Visual Cortex Parallels Task Performance around the Visual Field" by Benson, Kupers, Barbot, Carrasco, and Winawer (2021), published in the journal eLife. Data, code, and notebooks used in the calculations are provided and documented here.
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
38. Mapping Spatial Frequency Preferences Across Human Primary Visual Cortex
- Author
-
Eero Simoncelli, Jonathan Winawer, and William Broderick
- Subjects
Neurons ,Brain Mapping ,striate cortex ,V1 ,vision ,fMRI ,spatial vision ,spatial frequency ,Magnetic Resonance Imaging ,Sensory Systems ,neuroscience ,Ophthalmology ,Primary Visual Cortex ,Animals ,Humans ,human neuroscience ,Visual Fields ,visual cortex ,human vision ,visual neuroscience ,human brain ,Photic Stimulation - Abstract
Neurons in primate visual cortex (area V1) are tuned for spatial frequency, in a manner that depends on their position in the visual field. Several studies have examined this dependency using fMRI, reporting preferred spatial frequencies (tuning curve peaks) of V1 voxels as a function of eccentricity, but their results differ by as much as two octaves, presumably due to differences in stimuli, measurements, and analysis methodology. Here, we characterize spatial frequency tuning at a millimeter resolution within human primary visual cortex, across stimulus orientation and visual field locations. We measured fMRI responses to a novel set of stimuli, constructed as sinusoidal gratings in log-polar coordinates, which include circular, radial, and spiral geometries. For each individual stimulus, the local spatial frequency varies inversely with eccentricity, and for any given location in the visual field, the full set of stimuli span a broad range of spatial frequencies and orientations. Over the measured range of eccentricities, the preferred spatial frequency is well-fit by a function that varies as the inverse of the eccentricity plus a small constant. We also find small but systematic effects of local stimulus orientation, defined in both absolute coordinates and relative to visual field location. Specifically, peak spatial frequency is higher for pinwheel than annular stimuli and for horizontal than vertical stimuli.
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
39. Variability of V1, V2, and V3 in a Large Sample of Human Observers
- Author
-
Benson, Noah, Yoon, Jennifer, Forenzo, Dylan, Kay, Kendrick, Engel, Stephen, and Winawer, Jonathan
- Subjects
V2 ,striate cortex ,V1 ,vision ,V3 ,twins ,heritability ,neuroscience ,human connectome project ,extastriate cortex ,human neuroscience ,retinotopic maps ,retinotopy ,visual cortex ,human vision ,visual neuroscience ,human brain - Abstract
This repository contains data files and code/scripts used in the paper "Variability of V1, V2, and V3 in a large sample of human observers" by Benson, Yoon, Forenzo, Kay, Engel, and Winawer (2020). See the Wiki for information on how to use the dataset.
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
40. Image Sharpness and Contrast Tuning in the Early Visual Pathway.
- Author
-
Sánchez, Eduardo, Ferreiroa, Rubén, Arias, Adrián, and Martínez, Luis M.
- Subjects
- *
RECEPTIVE fields (Neurology) , *VISUAL pathways , *RETINAL ganglion cells , *VISUAL cortex , *PHOTORECEPTORS - Abstract
The center-surround organization of the receptive fields (RFs) of retinal ganglion cells highlights the presence of local contrast in visual stimuli. As RF of thalamic relay cells follow the same basic functional organization, it is often assumed that they contribute very little to alter the retinal output. However, in many species, thalamic relay cells largely outnumber their retinal inputs, which diverge to contact simultaneously several units at thalamic level. This gain in cell population as well as retinothalamic convergence opens the door to question how information about contrast is transformed at the thalamic stage. Here, we address this question using a realistic dynamic model of the retinothalamic circuit. Our results show that different components of the thalamic RF might implement filters that are analogous to two types of well-known image processing techniques to preserve the quality of a higher resolution version of the image on its way to the primary visual cortex. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
