240 results on '"Steven Pinker"'
Search Results
2. Why nature & nurture won’t go away
- Author
-
Steven Pinker
- Subjects
Nature-Nurture ,Environment ,Parenting ,Holistic Interactionism ,Genética-Cultura ,Meio Ambiente ,Educação dos Pais ,Interacionismo Holístico ,Medio Ambiente ,Educación Familiar ,Social Sciences ,Social sciences (General) ,H1-99 - Abstract
The nature-nurture debate has preoccupied psychology and the social sciences for centuries. Many writers have expressed a hope for a compromise that it will make the debate disappear.. In this view, all behavior comes from an inextricable interaction between heredity and environment, and it is a mistake to try to tease them apart. reasons, among them that it is simply false that all aspects of brain function involve a mixture of heredity and environment, and that holistic interactionism obscures our for several understanding of how the mind works. As an illustration, I discuss the case of the effects of parenting, where holistic interactionism has led to false and misleading conclusions.
- Published
- 2006
3. How to be more rational/A rationality reboot/Let's get logical 'Conspiracy theories are probably as old as human groups. Paranormal woo isn't new. Neither is fake news'
- Author
-
Steven Pinker
- Subjects
Multidisciplinary - Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
4. « La rationalité nous rend plus libres »
- Author
-
Steven Pinker and Sébastien Bohler
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
5. Inequality and Progress
- Author
-
Steven Pinker
- Subjects
Inequality ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Development economics ,Economics ,media_common - Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
6. Piled Modifiers, Buried Verbs, and Other Turgid Prose in the American Political Science Review
- Author
-
Steven Pinker and Peter DeScioli
- Subjects
Literature ,Sociology and Political Science ,business.industry ,Political science ,American political science ,business - Abstract
Academic writing is notoriously difficult to read. Can political science do better? To assess the state of prose in political science, we examined a recent issue of the American Political Science Review. We evaluated the articles according to the basic principles of style endorsed by writing experts. We find that the writing suffers most from heavy noun phrases in forms such as noun noun noun and adjective adjective noun noun. Further, we describe five contributors that swell noun phrases: piled modifiers, needless words, nebulous nouns, missing prepositions, and buried verbs. We document more than a thousand examples and demonstrate how to revise each one with principles of style. We also draw on research in cognitive science to explain why these constructions confuse, mislead, and distract readers.
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
7. Enlightenment Environmentalism: A Humanistic Response to Climate Change
- Author
-
Steven Pinker
- Subjects
media_common.quotation_subject ,Environmentalism ,Enlightenment ,Climate change ,Environmental ethics ,Sociology ,Humanism ,media_common - Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
8. The Science of Our Better Angels
- Author
-
Steven, Pinker, primary
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
9. Reply to Joachim Krueger's Review of Rationality
- Author
-
Steven Pinker
- Subjects
Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology - Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
10. The Language Instinct: How The Mind Creates Language
- Author
-
Steven Pinker
- Published
- 2010
11. Common knowledge, coordination, and strategic mentalizing in human social life
- Author
-
Kyle A. Thomas, Peter DeScioli, Steven Pinker, and Julian De Freitas
- Subjects
Male ,coordination ,Emotions ,Theory of Mind ,Social Sciences ,cooperation ,Mentalization ,Theory of mind ,Common knowledge ,Humans ,Interpersonal Relations ,Coordination game ,Cooperative Behavior ,Structure (mathematical logic) ,Ad infinitum ,Multidisciplinary ,common knowledge ,Bystander Effect ,Object (philosophy) ,Epistemology ,Knowledge ,Social Perception ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,Female ,Psychology ,Attribution - Abstract
Significance Humans are an unusually cooperative species, and our cooperation is of 2 kinds: altruistic, when actors benefit others at a cost to themselves, and mutualistic, when actors benefit themselves and others simultaneously. One major form of mutualism is coordination, in which actors align their choices for mutual benefit. Formal examples include meetings, division of labor, and legal and technological standards; informal examples include friendships, authority hierarchies, alliances, and exchange partnerships. Successful coordination is enabled by common knowledge: knowledge of others’ knowledge, knowledge of their knowledge of one’s knowledge, ad infinitum. Uncovering how people acquire and represent the common knowledge needed for coordination is thus essential to understanding human sociality, from large-scale institutions to everyday experiences of civility, hypocrisy, outrage, and taboo., People often coordinate for mutual gain, such as keeping to opposite sides of a stairway, dubbing an object or place with a name, or assembling en masse to protest a regime. Because successful coordination requires complementary choices, these opportunities raise the puzzle of how people attain the common knowledge that facilitates coordination, in which a person knows X, knows that the other knows X, knows that the other knows that he knows, ad infinitum. We show that people are highly sensitive to the distinction between common knowledge and mere private or shared knowledge, and that they deploy this distinction strategically in diverse social situations that have the structure of coordination games, including market cooperation, innuendo, bystander intervention, attributions of charitability, self-conscious emotions, and moral condemnation.
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
12. How the Mind Works
- Author
-
Steven Pinker
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
13. Sex and drugs and rock and roll
- Author
-
Steven Pinker
- Subjects
Behavioral Neuroscience ,Sex and drugs ,Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology ,Physiology ,Aesthetics ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Quality (philosophy) ,Psychology ,Centrality ,Pleasure ,media_common ,Skepticism - Abstract
This article is extraordinarily rigorous and rich, although there are reasons to be skeptical of its theory that music originated to signal group quality and infant solicitude. These include the lack of any signature of the centrality of these functions in the distribution or experience of music; of a role for the pleasure taken in music; and of its connections with language.
