132 results on '"Serial Processing"'
Search Results
2. Rapid Serial Visual Presentation of transposed-word sequences in the grammatical decision task: an examination of the roles of temporal and spatial cues to word order.
- Author
-
Spinelli, Giacomo, Yang, Huilan, and Lupker, Stephen J.
- Subjects
- *
COMPARATIVE grammar , *READING , *PROMPTS (Psychology) , *TASK performance , *DATA analysis , *RESEARCH funding , *PHONOLOGICAL awareness , *DESCRIPTIVE statistics , *LATENT structure analysis , *STATISTICS , *COLLEGE students , *VISUAL perception , *DATA analysis software - Abstract
Transposing two words in a sentence (e.g. "cat" and "was" in "the white cat was big") creates a sequence that is harder to classify as ungrammatical than control sequences (e.g. "the white was cat slowly"), suggesting that word position coding is noisy and can be affected by syntactic expectations. In the present research, this transposed-word effect was examined more closely using Rapid Serial Visual Presentation (RSVP) formats which provided either clear temporal cues to word order but no spatial cues, or both types of cues. Compared to when all words were presented simultaneously, the two RSVP formats reduced the transposed-word effect to the same degree while having no parallel impact on another ungrammatical comparison condition involving no transposition. These results are discussed in the context of serial and parallel models of reading as well as models that propose a later processing stage for the locus of the transposed-word effect. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
3. Serial and strategic memory processes in younger and older adults.
- Author
-
Murphy, Dillon H. and Castel, Alan D.
- Subjects
- *
OLDER people , *AGE groups , *MEMORY - Abstract
We investigated age-related differences in serial and strategic processing during the encoding and retrieval of high-value words. Younger and older adults were presented with word triads positioned left, center, and right, with one word being more valuable than the others. In Experiment 1, younger adults more effectively recalled the middle, high-value word, demonstrating enhanced strategic memory. Younger adults were more likely to initiate recall with a high-value word whereas older adults were equally likely to initiate recall with a left and high-value word. Additionally, older adults were more likely to recall words in their presented order while younger adults strategically recalled successive high-value words. However, both age groups demonstrated strategic processing in Experiments 2 and 3, even without prior knowledge of the high-value word’s location. Thus, serial and strategic processing may differ based on age and task demands, but strategic processing is preserved in older adults in certain contexts. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
4. The transposed-word effect provides no unequivocal evidence for parallel processing.
- Author
-
Milledge, Sara V., Bhatia, Neya, Mensah-Mcleod, Loren, Raghvani, Pallvi, A. McGowan, Victoria, Elsherif, Mahmoud M., Cutter, Michael G., Wang, Jingxin, Liu, Zhiwei, and Paterson, Kevin B.
- Subjects
- *
PARALLEL processing , *WORD recognition , *WORD order (Grammar) , *REVENUE accounting , *ENGLISH language , *LEXICAL access - Abstract
Studies using a grammaticality decision task have revealed surprising flexibility in the processing of word order during sentence reading in both alphabetic and non-alphabetic scripts. Participants in these studies typically exhibit a transposed-word effect, in which they make more errors and slower correct responses for stimuli that contain a word transposition and are derived from grammatical as compared to ungrammatical base sentences. Some researchers have used this finding to argue that words are encoded in parallel during reading, such that multiple words can be processed simultaneously and might be recognised out of order. This contrasts with an alternative account of the reading process, which argues that words must be encoded serially, one at a time. We examined, in English, whether the transposed-word effect provides evidence for a parallel-processing account, employing the same grammaticality decision task used in previous research and display procedures that either allowed for parallel word encoding or permitted only the serial encoding of words. Our results replicate and extend recent findings by showing that relative word order can be processed flexibly even when parallel processing is not possible (i.e., within displays requiring serial word encoding). Accordingly, while the present findings provide further evidence for flexibility in the processing of relative word order during reading, they add to converging evidence that the transposed-word effect does not provide unequivocal evidence for a parallel-processing account of reading. We consider how the present findings may be accounted for by both serial and parallel accounts of word recognition in reading. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
5. Serial Processing
- Author
-
Yanhong, Wu and Kan, Zhang, editor
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
6. A Dynamic Dual Process Model for Binary Choices: Serial Versus Parallel Architecture
- Author
-
Diederich, Adele
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
7. Dual-Task Performance with Simple Tasks
- Author
-
Fischer, Rico, Janczyk, Markus, Kiesel, Andrea, editor, Johannsen, Leif, editor, Koch, Iring, editor, and Müller, Hermann, editor