41. Synaptic properties of parabigeminal circuits.
- Author
-
Whyland, Kyle
- Subjects
- neuroscience, parabigeminal nucleus, neuroanatomy, synaptic properties, visual neuroscience, sensory neuroscience, Molecular and Cellular Neuroscience, Systems Neuroscience
- Abstract
Subcortical structures of the visual system have been the subject of intense study in recent years, but there remain some important unanswered questions regarding the synaptic relationships linking the nuclei that comprise this important sensory network within the brain. In these studies, we use several modern and traditional approaches, including viral tract tracing, in vitro slice physiology, immunohistochemistry, optogenetics, and electron microscopy to characterize the circuits linking the superior colliculus (SC), parabigeminal nucleus (PBG), and lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN), with particular focus on GABAergic and cholinergic cell types. We found that the SC, an important visuomotor structure with connections to the LGN, PBG, pretectum, and pulvinar nucleus (among others), hosts a large and diverse population of GABAergic neurons that is composed primarily of interneurons labeled in the GAD67-GFP reporter mouse. Among these GABAergic cells residing within stratum griseum superficiale (SGS) of the SC, we found that GABAergic projection cells targeting the PBG exhibit unique characteristics, distinguishing them from other GABAergic projection populations as well as parvalbumin positive SC-PBG cells that exert opposing effects on postsynaptic cells in the PBG. Utilizing optogenetics in conjunction with whole-cell patch-clamp slice physiology, we additionally demonstrate a high degree of convergence between these opposing tectal inputs onto PBG neurons. Our electron microscopy experiments and lightlevel immunohistochemistry in GAD67 reporter mice also revealed a previously unidentified extra-tectal souce of GABAergic input to the PBG, where these inhibitory synapses were observed to account for almost half of its inputs. Finally, using MATH5-/- mutant mice that lack retinofugal projections, we identify the largely cholinergic inputs from the PBG as the likely source of “retinal replacement terminals” observed in the LGN in previous studies utilizing animal models of retinofugal input loss.
- Published
- 2023
42. Dendritic Morphology of an Inhibitory Retinal Interneuron Enables Simultaneous Local and Global Synaptic Integration
- Author
-
Espen Hartveit, Margaret Lin Veruki, and Bas-Jan Zandt
- Subjects
Basic medical, dental and veterinary sciences: 710 [VDP] ,Male ,General Neuroscience ,Neurosciences ,Neurophysiology ,Dendrites ,Neurovitenskap / nevrovitenskap ,Axons ,Retina ,Rats ,Visual neuroscience ,Amacrine Cells ,Interneurons ,Synapses ,Animals ,Female ,Basale medisinske, odontologiske og veterinærmedisinske fag: 710 [VDP] ,Nevrofysiologi ,Research Articles - Abstract
Amacrine cells, inhibitory interneurons of the retina, feature synaptic inputs and outputs in close proximity throughout their dendritic trees, making them notable exceptions to prototypical somato-dendritic integration with output transmitted via axonal action potentials. The extent of dendritic compartmentalization in amacrine cells with widely differing dendritic tree morphology, however, is largely unexplored. Combining compartmental modeling, dendritic Ca2+imaging, targeted microiontophoresis and multielectrode patch-clamp recording (voltage and current clamp, capacitance measurement of exocytosis), we investigated integration in the AII amacrine cell, a narrow-field electrically coupled interneuron that participates in multiple, distinct microcircuits. Physiological experiments were performed within vitroslices prepared from retinas of both male and female rats. We found that the morphology of the AII enables simultaneous local and global integration of inputs targeted to different dendritic regions. Local integration occurs within spatially restricted dendritic subunits and narrow time windows and is largely unaffected by the strength of electrical coupling. In contrast, global integration across the dendritic tree occurs over longer time periods and is markedly influenced by the strength of electrical coupling. These integrative properties enable AII amacrines to combine local control of synaptic plasticity with location-independent global integration. Dynamic inhibitory control of dendritic subunits is likely to be of general importance for amacrine cells, including cells with small dendritic trees, as well as for inhibitory interneurons in other regions of the CNS.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENTOur understanding of synaptic integration is based on the prototypical morphology of a neuron with multiple dendrites and a single axon at opposing ends of a cell body. Many neurons, notably retinal amacrine cells, are exceptions to this arrangement, and display input and output synapses interspersed along their dendritic branches. In the large dendritic trees of some amacrine cells, such arrangements can give rise to multiple computational subunits. Other amacrine cells, with small dendritic trees, have been assumed to operate as single computational units. Here, we report the surprising result that despite a small dendritic tree, the AII amacrine cell simultaneously performs local integration of synaptic inputs (over smaller dendritic subregions) and global integration across the entire cell.