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
14. The pandemic exposes human nature: 10 evolutionary insights
- Author
-
Sam Harris, Steven Pinker, David M. Buss, Joe Alcock, Barbara N. Horowitz, Paul Bloom, Benjamin M. Seitz, David Wilson, Michele J. Gelfand, Athena Aktipis, Debra Lieberman, and Martie G. Haselton
- Subjects
Male ,Multidisciplinary ,Human Characteristics ,Physical Distancing ,COVID-19 ,Evolutionary medicine ,Environmental ethics ,Disease ,Biological Evolution ,Evolutionary psychology ,humanities ,Disgust ,Political science ,Perspective ,Psychological level ,Pandemic ,Humans ,Female ,Social Behavior ,Sociocultural evolution ,Social experiment ,Pandemics ,Demography - Abstract
Humans and viruses have been coevolving for millennia. Severe acute respiratory syndrome coronavirus 2 (SARS-CoV-2, the virus that causes COVID-19) has been particularly successful in evading our evolved defenses. The outcome has been tragic—across the globe, millions have been sickened and hundreds of thousands have died. Moreover, the quarantine has radically changed the structure of our lives, with devastating social and economic consequences that are likely to unfold for years. An evolutionary perspective can help us understand the progression and consequences of the pandemic. Here, a diverse group of scientists, with expertise from evolutionary medicine to cultural evolution, provide insights about the pandemic and its aftermath. At the most granular level, we consider how viruses might affect social behavior, and how quarantine, ironically, could make us susceptible to other maladies, due to a lack of microbial exposure. At the psychological level, we describe the ways in which the pandemic can affect mating behavior, cooperation (or the lack thereof), and gender norms, and how we can use disgust to better activate native “behavioral immunity” to combat disease spread. At the cultural level, we describe shifting cultural norms and how we might harness them to better combat disease and the negative social consequences of the pandemic. These insights can be used to craft solutions to problems produced by the pandemic and to lay the groundwork for a scientific agenda to capture and understand what has become, in effect, a worldwide social experiment.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
15. [Language Acquisition: How Do They Do It?]*
- Author
-
Steven Pinker
- Subjects
Computer science ,Language acquisition ,Linguistics - Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
16. Why No Mere Mortal Has Ever Flown Out to Center Field.
- Author
-
John J. Kim, Steven Pinker, Alan Price, and Sandeep Prasada
- Published
- 1991
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
17. A critical period for second language acquisition: Evidence from 2/3 million English speakers
- Author
-
Joshua B. Tenenbaum, Steven Pinker, Joshua K. Hartshorne, and Massachusetts Institute of Technology. Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences
- Subjects
Adult ,Linguistics and Language ,Time Factors ,Current age ,Offset (computer science) ,Adolescent ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,education ,Multilingualism ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Models, Psychological ,Language Development ,Article ,050105 experimental psychology ,Language and Linguistics ,Young Adult ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,Humans ,Learning ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Child ,Aged ,Language ,Aged, 80 and over ,Critical Period, Psychological ,05 social sciences ,Age Factors ,Linguistics ,Middle Aged ,Language acquisition ,Second-language acquisition ,Psychology ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Children learn language more easily than adults, though when and why this ability declines have been obscure for both empirical reasons (underpowered studies) and conceptual reasons (measuring the ultimate attainment of learners who started at different ages cannot by itself reveal changes in underlying learning ability). We address both limitations with a dataset of unprecedented size (669,498 native and non-native English speakers) and a computational model that estimates the trajectory of underlying learning ability by disentangling current age, age at first exposure, and years of experience. This allows us to provide the first direct estimate of how grammar-learning ability changes with age, finding that it is preserved almost to the crux of adulthood (17.4 years old) and then declines steadily. This finding held not only for “difficult” syntactic phenomena but also for “easy” syntactic phenomena that are normally mastered early in acquisition. The results support the existence of a sharply-defined critical period for language acquisition, but the age of offset is much later than previously speculated. The size of the dataset also provides novel insight into several other outstanding questions in language acquisition., National Institutes of Health (U.S.). Ruth L. Kirschstein National Research Service Award (5F32HD072748), National Science Foundation (U.S.). Center for Minds, Brains, & Machines (Grant NSF STC CCF-1231216)
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
18. Common knowledge, coordination, and the logic of self-conscious emotions
- Author
-
Kyle A. Thomas, Steven Pinker, and Peter DeScioli
- Subjects
Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,Self-conscious emotions ,05 social sciences ,Common knowledge ,050109 social psychology ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Psychology ,050105 experimental psychology ,Ecology, Evolution, Behavior and Systematics ,Cognitive psychology - Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
19. Rationality : What It Is, Why It Seems Scarce, Why It Matters
- Author
-
Steven Pinker and Steven Pinker
- Subjects
- Practical reason, Critical thinking, Choice (Psychology)
- Abstract
NEW YORK TIMES BESTSELLER“In our uncertain age, which can so often feel so dark and disturbing, Steven Pinker has distinguished himself as a voice of positivity.” – New York TimesCan reading a book make you more rational? Can it help us understand why there is so much irrationality in the world? Steven Pinker, author of Enlightenment Now (Bill Gates's'new favorite book of all time”) answers all the questions here Today humanity is reaching new heights of scientific understanding--and also appears to be losing its mind. How can a species that developed vaccines for Covid-19 in less than a year produce so much fake news, medical quackery, and conspiracy theorizing? Pinker rejects the cynical cliché that humans are simply irrational--cavemen out of time saddled with biases, fallacies, and illusions. After all, we discovered the laws of nature, lengthened and enriched our lives, and set out the benchmarks for rationality itself. We actually think in ways that are sensible in the low-tech contexts in which we spend most of our lives, but fail to take advantage of the powerful tools of reasoning we've discovered over the millennia: logic, critical thinking, probability, correlation and causation, and optimal ways to update beliefs and commit to choices individually and with others. These tools are not a standard part of our education, and have never been presented clearly and entertainingly in a single book--until now. Rationality also explores its opposite: how the rational pursuit of self-interest, sectarian solidarity, and uplifting mythology can add up to crippling irrationality in a society. Collective rationality depends on norms that are explicitly designed to promote objectivity and truth. Rationality matters. It leads to better choices in our lives and in the public sphere, and is the ultimate driver of social justice and moral progress. Brimming with Pinker's customary insight and humor, Rationality will enlighten, inspire, and empower.