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
8. Visual Search
- Author
-
Wolfe, Jeremy
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
9. Analytic visual word recognition among Chinese L2 learners.
- Author
-
Jiang, Nan and Feng, Lijuan
- Subjects
WORD recognition ,LEXICAL access ,PARALLEL processing ,SECOND language acquisition ,REGRESSION analysis - Abstract
The process of word recognition can be analytic (or serial) or holistic (or parallel). They differ in the size of the processing units (lexical vs. sublexical) or in whether sublexical units are processed sequentially or simultaneously. First language (L1) reading development has been found to involve a transition from serial processing to parallel processing, as shown in a decreasing length or stroke number effect among readers of increasing proficiencies. The present study was intended to explore the use of analytic versus holistic character recognition strategies in the second language (L2) learners, an issue that has not received much attention. Chinese native speakers and learners of Chinese as a second language (CSL) were tested in a variant of the lexical decision task on Chinese characters of different numbers of strokes. Regression analyses with reaction times as an outcome variable and stroke number as a predictor variable showed that CSL speakers produced a stroke number effect where native speakers did not. The results were interpreted as evidence for the employment of an analytic processing strategy by L2 speakers. The findings raised several important issues for future research and for pedagogical considerations. The Challenge: Little is known about whether adult L2 Chinese learners process Chinese characters holistically as developed Chinese native readers do, or analytically as beginning readers do. The present study explored this issue in a carefully designed experimental setting and obtained evidence for the analytical processing of Chinese characters by L2 learners. Pedagogical implications Table A1 for this finding were discussed. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
10. Visual Search for Circumscribed Interests in Autism Is Similar to That of Neurotypical Individuals
- Author
-
Benjamin M. Silver, Mary M. Conte, Jonathan D. Victor, and Rebecca M. Jones
- Subjects
autism spectrum disorder ,visual processing ,serial processing ,parallel processing ,circumscribed interests ,visual search ,Psychology ,BF1-990 - Abstract
Intense interests are a core symptom of autism spectrum disorders (ASD) and can be all-encompassing for affected individuals. This observation raises the hypothesis that intense interests in ASD are related to pervasive changes in visual processing for objects within that category, including visual search. We assayed visual processing with two novel tasks, targeting category search and exemplar search. For each task, three kinds of stimuli were used: faces, houses, and images personalized to each participant’s interest. 25 children and adults with ASD were compared to 25 neurotypical (NT) children and adults. We found no differences in either visual search task between ASD and NT controls for interests. Thus, pervasive alterations in perception are not likely to account for ASD behavioral symptoms.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
11. Searching for Strangely Shaped Cookies – Is Taking a Bite Out of a Cookie Similar to Occluding Part of It?
- Author
-
Brenner, Eli, Hurtado, Sergio Sánchez, Arias, Elena Alvarez, Smeets, Jeroen B. J., and Fleming, Roland W.
- Subjects
- *
FORM perception , *VISUAL perception , *PATTERN perception , *VISUAL memory , *SENSORY perception - Abstract
Does recognizing the transformations that gave rise to an object's retinal image contribute to early object recognition? It might, because finding a partially occluded object among similar objects that are not occluded is more difficult than finding an object that has the same retinal image shape without evident occlusion. If this is because the occlusion is recognized as such, we might see something similar for other transformations. We confirmed that it is difficult to find a cookie with a section missing when this was the result of occlusion. It is not more difficult to find a cookie from which a piece has been bitten off than to find one that was baked in a similar shape. On the contrary, the bite marks help detect the bitten cookie. Thus, biting off a part of a cookie has very different effects on visual search than occluding part of it. These findings do not support the idea that observers rapidly and automatically compensate for the ways in which objects' shapes are transformed to give rise to the objects' retinal images. They are easy to explain in terms of detecting characteristic features in the retinal image that such transformations may hide or create. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
12. Visual Search for Circumscribed Interests in Autism Is Similar to That of Neurotypical Individuals.
- Author
-
Silver, Benjamin M., Conte, Mary M., Victor, Jonathan D., and Jones, Rebecca M.