- Published
- 2022
43. Columnar and Laminar Segregation of Retinal Input to the Primate Superior Colliculus Revealed by Anterograde Tracer Injection Into Each Eye
- Author
-
Mikayla D. Dilbeck, Zachary R. Spahr, Rakesh Nanjappa, John R. Economides, and Jonathan C. Horton
- Subjects
Male ,Superior Colliculi ,genetic structures ,Proline ,Tritium ,proline autoradiography ,Macaca mulatta ,saccade ,eye diseases ,Axons ,pretectal olivary nucleus ,Neuroanatomical Tract-Tracing Techniques ,Animals ,Autoradiography ,Visual Pathways ,Alexa Fluor ,sense organs ,Visual Neuroscience ,cholera toxin subunit B ,Fluorescent Dyes ,Retinal Neurons - Abstract
Purpose After the lateral geniculate nucleus, the superior colliculus is the richest target of retinal projections in primates. Hubel et al. used tritium autoradiography to show that axon terminals emanating from one eye form irregular columns in the stratum griseum superficiale. Unlabeled gaps were thought to be filled by the other eye, but this assumption was never tested directly. Methods Experiments were performed in two normal macaques. In monkey 1, [3H]proline was injected into the left eye and the pattern of radiolabeling was examined in serial cross-sections through the entire superior colliculus. In monkey 2, cholera toxin subunit B conjugated to Alexa 488 was injected into the right eye and cholera toxin subunit B - Alexa 594 was injected into the left eye. The two fluorescent labels were compared in a reconstruction of the superior colliculus prepared from serial sections. Results In monkey 1, irregular columns of axon terminals were present in the superficial grey. The projection from the peripheral retina was stronger than the projection from the macula. In monkey 2, the two fluorescent Alexa tracers mainly interdigitated: a conspicuous gap in one label was usually filled by a clump of the other label. There was also partial laminar segregation of ocular inputs. In the far peripheral field representation, the contralateral eye's input generally terminated closer to the tectal surface. In the midperiphery the eyes switched, bringing the ipsilateral input nearer the surface. Conclusions Direct retinal input to the macaque superior colliculus is segregated into alternating columns and strata, despite the fact that tectal cells respond robustly to stimulation of either eye.
- Published
- 2022
44. Retinal Dopamine D2 Receptors Participate in the Development of Myopia in Mice
- Author
-
Furong Huang, Ziheng Shu, Qin Huang, Kaijie Chen, Wenjun Yan, Wenjing Wu, Jinglei Yang, Qiongsi Wang, Fengjiao Wang, Chunlan Zhang, Jia Qu, and Xiangtian Zhou
- Subjects
genetic structures ,Receptors, Dopamine D2 ,eye diseases ,Mice, Mutant Strains ,Retina ,D2 receptor ,Mice, Inbred C57BL ,Disease Models, Animal ,Mice ,Gene Expression Regulation ,Electroretinography ,Myopia ,Animals ,RNA ,sense organs ,Visual Neuroscience ,dopamine ,mouse - Abstract
Purpose To learn more about the locations of dopamine D2 receptors (D2Rs) that regulate form-deprivation myopia (FDM), using different transgenic mouse models. Methods One eye of D2R-knockout (KO) mice and wild-type littermates was subjected to four weeks of monocular FDM, whereas the fellow eye served as control. Mice in both groups received daily intraperitoneal injections of either the D2R antagonist sulpiride (8 µg/g) or vehicle alone. FDM was also induced in retina- (Six3creD2Rfl/fl) or fibroblast-specific (S100a4creD2Rfl/fl) D2R-KO mice. A subset of retina-specific D2R-KO mice and D2Rfl/fl littermates were also given sulpiride or vehicle injections. Refraction was measured with an eccentric infrared photorefractor, and other biometric parameters were measured by optical coherence tomography (n ≈ 20 for each group). Results FDM development was attenuated in wild-type littermates treated with sulpiride. However, this inhibitory effect disappeared in the D2R-KO mice, suggesting that antagonizing D2Rs suppressed myopia development. Similarly, the development of myopia was partially inhibited by retina-specific (deletion efficiency: 94.7%) but not fibroblast-specific (66.9%) D2R-KO. The sulpiride-mediated inhibitory effects on FDM also disappeared with retinal D2R-KO, suggesting that antagonizing D2Rs outside the retina may not attenuate myopia. Changes in axial length were less marked than changes in refraction, but in general the two were correlated. Conclusions This study demonstrates that D2Rs located in the retina participate in dopaminergic regulation of FDM in mice. These findings provide an important and fundamental basis for further exploring the retinal mechanism(s) involved in dopamine signaling and myopia development.