- Published
- 2021
20. Nous vivons dans un monde de moins en moins violent
- Author
-
Steven Pinker and Sébastien Bohler
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
21. Judith Rich Harris (1938–2019)
- Author
-
Steven Pinker
- Subjects
Visual search ,Childhood development ,Perception ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Personality development ,Socialization ,Psychophysics ,Personality ,General Medicine ,Psychology ,General Psychology ,media_common ,Developmental psychology - Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
22. Body Language: Come Padroneggiare L'arte Della Comunicazione Non Verbale
- Author
-
Steven Pinker and Steven Pinker
- Abstract
Imparando ad usare questi principi, sarete in grado di migliorare diversi aspetti della vostra vita, come le vostre relazioni, la vostra carriera e la vostra vita sociale in generale. Le informazioni contenute in questo libro vi forniranno indicazioni su come attuare una comunicazione efficace e su come utilizzarla in maniera attiva. Se siete pronti ad agire e a cambiare la vostra vita in meglio, questo libro vi guiderà sicuramente nella direzione giusta!
- Published
- 2019
23. Recursive mentalizing and common knowledge in the bystander effect
- Author
-
Peter DeScioli, Kyle A. Thomas, Julian De Freitas, and Steven Pinker
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Emotions ,05 social sciences ,Theory of Mind ,Diffusion of responsibility ,050109 social psychology ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Bystander Effect ,050105 experimental psychology ,Dilemma ,Developmental Neuroscience ,Mentalization ,Theory of mind ,Common knowledge ,Bystander effect ,Humans ,Female ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Social Behavior ,Psychology ,Social psychology ,General Psychology - Abstract
The more potential helpers there are, the less likely any individual is to help. A traditional explanation for this bystander effect is that responsibility diffuses across the multiple bystanders, diluting the responsibility of each. We investigate an alternative, which combines the volunteer's dilemma (each bystander is best off if another responds) with recursive theory of mind (each infers what the others know about what he knows) to predict that actors will strategically shirk when they think others feel compelled to help. In 3 experiments, participants responded to a (fictional) person who needed help from at least 1 volunteer. Participants were in groups of 2 or 5 and had varying information about whether other group members knew that help was needed. As predicted, people's decision to help zigzagged with the depth of their asymmetric, recursive knowledge (e.g., "John knows that Michael knows that John knows help is needed"), and replicated the classic bystander effect when they had common knowledge (everyone knowing what everyone knows). The results demonstrate that the bystander effect may result not from a mere diffusion of responsibility but specifically from actors' strategic computations.
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
24. Universality and diversity in human song
- Author
-
Timothy J. O'Donnell, Dean Knox, Jan Simson, Rhea M. Howard, Constance M. Bainbridge, Luke Glowacki, S. Atwood, Daniel Pickens-Jones, Manvir Singh, Max M. Krasnow, Steven Pinker, Daniel Ketter, Joshua K. Hartshorne, Erin J. Hopkins, Mariela V. Jennings, Chris Lucas, Nori Jacoby, Samuel A. Mehr, and Alena Egner
- Subjects
Dance ,Musical ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Cultural Psychology|Multi-cultural Psychology ,0302 clinical medicine ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Perception|Audition ,B- ECONOMIE ET FINANCE ,0303 health sciences ,bepress|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Psychology ,Multidisciplinary ,05 social sciences ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Quantitative Methods|Computational Modeling ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Cultural Psychology|Cross-cultural Psychology ,Formality ,bepress|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Psychology|Multicultural Psychology ,Linguistics ,Religion ,bepress|Arts and Humanities|Music ,Auditory Perception ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Cognitive Psychology ,Singing ,Psychology ,Amateur ,Psychoacoustics ,Cross-Cultural Comparison ,Melody ,bepress|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Psychology|Cognition and Perception ,bepress|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Psychology|Quantitative Psychology ,Article ,050105 experimental psychology ,03 medical and health sciences ,Humans ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Evolution|Evolutionary Biology ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Quantitative Methods|Statistical Methods ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Dancing ,Tonality ,Anthropology, Cultural ,030304 developmental biology ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Evolution ,Behavior ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Perception ,Infant, Newborn ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Cultural Psychology ,Love ,Cross-cultural studies ,bepress|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Psychology|Cognitive Psychology ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Music ,Infant Care ,bepress|Social and Behavioral Sciences ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Quantitative Methods ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Emotion ,Music ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,bepress|Life Sciences|Ecology and Evolutionary Biology|Evolution - Abstract
Cross-cultural analysis of song It is unclear whether there are universal patterns to music across cultures. Mehr et al. examined ethnographic data and observed music in every society sampled (see the Perspective by Fitch and Popescu). For songs specifically, three dimensions characterize more than 25% of the performances studied: formality of the performance, arousal level, and religiosity. There is more variation in musical behavior within societies than between societies, and societies show similar levels of within-society variation in musical behavior. At the same time, one-third of societies significantly differ from average for any given dimension, and half of all societies differ from average on at least one dimension, indicating variability across cultures. Science , this issue p. eaax0868 ; see also p. 944
- Published
- 2018
25. Maimonides' ladder: States of mutual knowledge and the perception of charitability
- Author
-
Kyle A. Thomas, Julian De Freitas, Peter DeScioli, and Steven Pinker
- Subjects
Adult ,Social perception ,media_common.quotation_subject ,05 social sciences ,Identity (social science) ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Altruism ,Choice Behavior ,050105 experimental psychology ,Developmental Neuroscience ,Social Perception ,Charities ,Donation ,Common knowledge ,Normative ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Psychology ,Attribution ,Social psychology ,General Psychology ,Reciprocity (cultural anthropology) ,media_common - Abstract
Why do people esteem anonymous charitable giving? We connect normative theories of charitability (captured in Maimonides' Ladder of Charity) with evolutionary theories of partner choice to test predictions on how attributions of charitability are affected by states of knowledge: whether the identity of the donor or of the beneficiary is revealed to the other. Consistent with the theories, in Experiments 1-2 participants judged a double-blind gift as more charitable than one to a revealed beneficiary, which in turn was judged as more charitable than one from a revealed donor. We also found one exception: Participants judged a donor who revealed only himself as slightly less, rather than more, charitable than one who revealed both identities. Experiment 3 explains the exception as a reaction to the donor's perceived sense of superiority and disinterest in a social relationship. Experiment 4 found that donors were judged as more charitable when the gift was shared knowledge (each aware of the other's identity, but unsure of the other's awareness) than when it was common knowledge (awareness of awareness). Experiment 5, which titrated anonymity against donation size, found that not even a hundredfold larger gift could compensate for the disapproval elicited by a donor revealing his identity. Experiment 6 showed that participants' judgments of charitability flip depending on whose perspective they take: Observers disapprove of donations that they would prefer as beneficiaries. Together, these experiments provide insight into why people care about how a donor gives, not just how much. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2018 APA, all rights reserved).