- Subjects
VISUAL perception ,AUTISM spectrum disorders ,AUTISM ,SYMPTOMS - Abstract
Intense interests are a core symptom of autism spectrum disorders (ASD) and can be all-encompassing for affected individuals. This observation raises the hypothesis that intense interests in ASD are related to pervasive changes in visual processing for objects within that category, including visual search. We assayed visual processing with two novel tasks, targeting category search and exemplar search. For each task, three kinds of stimuli were used: faces, houses, and images personalized to each participant's interest. 25 children and adults with ASD were compared to 25 neurotypical (NT) children and adults. We found no differences in either visual search task between ASD and NT controls for interests. Thus, pervasive alterations in perception are not likely to account for ASD behavioral symptoms. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
13. Serial Processing
- Author
-
Volkmar, Fred R., editor
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
14. 一种并行处理的联合频相估计.
- Author
-
杨茜 and 于中阳
- Abstract
Copyright of Journal of Data Acquisition & Processing / Shu Ju Cai Ji Yu Chu Li is the property of Editorial Department of Journal of Nanjing University of Aeronautics & Astronautics and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use. This abstract may be abridged. No warranty is given about the accuracy of the copy. Users should refer to the original published version of the material for the full abstract. (Copyright applies to all Abstracts.)
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
15. Parallel spatial channels converge at a bottleneck in anterior word-selective cortex.
- Author
-
White, Alex L., Palmer, John, Boynton, Geoffrey M., and Yeatman, Jason D.
- Subjects
- *
FUSIFORM gyrus , *WORD recognition , *BRAIN imaging , *FUNCTIONAL magnetic resonance imaging , *EYE movements - Abstract
In most environments, the visual system is confronted with many relevant objects simultaneously. That is especially true during reading. However, behavioral data demonstrate that a serial bottleneck prevents recognition of more than one word at a time. We used fMRI to investigate how parallel spatial channels of visual processing converge into a serial bottleneck for word recognition. Participants viewed pairs of words presented simultaneously. We found that retinotopic cortex processed the twowords in parallel spatial channels, one in each contralateral hemisphere. Responses were higher for attended than for ignored words but were not reduced when attention was divided. We then analyzed two word-selective regions along the occipitotemporal sulcus (OTS) of both hemispheres (subregions of the visual word form area, VWFA). Unlike retinotopic regions, each word-selective region responded to words on both sides of fixation. Nonetheless, a single region in the left hemisphere (posterior OTS) contained spatial channels for both hemifields that were independently modulated by selective attention. Thus, the left posterior VWFA supports parallel processing of multiple words. In contrast, activity in amore anteriorword-selective region in the left hemisphere (mid OTS) was consistent with a single channel, showing (i) limited spatial selectivity, (ii) no effect of spatial attention on mean response amplitudes, and (iii) sensitivity to lexical properties of only one attended word. Therefore, the visual system can process two words in parallel up to a late stage in the ventral stream. The transition to a single channel is consistent with the observed bottleneck in behavior. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
16. Finger Tracking Reveals the Covert Stages of Mental Arithmetic
- Author
-
Pedro Pinheiro-Chagas, Dror Dotan, Manuela Piazza, and Stanislas Dehaene
- Subjects
arithmetic ,finger tracking ,serial processing ,problem-size effect ,operational momentum effect ,Consciousness. Cognition ,BF309-499 ,Neurophysiology and neuropsychology ,QP351-495 - Abstract
We introduce a novel method capable of dissecting the succession of processing stages underlying mental arithmetic, thus revealing how two numbers are transformed into a third. We asked adults to point to the result of single-digit additions and subtractions on a number line, while their finger trajectory was constantly monitored. We found that the two operands are processed serially: the finger first points toward the larger operand, then slowly veers toward the correct result. This slow deviation unfolds proportionally to the size of the smaller operand, in both additions and subtractions. We also observed a transient operator effect: a plus sign attracted the finger to the right and a minus sign to the left and a transient activation of the absolute value of the subtrahend. These findings support a model whereby addition and subtraction are computed by a stepwise displacement on the mental number line, starting with the larger number and incrementally adding or subtracting the smaller number.