- Published
- 2022
45. Inhibitory inputs to an inhibitory interneuron: Spontaneous postsynaptic currents and GABA_A receptors of A17 amacrine cells in the rat retina
- Author
-
Pablo Beltrán‐Matas, Áurea Castilho, Barbora Tencer, Margaret L. Veruki, and Espen Hartveit
- Subjects
Visual neuroscience ,Basic medical, dental and veterinary sciences: 710 [VDP] ,General Neuroscience ,Neurosciences ,Biophysics ,Neurophysiology ,Basale medisinske, odontologiske og veterinærmedisinske fag: 710 [VDP] ,Nevrofysiologi ,Biofysikk ,Neurovitenskap / nevrovitenskap - Abstract
Amacrine cells constitute a large and heterogeneous group of inhibitory interneurons in the retina. The A17 amacrine plays an important role for visual signalling in the rod pathway microcircuit of the mammalian retina. It receives excitatory input from rod bipolar cells and provides feedback inhibition to the same cells. However, from ultrastructural investigations, there is evidence for input to A17s from other types of amacrine cells, presumably inhibitory, but there is a lack of information about the identity and functional properties of the synaptic receptors and how inhibition contributes to the integrative properties of A17s. Here, we studied the biophysical and pharmacological properties of GABAergic spontaneous inhibitory postsynaptic currents (spIPSCs) and GABA_A receptors of A17 amacrines using whole-cell and outside-out patch recordings from rat retinal slices. The spIPSCs displayed fast onsets (10%–90% rise time ~740 μs) and double-exponential decays (τ_fast ~4.5 ms [43% of amplitude]; τ_slow ~22 ms). Ultra-fast application of brief pulses of GABA (3 mM) to patches evoked responses with deactivation kinetics best fitted by a triple-exponential function (τ1 ~5.3 ms [55% of amplitude]; τ2 ~48 ms [32% of amplitude]; τ3 ~187 ms). Non-stationary noise analysis of spIPSCs and patch responses yielded single-channel conductances of ~21 and ~25 pS, respectively. Pharmacological analysis suggested that the spIPSCs are mediated by receptors with an α1βγ2 subunit composition and the somatic receptors have an α2βγ2 and/or α3βγ2 composition. These results demonstrate the presence of synaptic GABA_A receptors on A17s, which may play an important role in signal integration in these cells. publishedVersion
- Published
- 2022
46. Pupillary Light Reflex Induced by Two-Photon Vision
- Author
-
Agnieszka Zielińska, Piotr Ciąćka, Maciej Szkulmowski, and Katarzyna Komar
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Photons ,perception of light ,Light ,Infrared Rays ,Dark Adaptation ,Pupil ,Reflex, Pupillary ,two-photon vision ,pupillary light reflex ,Young Adult ,Reaction Time ,Humans ,Female ,Visual Neuroscience - Abstract
Purpose Two-photon vision relies on the perception of pulsed infrared light due to two-photon absorption in visual pigments. This study aimed to measure human pupil reaction caused by a two-photon 1040-nm stimulus and compare it with pupil responses elicited by 520-nm stimuli of similar color. Methods Pupillary light reflex (PLR) was induced on 14 dark-adapted healthy subjects. Three types of fovea-centered stimuli of 3.5° diameter were tested: spirals formed by fast scanning 1040-nm (infrared [IR] laser) or 520-nm (visible [VIS] laser) laser beams and uniformly filled circle created by 520-nm LED (VIS light-emitting diode [LED]). The power of visible stimuli was determined with a dedicated procedure to obtain the same perceived brightness equivalent as for 800 µW used for two-photon stimulation. Results The minimum pupil diameter for IR laser was 88% ± 10% of baseline, significantly larger than for both VIS stimuli: 74% ± 10% (laser) and 69% ± 9% (LED). Mean constriction velocity and time to maximum constriction had significantly smaller values for IR than for both VIS stimuli. Latency times were similar for IR and VIS lasers and slightly smaller for VIS LED. Conclusions The two-photon stimulus caused a considerably weaker pupil reaction than one-photon stimuli of the same shape, brightness, and similar color. The smaller pupil response may be due to weaker two-photon stimulation of rods relative to cones as previously observed for two-photon vision. Contrary to normal vision, in a two-photon process the stray light is not perceived, which might reduce the number of stimulated photoreceptors and further weaken the PLR.