- Published
- 2018
26. Dissociation of English Past Tense Forms in Aphasia
- Author
-
Gregory Hickok and Steven Pinker
- Subjects
Dissociation (neuropsychology) ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Cognitive Psychology|Problem Solving ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Cognitive Psychology|Consciousness ,Past tense ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Cognitive Psychology|Creativity ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Linguistics|Psycholinguistics and Neurolinguistics ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Cognitive Psychology|Reasoning ,Aphasia ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Cognitive Psychology|Judgment and Decision Making ,medicine ,bepress|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Linguistics|Psycholinguistics and Neurolinguistics ,bepress|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Linguistics ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Cognitive Psychology|Biases, Framing, and Heuristics ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Cognitive Psychology|Attention ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Cognitive Psychology|Memory ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Cognitive Psychology|Concepts and Categories ,Linguistics ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Cognitive Psychology|Imagery ,bepress|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Psychology|Cognitive Psychology ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Cognitive Psychology|Language ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences ,bepress|Social and Behavioral Sciences ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Cognitive Psychology ,medicine.symptom ,Psychology ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Linguistics ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Cognitive Psychology|Learning - Abstract
Examines the ability of three individuals with aphasia to read aloud regular and irregular past tense verb forms. All three were more impaired in reading regular than irregular forms. These findings support a dual mechanism model of past tense formation in production.
- Published
- 2018
27. Enlightenment Now : The Case for Reason, Science, Humanism, and Progress
- Author
-
Steven Pinker and Steven Pinker
- Subjects
- Quality of life, Humanism, Social change, Progress, Civilization, Modern--21st century
- Abstract
INSTANT NEW YORK TIMES BESTSELLER A NEW YORK TIMES NOTABLE BOOK OF 2018ONE OF THE ECONOMIST'S BOOKS OF THE YEAR'My new favorite book of all time.'--Bill Gates If you think the world is coming to an end, think again: people are living longer, healthier, freer, and happier lives, and while our problems are formidable, the solutions lie in the Enlightenment ideal of using reason and science. By the author of the new book, Rationality. Is the world really falling apart? Is the ideal of progress obsolete? In this elegant assessment of the human condition in the third millennium, cognitive scientist and public intellectual Steven Pinker urges us to step back from the gory headlines and prophecies of doom, which play to our psychological biases. Instead, follow the data: In seventy-five jaw-dropping graphs, Pinker shows that life, health, prosperity, safety, peace, knowledge, and happiness are on the rise, not just in the West, but worldwide. This progress is not the result of some cosmic force. It is a gift of the Enlightenment: the conviction that reason and science can enhance human flourishing.Far from being a naïve hope, the Enlightenment, we now know, has worked. But more than ever, it needs a vigorous defense. The Enlightenment project swims against currents of human nature--tribalism, authoritarianism, demonization, magical thinking--which demagogues are all too willing to exploit. Many commentators, committed to political, religious, or romantic ideologies, fight a rearguard action against it. The result is a corrosive fatalism and a willingness to wreck the precious institutions of liberal democracy and global cooperation. With intellectual depth and literary flair, Enlightenment Now makes the case for reason, science, and humanism: the ideals we need to confront our problems and continue our progress.
- Published
- 2018
28. The False Allure of Group Selection
- Author
-
Steven Pinker
- Subjects
Group selection ,Natural selection ,Cultural group selection ,Inclusive fitness ,Altruism (biology) ,Psychology ,Sociocultural evolution ,Evolutionary psychology ,Social psychology ,Selection (genetic algorithm) - Abstract
Does the human mind include psychological adaptations that were selected because they fostered the competitive advantage of ancestral groups, even if they harmed the individuals that bore those adaptations? This notion of group selection is the default folk theory of evolution among most nonbiologists, and even among many biologists until the 1960s, when the theory was shown to be at best improbable and at worst incoherent. Nonetheless group selection refuses to die, and has recently been endorsed by a few prominent biologists and anthropologists. I show that the intuitive appeal of group selection is based on multiple confusions. First, group psychology—the phenomenon in which people identify and make sacrifices for their group—should not be equated with group selection. Second, the size, power, influence, or geographic spread of a group over the course of history (the loose analogue of fitness in cultural evolution) is not analogous to an increase in the number of copies of a replicator in biological evolution. Finally, the appeal of group selection rests on an unexamined and highly implausible assumption: that the groups most victorious in violent combat were those that practiced the greatest degree of kindness and generosity within their own societies. I conclude that the theory of natural selection should be invoked in its rigorous sense of the differential representation of replicators across generations, and that “group selection” is a pernicious concept in evolutionary psychology, guaranteed to confuse. Keywords: group selection; altruism; cultural evolution; individual selection; inclusive fitness
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
29. Response to the Book Review Symposium: Steven Pinker, The Better Angels of Our Nature
- Author
-
Steven Pinker
- Subjects
Libertarianism ,Sociology and Political Science ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Autocracy ,False accusation ,Surprise ,Critical theory ,Aesthetics ,Humanity ,Middle Ages ,Ideology ,Sociology ,media_common ,Law and economics - Abstract
They say that ideology is like breath: you never smell your own.1 And so I was not surprised to see my book The Better Angels of Our Nature: A History of Violence and Humanity described as ‘ideological’ by reviewers who strike me as black pots in glass houses casting the first stone. By the same token, it is not easy for an author to defend himself against such an accusation: ‘I am not ideological’ is bound to sound as convincing as ‘I am not a crook’ and ‘I did not have sex with that woman.’ But I will take my chances. The arguments in The Better Angels of Our Nature are in fact not ideological. They are empirical, though the facts on which those arguments are based are bound to gore some oxen of the hard left, critical theory, and various forms of post-X-ism (together with certain livestock of the hard right, libertarianism, and anarchism). As I note in the preface, and as the paper and internet trails of my writing confirm, Better Angels was inspired by my coming across diverse datasets showing historical declines in violence. The existence of these declines (such as homicide since the Middle Ages, corporal and capital punishment since the 18th century, great-power wars since 1945, and autocracies since the 1980s) are well accepted by the scholarly communities who study them, but they surprised me at the time, continue to surprise most readers, and are adamantly denied by those who are unfamiliar with the relevant literatures.