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
17. Integrated Scheduling Algorithm Based on Dynamic Essential Short Path
- Author
-
Xie, Zhiqiang, Wang, Peng, Gui, Zhongyan, Yang, Jing, Jin, David, editor, and Lin, Sally, editor
- Published
- 2012
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
18. Transposed word effect and parallel processing in correct spatial location
- Author
-
Paterson, Kevin, Cutter, Michael, McGowan, Victoria, and Elsherif, Mahmoud
- Subjects
word position coding ,parallel processing ,grammatical decision task ,Cognitive Psychology ,Linguistics ,Social and Behavioral Sciences ,FOS: Psychology ,Psycholinguistics and Neurolinguistics ,reading ,FOS: Languages and literature ,Psychology ,rapid serial visual presentation ,serial processing ,RSVP ,word transposition effect - Abstract
Evidence has supported the idea that readers lexically process words serially (one at a time), but evidence by Mirault et al. (2018) has demonstrated a transposed word effect. This effect is where readers have difficulty deciding that sentences such as “do love you me? “are ungrammatical, in which the transposition of two adjacent words (i.e., love you) would form a grammatical sentence, compared to sentences in which the transposition of two words does not result in grammaticality (e.g. the cat white was slowly). This has been taken as evidence for the parallel lexical processing of multiple words simultaneously. Evidence has also shown that the transposed word effect can be attributed to a combination of noisy bottom-up word position coding and top-down syntactic constraints. This effect can be interpreted within serial, as opposed to parallel, reading models. The present study will examine the transposed word effect by varying the duration for which each of the two transposed words will be presented. In the previous experiment, we observed the transposed word effects were much larger when all words were presented for 125ms than when they were all presented for 250ms. It has been recently argued by Huang and Staub (2021) that the integration of word n+1 may begin before the integration of word n is complete, which may allow the transposed word effect to occur. As a result, the findings we observed may be more likely to ensue, as there is a higher likelihood that word n would not complete when the integration of word n+1 began when all words were presented for 125ms than when they were presented for 250ms. In the previous experiment, we presented all the words with a central presentation. We observed the transposed word effects were much larger when all words were presented for 125ms than when they were all presented for 250ms. In this experiment, we manipulate the duration of all the words in the sentence are presented and presented the words in the correct spatial location. As a result, instead of the transposed pair being presented at 125ms, in one condition, all the words in the sentence are presented at 125ms. In another set of conditions, all the words in the sentence are presented at 250ms. For instance, You [125]| that [125]| read [125]| wrong [125]| again. Vs. You [250]| that [250]| read [250]| wrong [250]| again. We would expect that when all the words are presented at a short duration (125ms), then the integration of each word might not be complete before integration of the next word, leading to a higher likelihood of a transposition. However, if the each word is presented at a longer duration (250ms), then it is more likely that the integration of the first word will have completed before integration of the second word, leading to a lower likelihood of a transposition. Thus, we expected the transposed word effect to be larger when all the words are presented at 125ms than 250ms.
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
19. Transposed words and RSVP in correct spatial location
- Author
-
Paterson, Kevin, Cutter, Michael, Elsherif, Mahmoud, and McGowan, Victoria
- Subjects
word position coding ,parallel processing ,grammatical decision task ,Cognitive Psychology ,Linguistics ,Social and Behavioral Sciences ,FOS: Psychology ,Psycholinguistics and Neurolinguistics ,reading ,FOS: Languages and literature ,Psychology ,serial processing ,rapid serial visual presentation ,word transposition effect ,RSVP - Abstract
Evidence has supported the idea that readers lexically process words serially (one at a time), but evidence by Mirault et al. (2018) has demonstrated a transposed word effect. This effect is where readers have difficulty deciding that sentences such as “do love you me? “are ungrammatical, in which the transposition of two adjacent words (i.e., love you) would form a grammatical sentence, compared to sentences in which the transposition of two words does not result in grammaticality (e.g. the cat white was slowly). This has been taken as evidence for the parallel lexical processing of multiple words simultaneously. Evidence has also shown that the transposed word effect can be attributed to a combination of noisy bottom-up word position coding and top-down syntactic constraints. This effect can be interpreted within serial, as opposed to parallel, reading models. In a previous experiment, we presented the first (e.g. cat) or the second word (e.g. white) of the transposed word pair at 125 ms. We observed the transposed word effects were much larger when the words were presented for 125ms than when they were presented for 250ms. In a second experiment, we manipulated the duration of just one of the words in the transposed pair, so that either the first or second word in the transposed pair was presented at 125ms, and the remaining words were presented at 250ms. We found that the transposed word effect was larger when the first word in the pair was presented for 125ms than when the second word in the pair was presented for 125ms. The present study will examine the transposed word effect by varying the duration for which each of the two transposed words will be presented. In contrast to our previous experiment, words will be presented in the correct spatial position, as opposed to being presented centrally. It has been recently argued by Huang and Staub (2021) that the integration of word n+1 may begin before the integration of word n is complete, which may allow the transposed word effect to occur. As a result, the findings we observed may be more likely to ensue, as there is a higher likelihood that word n would not complete when the integration of word n+1 began when all words were presented for 125ms than when they were presented for 250ms. In this experiment, we manipulate the duration of the transposed pair. As a result, instead of the sentence being presented at 125ms, in one condition, the first word will be presented for 125ms while the second word will be presented for 250ms. In another set of conditions, the first word will be presented for 250ms while the second will be presented for 125ms. The remaining words will be presented for 250ms. For instance, You [250]| that [125]| read [250]| wrong [250]| again. Vs. You [250]| that [250]| read [125]| wrong [250]| again. We would expect that when the first word of the pair is presented at a short duration (125ms), then the integration of the first word might not be complete before integration of the second word occurs, leading to a higher likelihood of a transposition. However, if the first word of the pair is presented at a longer duration (250ms), then it is more likely that the integration of the first word will have completed before integration of the second word, leading to a lower likelihood of a transposition. Thus, we expected the transposed word effect to be larger when the first word is of the pair is presented at 125ms than the second word.