- Published
- 2021
47. Alertness and Visual Attention Impact Different Aspects of the Optokinetic Reflex
- Author
-
Davide Frattini and Tobias Wibble
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Visual perception ,Visual acuity ,genetic structures ,optokinetic nystagmus ,Nystagmus ,Audiology ,Pupil ,ocular torsion ,Reference Values ,Reflex ,medicine ,Pupillary response ,Humans ,Attention ,Nystagmus, Optokinetic ,alertness ,General Medicine ,Optokinetic reflex ,Awareness ,Adaptation, Physiological ,eye diseases ,Alertness ,visual attention ,Visual Perception ,Eye tracking ,Female ,medicine.symptom ,Visual Neuroscience ,Psychology - Abstract
Purpose Assessing visual attention and alertness is of great importance in visual and cognitive neuroscience, providing objective measures valuable for both researchers and clinicians. This study investigates how the optokinetic response differs between levels of visual attention in healthy adults while controlling for alertness. Methods Twelve healthy subjects (8 men and 4 women; mean age = 33 ± 9.36) with intact gaze-stability, visual acuity, and binocularity were recruited. Subjects viewed a rotating visual scene provoking torsional optokinetic nystagmus (OKN) while wearing a video eye tracker in a seated head-fixed position. Tasks requiring focused, neutral, and divided visual attention were issued to each subject and the OKN was recorded. Pupil sizes were monitored as a proxy for alertness. Results Pupil dilation was increased for both focused and divided visual attention. The number of nystagmus beats was highest for the focused condition and lowest for the divided attentional task. OKN gain was increased during both focused and divided attention. The distribution of nystagmus beats over time showed that only focused attention produced a reliable adaptation of the OKN. Conclusions Results consequently indicate that OKN frequency is adaptive to a viewer's level of visual attention, whereas OKN gain is influenced by alertness levels. This pattern offers insight into the neural processes integrating visual input with reflexive motor responses. For example, it contextualizes why attention to visual stimuli can cause dizziness, as the OKN frequency reflects activity of the velocity storage mechanism. Additionally, the OKN could offer a possible venue for differentiating between visual attention and alertness during psychometric testing.
- Published
- 2021
48. The contribution of single case studies to the neuroscience of vision.
- Author
-
Zihl, Josef and Heywood, Charles A.