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
30. The Moral Instinct
- Author
-
Steven Pinker
- Subjects
Instinct ,Unconscious mind ,Harm ,Modernity ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Moral intuitions ,Moral reasoning ,Rationalization (economics) ,Psychology ,Game theory ,media_common ,Epistemology - Abstract
Moral intuitions are being drawn out of people in the lab, on websites, and in brain scanners, and are being explained with tools from game theory, neuroscience, and evolutionary biology. The human moral sense turns out to be an organ of considerable complexity, with quirks that reflect its evolutionary history and neurobiological foundations. Many of these moralizations, such as the assault on smoking, may be understood as practical tactics to reduce some recently identified harm. People don't engage in moral reasoning, Haidt argues, but moral rationalization: they begin with the conclusion coughed up by an unconscious emotion, and then work backward to a plausible justification. The institutions of modernity often question and experiment with the way activities are assigned to moral spheres. Market economies tend to put everything up for sale. The science of the moral sense alerts us to ways in which our psychological makeup can get in the way of our arriving at the most defensible moral conclusions.
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
31. Kill or die: Moral judgment alters linguistic coding of causality
- Author
-
Jason Nemirow, Maxim Massenkoff, Peter DeScioli, Steven Pinker, and Julian De Freitas
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Linguistics and Language ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Verb ,Trolley problem ,Causative ,Morals ,Choice Behavior ,050105 experimental psychology ,Language and Linguistics ,Judgment ,Value judgment ,050602 political science & public administration ,Intransitive verb ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Psychological Tests ,Psycholinguistics ,05 social sciences ,0506 political science ,Harm ,Action (philosophy) ,Female ,Psychology ,Social psychology ,Mirroring - Abstract
What is the relationship between the language people use to describe an event and their moral judgments? We test the hypothesis that moral judgment and causative verbs rely on the same underlying mental model of people's actions. Experiment 1a finds that participants choose different verbs to describe the major variants of a moral dilemma, the trolley problem, mirroring differences in their wrongness judgments: they described direct harm with a single causative verb (Adam killed the man), and indirect harm with an intransitive verb in a periphrastic construction (Adam caused the man to die). Experiments 1b and 2 separate physical causality from moral valuation by varying whether the victim is a person or animal and whether the harmful action rescues people or inanimate objects. The results show that people's moral judgments lead them to portray a causal event as either more or less direct and intended, which in turn shapes their verb choices. Experiment 3 finds the same basic asymmetry in verb usage in a production task in which participants freely described what happened. (PsycINFO Database Record
- Published
- 2017
32. Malgré le déclinisme ambiant, l’humanité se porte mieux qu’avant
- Author
-
Steven Pinker and Guillaume Jacquemont
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
33. The biological basis of language: insight from developmental grammatical impairments
- Author
-
Steven Pinker and Heather K. J. van der Lely
- Subjects
Cognitive science ,Psycholinguistics ,Syntax (programming languages) ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Phonology ,Specific language impairment ,medicine.disease ,Lateralization of brain function ,Developmental disorder ,Child Development ,Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology ,Phonetics ,Neurolinguistics ,Developmental linguistics ,medicine ,Humans ,Language Development Disorders ,Child ,Psychology ,Child Language ,Language ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Specific language impairment (SLI), a genetic developmental disorder, offers insights into the neurobiological and computational organization of language. A subtype, Grammatical-SLI (G-SLI), involves greater impairments in ‘extended’ grammatical representations, which are nonlocal, hierarchical, abstract, and composed, than in ‘basic’ ones, which are local, linear, semantic, and holistic. This distinction is seen in syntax, morphology, and phonology, and may be tied to abnormalities in the left hemisphere and basal ganglia, consistent with new models of the neurobiology of language which distinguish dorsal and ventral processing streams. Delineating neurolinguistic phenotypes promises a better understanding of the effects of genes on the brain circuitry underlying normal and impaired language abilities. Developmental disorders as a window into the biology of language Given the lack of animal models for language, and the inability to use invasive procedures with humans except out of medical necessity, our knowledge of the neurobiology of language has long depended upon natural experiments. During the 19th and 20th centuries, studies of patients with acquired brain lesions provided key insights [1–3]. Understanding of language in the 21st century promises to be enriched by data from developmental disorders. SLI, a family of language impairments in otherwise normal children, is highly heritable and has been linked so far to four genes. These discoveries provide a new route to understanding the complex pathways from genes and environment to the neural systems underpinning language. This understanding depends, however, on breaking down the coarse categories of ‘language’ and ‘language impairment’ and examining the way that specific components of language are affected in specific disorders, and how they correlate with brain function and structure. That is, rather than searching for a direct link from genotype to behavior, we suggest linking genetic variants
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
34. « Oui, les Lumières triomphent ! »
- Author
-
Jean-François Marmion and Steven Pinker
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
35. Corrigendum to 'common knowledge, coordination, and the logic of self-conscious emotions' [Evolution and Human Behavior volume 39, issue 2, march 2018, pages 179–190]
- Author
-
Kyle A. Thomas, Peter DeScioli, and Steven Pinker
- Subjects
Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Ecology, Evolution, Behavior and Systematics - Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