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
20. What are proteins teaching us on fundamental strategies for molecular nanotechnology?
- Author
-
Koehler Johann Michael
- Subjects
entropy management ,hierarchical construction ,limited mobility ,modularization ,molecular nanotechnology ,restriction of dimensions ,serial processing ,soft matter ,Technology ,Chemical technology ,TP1-1185 ,Physical and theoretical chemistry ,QD450-801 - Abstract
Besides the fundamental competition between the top-down and bottom-up approaches in nanotechnology, there are some basic aspects for organizing structures and functions at the molecular level. The recent challenges to the development of nanotechnology are marked by a group of general requirements: selection of suited building units, overcoming the restrictions of planar technology, shrinking of nanofabrication facilities, sustainable production and management of life cycles, organization of autonomy and communication at the nano-level, and the optimization of power consumption and energy management. Looking at the natural principles in the construction, synthesis, and function of proteins helps in understanding the principal differences between the currently applied technologies and the characteristics of biomolecular mechanisms in cells. This view allows formulating seven basic rules to meet the general requirements for future developments in molecular nanotechnology.
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
21. The transposed word effect is consistent with serial word recognition and varies with reading speed.
- Author
-
Hossain, Jannat and White, Alex L.
- Subjects
- *
WORD recognition , *ORTHOGRAPHY & spelling , *ERROR rates , *INDIVIDUAL differences , *SPEED , *VOCABULARY - Abstract
The scientific study of reading has long been animated by questions of parallel vs. serial processing. Do readers recognize words serially, adding each one sequentially to a representation of the sentence structure? One fascinating phenomenon to emerge from this research is the transposed word effect : when asked to judge whether sentences are grammatical, readers often fail to notice grammatical errors caused by transposing two words. This effect could be evidence that readers recognize multiple words in parallel. Here we provide converging evidence that the transposed word effect is also consistent with serial processing because it occurs robustly when the words in each sentence are presented serially. We further investigated how the effect relates to individual differences in reading speed, to gaze fixation patterns, and to differences in difficulty across sentences. In a pretest, we first measured the natural English reading rate of 37 participants, which varied widely. In a subsequent grammatical decision task, we presented grammatical and ungrammatical sentences in two modes: one with all words presented simultaneously, and the other with single words presented sequentially at each participant's natural rate. Unlike prior studies that used a fixed sequential presentation rate, we found that the magnitude of the transposed word effect was at least as strong in the sequential presentation mode as in the simultaneous mode, for both error rates and response times. Moreover, faster readers were more likely to miss transpositions of words presented sequentially. We argue that these data favor a "noisy channel" model of comprehension in which skilled readers rely on prior knowledge to rapidly infer the meaning of sentences, allowing for apparent errors in spatial or temporal order, even when the individual words are recognized one at a time. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
22. Crossmodal transposed word serial processing
- Author
-
Elsherif, Mahmoud
- Subjects
Cognition and Perception ,word position coding ,Cognitive Psychology ,audition ,rapid serial crossmodal presentation ,Linguistics ,serial crossmodal presentation ,Social and Behavioral Sciences ,RSCP ,FOS: Psychology ,Psycholinguistics and Neurolinguistics ,reading ,FOS: Languages and literature ,Psychology ,serial processing ,grammatical decision ,transposed word effect - Abstract