- Subjects
- *
NEUROSCIENCES , *VISUAL agnosia , *VISUAL perception , *DISSOCIATION (Psychology) , *NEUROBIOLOGY - Abstract
Visual neuroscience is concerned with the neurobiological foundations of visual perception, that is, the morphological, physiological, and functional organization of the visual brain and its co-operative partners. One important approach for understanding the functional organization of the visual brain is the study of visual perception from the pathological perspective. The study of patients with focal injury to the visual brain allows conclusions about the representation of visual perceptual functions in the framework of association and dissociation of functions. Selective disorders have been reported for more 'elementary' visual capabilities, for example, color and movement vision, but also for visuo-cognitive capacities, such as visual agnosia or the visual field of attention. Because these visual disorders occur rather seldom as selective and specific dysfunctions, single cases have always played, and still play, a significant role in gaining insights into the functional organization of the visual brain. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
49. Object size classification and sensorimotor decision making in the larval zebrafish
- Author
-
Barker, Alison Joyce
- Subjects
Neurosciences ,object size classification ,optic tectum ,visual neuroscience ,zebrafish - Abstract
An organism’s survival depends on its success in evading threats and acquiring resources. These basic needs require that sensory information be processed to maximize behavioral outputs in a given environment. The appropriate classification of visual objects can be used to drive context specific behavior (e.g. avoiding potential threats or pursuing potential food sources). In this work using the larval zebrafish as a model, I explore two central questions: How is visual information classified? Where along the visuomotor pathway is “positive” or “negative” value assigned to perceived objects? This work succeeds in linking visual perception (evaluation of object size) to action selection (deciding between approach and avoidance). Chapter 2 describes the development of behavioral paradigms to evaluate object size classification. A free swimming assay for the larval zebrafish was developed that elicited both approach and avoidance through the presentation of moving dots of different sizes. This assay was used to identify neural components mediating both approach and avoidance behaviors. Chapters 3 and 4 investigate the roles of specific neuronal populations that contribute to the implementation of size-mediated behaviors. A significant advance is the identification of a population of glutamatergic interneurons residing in the optic tectum which are components of a neural pathway for approach towards small objects. Ablation of these neurons results in a shift from approach to avoidance when small moving dots are presented. Conversely activating these interneurons enhances approaches to small dots. These results suggests that neural circuits in the optic tectum are actively involved in object classification which biases behavioral output towards approach or avoidance.
- Published
- 2015
50. The Role of Dopamine in Emmetropization Modulated by Wavelength and Temporal Frequency in Guinea Pigs
- Author
-
Leilei Zou, Tian Tian, Rui Liu, Shu Wang, and Hong Liu
- Subjects
3,4-Dihydroxyphenylacetic acid ,Light ,Dopamine ,temporal ,Guinea Pigs ,Refraction, Ocular ,Retina ,chemistry.chemical_compound ,wavelength ,White light ,medicine ,Myopia ,Animals ,Retinoscopy ,Chromatography, High Pressure Liquid ,Blue light ,Ultrasonography ,medicine.diagnostic_test ,Retinal ,Emmetropia ,Vitreous Body ,Wavelength ,Axial Length, Eye ,Hyperopia ,chemistry ,Ultrasound imaging ,Biophysics ,3,4-Dihydroxyphenylacetic Acid ,Visual Neuroscience ,medicine.drug - Abstract
Purpose Wavelength and temporal frequency have been found to influence refractive development. This study investigated whether retinal dopamine (DA) plays a role in these processes. Methods Guinea pigs were randomly divided into nine groups that received different lighting conditions for 4 weeks, as follows: white, green, or blue light at 0, 0.5, or 20.0 Hz. Refractions and axial lengths were measured using streak retinoscopy and A-scan ultrasound imaging. DA and its metabolites were measured by high-pressure liquid chromatography-electrochemical detection. Results At 0 Hz, green and blue light produced myopic and hyperopic shifts compared with that of white light. At 0.5 Hz, no significant changes were observed compared with those of green or blue light at 0 Hz, whereas white light at 0.5 Hz induced a myopic shift compared with white light at 0 or 20 Hz. At 20 Hz, green and blue light acted like white light. Among all levels of DA and its metabolites, only vitreous 3, 4-dihydroxyphenylacetic acid (DOPAC) levels and retinal DOPAC/DA ratios were dependent on wavelength, frequency, and their interaction. Specifically, retinal DOPAC/DA ratios were positively correlated with refractions in white and green light conditions. However, blue light (0, 0.5, and 20.0 Hz) produced hyperopic shifts but decreased vitreous DOPAC levels and retinal DOPAC/DA ratios. Conclusions The retinal DOPAC/DA ratio, indicating the metabolic efficiency of DA, is correlated with ocular growth. It may underlie myopic shifts from light exposure with a long wavelength and low temporal frequency. However, different biochemical pathways may contribute to the hyperopic shifts from short wavelength light.
- Published
- 2021
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.