36. Do Humankind's Best Days Lie Ahead?
- Author
-
Steven Pinker, Matt Ridley, Alain de Botton, Malcolm Gladwell, Steven Pinker, Matt Ridley, Alain de Botton, and Malcolm Gladwell
- Subjects
- Civilization, Progress, Forecasting, Social prediction
- Abstract
‘It's just a brute fact that we don't throw virgins into volcanoes any more. We don't execute people for shoplifting a cabbage. And we used to.'–Steven Pinker ‘The idea that because things have gotten better in the past they will continue to do so in the future is a fallacy I would have thought confined to the lower reaches of Wall Street.'–Malcolm Gladwell In a world driven by technology and globalization, is humanity approaching a Golden Age or is the notion of progress a Western delusion? Four of the world's most renowned thinkers take on one of the biggest debates of the modern era…
- Published
- 2016
37. The untenability of faitheism
- Author
-
Steven Pinker
- Subjects
Organized religion ,Agricultural and Biological Sciences(all) ,Biochemistry, Genetics and Molecular Biology(all) ,Religious belief ,Religious studies ,Biology ,General Agricultural and Biological Sciences ,New Atheism ,General Biochemistry, Genetics and Molecular Biology - Abstract
Between 2005 and 2007, a quartet of bestsellers by Sam Harris, Richard Dawkins, Dan Dennett, and Christopher Hitchens launched the New Atheism. Emboldened by the growing success of science in explaining the world (including our own minds), inspired by new research on the sources of religious belief, and galvanized by the baleful influence of religion in world affairs (particularly 9/11 and its aftermath), these Four Horsemen of the New Atheism — as they came to be called — pressed the case that God does not exist and that many aspects of organized religion are pernicious.
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
38. The psychology of coordination and common knowledge
- Author
-
Kyle A. Thomas, Omar Sultan Haque, Peter DeScioli, and Steven Pinker
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Sociology and Political Science ,Social Psychology ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Theory of Mind ,Poison control ,Performative utterance ,Interpersonal relationship ,Risk-Taking ,Reward ,Stag hunt ,Humans ,Interpersonal Relations ,Emotional expression ,Cooperative Behavior ,media_common ,Knowledge level ,Hypocrisy ,Cognition ,Middle Aged ,Altruism ,Knowledge ,Female ,Psychology ,Social psychology - Abstract
Research on human cooperation has concentrated on the puzzle of altruism, in which 1 actor incurs a cost to benefit another, and the psychology of reciprocity, which evolved to solve this problem. We examine the complementary puzzle of mutualism, in which actors can benefit each other simultaneously, and the psychology of coordination, which ensures such benefits. Coordination is facilitated by common knowledge: the recursive belief state in which A knows X, B knows X, A knows that B knows X, B knows that A knows X, ad infinitum. We test whether people are sensitive to common knowledge when deciding whether to engage in risky coordination. Participants decided between working alone for a certain profit and working together for a potentially higher profit that they would receive only if their partner made the same choice. Results showed that more participants attempted risky coordination when they and their prospective partner had common knowledge of the payoffs (broadcast over a loudspeaker) than when they had only shared knowledge (conveyed to both by a messenger) or private knowledge (revealed to each partner separately). These results support the hypothesis that people represent common knowledge as a distinct cognitive category that licenses them to coordinate with others for mutual gain. We discuss how this hypothesis can provide a unified explanation for diverse phenomena in human social life, including recursive mentalizing, performative speech acts, public protests, hypocrisy, and self-conscious emotional expressions.
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
39. The Forum: The Decline of War
- Author
-
Nils Petter Gleditsch, William R. Thompson, Jack S. Levy, Bradley A. Thayer, and Steven Pinker
- Subjects
media_common.quotation_subject ,Geography, Planning and Development ,Empathy ,Romance ,Nature versus nurture ,Competition (economics) ,State (polity) ,Phenomenon ,Political economy ,Law ,Political Science and International Relations ,Sociology ,China ,media_common - Abstract
The debate on the waning of war has recently moved into higher gear. This forum contributes to that debate. Steven Pinker observes that a decline in war does not require a romantic theory of human nature. In fact, it is compatible with a hardheaded view of human violent inclinations, firmly rooted in evolutionary biology. Homo sapiens evolved with violent tendencies, but they are triggered by particular circumstances rather than a hydraulic urge that must periodically be discharged. And, although our species evolved with motives that can erupt in violence, it also evolved motives that can inhibit violence, including self-control, empathy, a sense of fairness, and open-ended cognitive mechanisms that can devise technologies for reducing violence. Bradley Thayer argues that the decline of war thesis is flawed because the positive forces identified by these authors do not rule outside of the West or even fully inside of it. Their analysis also neglects the systemic causes of conflict and its insights for increasingly intense security competition between China and the United States. Jack Levy and William Thompson question some of the theoretical arguments advanced to explain the historical pattern of declining violence. They argue that cultural and ideational explanations for the decline in interstate war underestimate the extent to which those factors are endogenous to material and institutional variables. Arguments about the pacifying effects of the rise of the state and of commerce fail to recognize that in some historical contexts, those factors have contributed to the escalation of warfare. The introduction to the symposium outlines briefly some of the major issues: nature versus nurture, the reliability of the data, how broadly violence should be defined, whether there is more agreement on the phenomenon than on its causes, and finally whether the future will be like the past.
- Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
40. The Sense of Style : The Thinking Person's Guide to Writing in the 21st Century
- Author
-
Steven Pinker and Steven Pinker
- Subjects
- English language--Grammar, English language--Writing, English language--Style, Writing
- Abstract
“Charming and erudite,'from the author of Rationality and Enlightenment Now,'The wit and insight and clarity he brings... is what makes this book such a gem.” —Time.com Why is so much writing so bad, and how can we make it better? Is the English language being corrupted by texting and social media? Do the kids today even care about good writing—and why should we care? From the author of The Better Angels of Our Nature and Enlightenment Now.In this entertaining and eminently practical book, the cognitive scientist, dictionary consultant, and New York Times–bestselling author Steven Pinker rethinks the usage guide for the twenty-first century. Using examples of great and gruesome modern prose while avoiding the scolding tone and Spartan tastes of the classic manuals, he shows how the art of writing can be a form of pleasurable mastery and a fascinating intellectual topic in its own right. The Sense of Style is for writers of all kinds, and for readers who are interested in letters and literature and are curious about the ways in which the sciences of mind can illuminate how language works at its best.
- Published
- 2015
41. Lexical semantics and irregular inflection
- Author
-
Steven Pinker and Yi Ting Huang
- Subjects
Linguistics and Language ,Lexical semantics ,Computer science ,business.industry ,Principle of compositionality ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Verb ,computer.software_genre ,Lexicon ,Article ,Language and Linguistics ,Linguistics ,Education ,Semantic similarity ,Noun ,Inflection ,Artificial intelligence ,business ,computer ,Natural language processing ,Regular and irregular verbs - Abstract
Whether a word has an irregular inflection does not depend on its sound alone: compare lie-lay (recline) and lie-lied (prevaricate). Theories of morphology, particularly connectionist and symbolic models, disagree on which nonphonological factors are responsible. We test four possibilities: (1) Lexical effects, in which two lemmas differ in whether they specify an irregular form; (2) Semantic effects, in which the semantic features of a word become associated with regular or irregular forms; (3) Morphological structure effects, in which a word with a headless structure (e.g., a verb derived from a noun) blocks access to a stored irregular form; (4) Compositionality effects, in which the stored combination of an irregular word's meaning (e.g., the verb's inherent aspect) with the meaning of the inflection (e.g., pastness) doesn't readily transfer to new senses with different combinations of such meanings. In four experiments, speakers were presented with existing and novel verbs and asked to rate their past-tense forms, semantic similarities, grammatical structure, and aspectual similarities. We found (1) an interaction between semantic and phonological similarity, coinciding with reported strategies of analogizing to known verbs and implicating lexical effects; (2) weak and inconsistent effects of semantic similarity; (3) robust effects of morphological structure, and (4) robust effects of aspectual compositionality. Results are consistent with theories of language that invoke lexical entries and morphological structure, and which differentiate the mode of storage of regular and irregular verbs. They also suggest how psycholinguistic processes have shaped vocabulary structure over history.
- Published
- 2010
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
42. The cognitive niche: Coevolution of intelligence, sociality, and language
- Author
-
Steven Pinker
- Subjects
Cognitive science ,Multidisciplinary ,Natural selection ,Intelligent design ,Human intelligence ,Poison control ,Cognition ,Biology ,Adaptation (computer science) ,Coevolution ,Sociality - Abstract
Although Darwin insisted that human intelligence could be fully explained by the theory of evolution, the codiscoverer of natural selection, Alfred Russel Wallace, claimed that abstract intelligence was of no use to ancestral humans and could only be explained by intelligent design. Wallace's apparent paradox can be dissolved with two hypotheses about human cognition. One is that intelligence is an adaptation to a knowledge-using, socially interdependent lifestyle, the “cognitive niche.” This embraces the ability to overcome the evolutionary fixed defenses of plants and animals by applications of reasoning, including weapons, traps, coordinated driving of game, and detoxification of plants. Such reasoning exploits intuitive theories about different aspects of the world, such as objects, forces, paths, places, states, substances, and other people's beliefs and desires. The theory explains many zoologically unusual traits inHomo sapiens, including our complex toolkit, wide range of habitats and diets, extended childhoods and long lives, hypersociality, complex mating, division into cultures, and language (which multiplies the benefit of knowledge because know-how is useful not only for its practical benefits but as a trade good with others, enhancing the evolution of cooperation). The second hypothesis is that humans possess an ability ofmetaphorical abstraction, which allows them to coopt faculties that originally evolved for physical problem-solving and social coordination, apply them to abstract subject matter, and combine them productively. These abilities can help explain the emergence of abstract cognition without supernatural or exotic evolutionary forces and are in principle testable by analyses of statistical signs of selection in the human genome.
- Published
- 2010
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
43. Compound formation is constrained by morphology
- Author
-
Iris Berent and Steven Pinker
- Subjects
Linguistics and Language ,Connectionism ,Semantics (computer science) ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,Structure (category theory) ,Haskell ,Phonology ,computer ,Language and Linguistics ,Linguistics ,computer.programming_language ,Mathematics - Abstract
Why do compounds containing regular plurals, such as rats-infested, sound so much worse than corresponding compounds containing irregular plurals, such as mice-infested? Berent and Pinker (2007) reported five experiments showing that this theoretically important effect hinges on the morphological structure of the plurals, not their phonological properties, as had been claimed by Haskell, MacDonald, and Seidenberg (2003). In this note we reply to a critique by these authors. We show that the connectionist model they invoke to explain the data has nothing to do with compounding but exploits fortuitous properties of adjectives, and that our experimental results disconfirm explicit predictions the authors had made. We also present new analyses which answer the authors’ methodological objections. We conclude that the interaction of compounding with regularity is a robust effect, unconfounded with phonology or semantics.