Evidence has supported the idea that readers lexically process words serially (one at a time), but evidence by Mirault et al. (2018) has demonstrated a transposed word effect. This effect is where readers have difficulty deciding that sentences such as “do love you me? “are ungrammatical, in which the transposition of two adjacent words (i.e., love you) would form a grammatical sentence, compared to sentences in which the transposition of two words does not result in grammaticality (e.g. the cat white was slowly). This has been taken as evidence for the parallel lexical processing of multiple words simultaneously. Evidence has also shown that the transposed word effect can be attributed to a combination of noisy bottom-up word position coding and top-down syntactic constraints. This effect can be interpreted within serial, as opposed to parallel, reading models. The present study will examine the transposed word effect by varying modality input and type of processing for which each of the two transposed words will be presented. However, we aim to replicate the previous findings in students and assess this with different input modalities (auditory and crossmodal) and different processing strategies (visual transposed word parallel, crossmodal transposed word serial and crossmodal transposed word parallel). In this experiment, we manipulate sequence type and experiment. Thus, we expected that the transposed word effect in the crossmodal transposed word serial processing should be replicated (such that transposed words are responded to more slowly and are more error prone than control words). The main focus is on accuracy. Huang and Staub (2021) argued that the integration of word n+1 may begin before the integration of word n is complete, which may allow the transposed word effect to occur. As a result, the findings we observed may be more likely to ensue, as there is a higher likelihood that word n would be completed when the integration of word n+1 began when all words were presented and heard, the transposed word effect should be smaller or not present in crossmodal serial relative to visual transposed word serial and parallel processing and auditory transposed word tasks.
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
23. Auditory transposed word effect
- Author
-
Elsherif, Mahmoud
- Subjects
RSAP ,Cognition and Perception ,Serial auditory presentation ,word position coding ,Cognitive Psychology ,Linguistics ,Social and Behavioral Sciences ,FOS: Psychology ,Psycholinguistics and Neurolinguistics ,auditory lexical decision task ,Rapid serial auditory presentation ,FOS: Languages and literature ,Psychology ,serial processing ,grammatical decision ,auditory processing ,transposed word effect - Abstract
Evidence has supported the idea that readers lexically process words serially (one at a time), but evidence by Mirault et al. (2018) has demonstrated a transposed word effect. This effect is where readers have difficulty deciding that sentences such as “do love you me? “are ungrammatical, in which the transposition of two adjacent words (i.e., love you) would form a grammatical sentence, compared to sentences in which the transposition of two words does not result in grammaticality (e.g. the cat white was slowly). This has been taken as evidence for the parallel lexical processing of multiple words simultaneously. Evidence has also shown that the transposed word effect can be attributed to a combination of noisy bottom-up word position coding and top-down syntactic constraints. This effect can be interpreted within serial, as opposed to parallel, reading models. The present study will examine the transposed word effect by varying modality input and type of processing for which each of the two transposed words will be presented. In this experiment, we manipulate sequence type and experiment. We aim to investigate this in an auditory modality, which has not been conducted. In auditory lexical decision task, the processing has been argued to be strictly serial in nature (Henderson et al., 2021). As a result, if the transposed word effect is serial in nature, we predict that the transposed word effect should be observed in auditory lexical decision task. However, if transposed word effect is parallel in nature, it should not be observed in the auditory lexical decision task. In addition, we aim to compare the results of auditory transposed word effect with that of visual transposed word serial processing and parallel processing. This is to assess whether the transposed word effect occurs as a result of the integration of word n+1 may begin before the integration of word n is complete, which may allow the transposed word effect to occur (Huang & Staub, 2021) . As a result, the findings we observed may be more likely to ensue, as there is a higher likelihood that word n would not complete when the integration of word n+1 began when all words are presented in a parallel, as opposed to serial manner.