- Published
- 2008
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
44. STEVEN PINKER ON VIOLENCE AND HUMAN NATURE
- Author
-
Steven Pinker
- Subjects
Psychoanalysis ,Sociology - Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
45. The dislike of regular plurals in compounds
- Author
-
Iris Berent and Steven Pinker
- Subjects
Constraint (information theory) ,Linguistics and Language ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,Noun ,Inflection ,Phonology ,Language and Linguistics ,Linguistics ,Mathematics ,Plural - Abstract
English speakers disfavor compounds containing regular plurals compared to irregular ones. Haskell, MacDonald and Seidenberg (2003) attribute this phenomenon to the rarity of compounds containing words with the phonological properties of regular plurals. Five experiments test this proposal. Experiment 1 demonstrated that novel regular plurals (e.g., loonks-eater) are disliked in compounds compared to irregular plurals with illicit (hence less frequent) phonological patterns (e.g., leevk-eater, plural of loovk). Experiments 2–3 found that people show no dispreference for compounds containing nouns that merely sound like regular plurals (e.g., hose-installer vs. pipe-installer). Experiments 4–5 showed a robust effect of morphological regularity when phonological familiarity was controlled: Compounds containing regular plural nonwords (e.g., gleeks-hunter, plural of gleek) were disfavored relative to irregular, phonologically-identical, plurals (e.g., breex-container, plural of broox). The dispreference for regular plurals inside compounds thus hinges on the morphological distinction between irregular and regular forms and it is irreducible to phonological familiarity.
- Published
- 2007
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
46. Does language frame politics?
- Author
-
George Lakoff and Steven Pinker
- Subjects
Politics ,Public Administration ,Frame (networking) ,Sociology ,Management, Monitoring, Policy and Law ,Linguistics - Published
- 2007
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
47. Toward a Consilient Study of Literature
- Author
-
Steven Pinker
- Subjects
Philosophy ,Literature and Literary Theory ,Psychology - Published
- 2007
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
48. The Man Who Made Language a Window into Human Nature
- Author
-
Steven Pinker
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
49. Por que o debate ‘genética e cultura’ não desapareceráWhy nature & nurture won’t go away
- Author
-
Steven Pinker
- Subjects
Nature-Nurture ,Educación Familiar ,Genética-Cultura ,Parenting ,Holistic Interactionism ,Environment ,Meio Ambiente ,lcsh:Social Sciences ,lcsh:H ,Medio Ambiente ,Interacionismo Holístico ,lcsh:H1-99 ,Educação dos Pais ,lcsh:Social sciences (General) - Abstract
PortuguesO debate genética-cultura tem sido uma preocupação para a Psicologia e para as Ciências Sociais há séculos. Muitos escritores têm expressado uma esperança que se chegue a um meio termo que faça o debate desaparecer. Neste meio termo, todo comportamento vem de uma interação intrincada entre a hereditariedade e o meio ambiente, e seria um erro tentar separá-los. Eu contesto este ponto de vista, o qual denominei interacionismo holístico, por várias razões, entre as quais o fato de que é simplesmente falso que todos os aspectos da função cerebral envolvem uma mistura de hereditariedade e meio ambiente, e esse interacionismo holístico obscurece nossa compreensão de como a mente trabalha. Como ilustração, eu discuto o caso dos efeitos da educação dada pelos pais, no qual o interacionismo holístico tem conduzido a conclusões falsas e enganosas.EnglishThe nature-nurture debate has preoccupied psychology and the social sciences for centuries. Many writers have expressed a hope for a compromise that it will make the debate disappear.. In this view, all behavior comes from an inextricable interaction between heredity and environment, and it is a mistake to try to tease them apart. reasons, among them that it is simply false that all aspects of brain function involve a mixture of heredity and environment, and that holistic interactionism obscures our for several understanding of how the mind works. As an illustration, I discuss the case of the effects of parenting, where holistic interactionism has led to false and misleading conclusions.
- Published
- 2006
50. Abstract Grammatical Processing of Nouns and Verbs in Broca's Area: Evidence from FMRI
- Author
-
Ned T. Sahin, Eric Halgren, and Steven Pinker
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Speech production ,Adolescent ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,Prefrontal Cortex ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Functional Laterality ,Neural Pathways ,Inflection ,medicine ,Humans ,Speech ,Broca's area ,Language ,Brain Mapping ,Psycholinguistics ,Supplementary motor area ,medicine.diagnostic_test ,Working memory ,Motor Cortex ,Magnetic Resonance Imaging ,Linguistics ,Frontal Lobe ,Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology ,medicine.anatomical_structure ,Reading ,Female ,Cues ,Psychology ,Functional magnetic resonance imaging ,Insula ,Brodmann area - Abstract
The role of Broca's area in grammatical computation is unclear, because syntactic processing is often confounded with working memory, articulation, or semantic selection. Morphological processing potentially circumvents these problems. Using event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), we had 18 subjects silently inflect words or read them verbatim. Subtracting the activity pattern for reading from that for inflection, which indexes processes involved in inflection (holding constant lexical processing and articulatory planning) highlighted left Brodmann area (BA) 44/45 (Broca's area), BA 47, anterior insula, and medial supplementary motor area. Subtracting activity during zero inflection (the hawk; they walk) from that during overt inflection (the hawks; they walked), which highlights manipulation of phonological content, implicated subsets of the regions engaged by inflection as a whole. Subtracting activity during verbatim reading from activity during zero inflection (which highlights the manipulation of inflectional features) implicated distinct regions of BA 44, 47, and a premotor region (thereby tying these regions to grammatical features), but failed to implicate the insula or BA 45 (thereby tying these to articulation). These patterns were largely similar in nouns and verbs and in regular and irregular forms, suggesting these regions implement inflectional features cutting across word classes. Greater activity was observed for irregular than regular verbs in the anterior cingulate and supplementary motor area (SMA), possibly reflecting the blocking of regular or competing irregular candidates. The results confirm a role for Broca's area in abstract grammatical processing, and are interpreted in terms of a network of regions in left prefrontal cortex (PFC) that are recruited for processing abstract morphosyntactic features and overt morphophonological content.
- Published
- 2006
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.