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
24. Visual Transposed word serial processing
- Author
-
Elsherif, Mahmoud
- Subjects
Cognition and Perception ,word position coding ,Cognitive Psychology ,Linguistics ,Social and Behavioral Sciences ,FOS: Psychology ,Psycholinguistics and Neurolinguistics ,reading ,serial visual presentation ,FOS: Languages and literature ,Psychology ,rapid serial visual presentation ,serial processing ,grammatical decision ,RSVP ,transposed word effect - Abstract
Evidence has supported the idea that readers lexically process words serially (one at a time), but evidence by Mirault et al. (2018) has demonstrated a transposed word effect. This effect is where readers have difficulty deciding that sentences such as “do love you me? “are ungrammatical, in which the transposition of two adjacent words (i.e., love you) would form a grammatical sentence, compared to sentences in which the transposition of two words does not result in grammaticality (e.g. the cat white was slowly). This has been taken as evidence for the parallel lexical processing of multiple words simultaneously. Evidence has also shown that the transposed word effect can be attributed to a combination of noisy bottom-up word position coding and top-down syntactic constraints. This effect can be interpreted within serial, as opposed to parallel, reading models. The present study will examine the transposed word effect by varying modality input and type of processing for which each of the two transposed words will be presented. In the previous experiment, we observed the transposed word effect in visual grammatical decision task. However, we aim to replicate the previous findings and assess this pattern of findings with different input modalities (auditory and crossmodal) and different processing strategies (visual transposed word parallel, crossmodal transposed word serial and crossmodal transposed word parallel). In this experiment, we manipulate sequence type and later on modality. Thus, we expected that the transposed word effect in visual serial paradigm to be replicated such that transposed words are responded to more slowly and are more error prone than control words. However, the main focus is on accuracy.
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
25. Transposed word effect and integration
- Author
-
Elsherif, Mahmoud, McGowan, Victoria, Cutter, Michael, and Paterson, Kevin
- Subjects
word position coding ,parallel processing ,grammatical decision task ,Cognitive Psychology ,Linguistics ,Social and Behavioral Sciences ,FOS: Psychology ,Psycholinguistics and Neurolinguistics ,reading ,FOS: Languages and literature ,Psychology ,rapid serial visual presentation ,serial processing ,RSVP ,word transposition effect - Abstract
Evidence has supported the idea that readers lexically process words serially (one at a time), but evidence by Mirault et al. (2018) has demonstrated a transposed word effect. This effect is where readers have difficulty deciding that sentences such as “do love you me? “are ungrammatical, in which the transposition of two adjacent words (i.e., love you) would form a grammatical sentence, compared to sentences in which the transposition of two words does not result in grammaticality (e.g. the cat white was slowly). This has been taken as evidence for the parallel lexical processing of multiple words simultaneously. Evidence has also shown that the transposed word effect can be attributed to a combination of noisy bottom-up word position coding and top-down syntactic constraints. This effect can be interpreted within serial, as opposed to parallel, reading models. The present study will examine the transposed word effect by varying the duration for which each of the two transposed words will be presented. In the previous experiment, we observed the transposed word effects were much larger when all words were presented for 125ms than when they were all presented for 250ms. It has been recently argued by Huang and Staub (2021) that the integration of word n+1 may begin before the integration of word n is complete, which may allow the transposed word effect to occur. As a result, the findings we observed may be more likely to ensue, as there is a higher likelihood that word n would not complete when the integration of word n+1 began when all words were presented for 125ms than when they were presented for 250ms. In this experiment, we manipulate the duration of the transposed pair. As a result, instead of the sentence being presented at 125ms, in one condition, the first word will be presented for 125ms while the second word will be presented for 250ms. In another set of conditions, the first word will be presented for 250ms while the second will be presented for 125ms. The remaining words will be presented for 250ms. For instance, You [250]| that [125]| read [250]| wrong [250]| again. Vs. You [250]| that [250]| read [125]| wrong [250]| again. We would expect that when the first word of the pair is presented at a short duration (125ms), then the integration of the first word might not be complete before integration of the second word occurs, leading to a higher likelihood of a transposition. However, if the first word of the pair is presented at a longer duration (250ms), then it is more likely that the integration of the first word will have completed before integration of the second word, leading to a lower likelihood of a transposition. Thus, we expected the transposed word effect to be larger when the first word is of the pair is presented at 125ms than the second word.
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
26. Transposed word effect and presentation pairing
- Author
-
Elsherif, Mahmoud, McGowan, Victoria, Cutter, Michael, and Paterson, Kevin
- Subjects
word position coding ,parallel processing ,grammatical decision task ,presentation pairing ,Cognitive Psychology ,Linguistics ,Social and Behavioral Sciences ,FOS: Psychology ,Psycholinguistics and Neurolinguistics ,reading ,FOS: Languages and literature ,Psychology ,rapid serial visual presentation ,serial processing ,word transposition effect - Abstract
Evidence has supported the idea that readers lexically process words serially (one at a time), but evidence by Mirault et al. (2018) has demonstrated a transposed word effect. This effect is where readers have difficulty deciding that sentences such as “do love you me? “are ungrammatical, in which the transposition of two adjacent words (i.e., love you) would form a grammatical sentence, compared to sentences in which the transposition of two words does not result in grammaticality (e.g. the cat white was slowly). This has been taken as evidence for the parallel lexical processing of multiple words simultaneously. Evidence has also shown that the transposed word effect can be attributed to a combination of noisy bottom-up word position coding and top-down syntactic constraints. This effect can be interpreted within serial, as opposed to parallel, reading models. The present study will examine the transposed word effect by pairing the transposed words together or presenting them separately. Specifically, we will present the sentences in a word-by-word manner with the transposed words presented together (e.g. the |rich| was man| walked) or separately (e.g. the |rich was| man| walked) to test specific predictions of a serial processing account of the transposed word effect by manipulating sentence type and presentation pairing.
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
27. Transposed word effect and RSVP
- Author
-
Elsherif, Mahmoud, McGowan, Victoria, Paterson, Kevin, and Cutter, Michael
- Subjects
Rapid serial visual presentation ,Cognition and Perception ,word position coding ,grammatical decision task ,Serial processing ,Cognitive Psychology ,Linguistics ,Social and Behavioral Sciences ,FOS: Psychology ,Psycholinguistics and Neurolinguistics ,Reading ,Word-transposition effect ,FOS: Languages and literature ,Parallel processing ,Psychology ,RSVP - Abstract
Evidence has supported the idea that readers lexically process words serially (one at a time), but evidence by Mirault et al. (2018) has demonstrated a transposed word effect. This effect is where readers have difficulty deciding that sentences such as “do love you me? “are ungrammatical, in which the transposition of two adjacent words (i.e., love you) would form a grammatical sentence, compared to sentences in which the transposition of two words does not result in grammaticality (e.g. the cat white was slowly). This has been taken as evidence for the parallel lexical processing of multiple words simultaneously. Evidence has also shown that the transposed word effect can be attributed to a combination of noisy bottom-up word position coding and top-down syntactic constraints. This effect can be interpreted within serial, as opposed to parallel, reading models. Nevertheless, the present study will examine the transposed word effect with rapid serial visual presentation, in which we present the sentences in a word-by-word manner at a very fast sequential rate (i.e. 125ms) or slower rate (i.e.250ms) to test specific predictions of a serial processing account of the transposed word effect by manipulating sentence type and RSVP.
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
28. 上而下控制對雙光點偵測作業知覺決策歷程的影響.
- Author
-
陳世杰, 林佩誼, and 楊政達
- Abstract
Top-down control plays an important role in the selection of an optimal perceptual-decision strategy. However, in previous studies, inferences about the strategies have primarily been based on the participants’ subjective reports after they completed all experiments, which can not reflect their trial-by-trial strategies. Hence, this study investigated whether participants can be consciously aware of and control their strategies. A double-dot detection task was conducted and Systems Factorial Technology (SFT) was used for the experimental design, data analysis, and inferences. In Experiment 1, participants were asked to report their strategy (serial or parallel strategy) immediately after each trial; in Experiment 2, participants were asked to pre-select a strategy before a trial started. The results of Experiment 1 showed that all participants utilized either parallel self-terminating processing or coactive processing regardless of their reported strategies. Similarly, the results of Experiment 2 showed that all participants adopted parallel self-terminating processing regardless of their chosen strategies. These findings suggest that participants fail to intentionally adopt serial processing to detect redundant targets. Moreover, these outcomes occurred perhaps because pre-attentive processing is not affected by top-down control; accordingly, our results could further help understandings of how top-down control affects the perceptual decision process. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
29. An Extension of a Parallel-Distributed Processing Framework of Reading Aloud in Japanese: Human Nonword Reading Accuracy Does Not Require a Sequential Mechanism.
- Author
-
Ikeda, Kenji, Ueno, Taiji, Ito, Yuichi, Kitagami, Shinji, and Kawaguchi, Jun
- Subjects
- *
PDF (Computer file format) , *GRAPHEMICS , *PSYCHOLINGUISTICS , *READING comprehension , *LANGUAGE & languages - Abstract
Humans can pronounce a nonword (e.g., rint). Some researchers have interpreted this behavior as requiring a sequential mechanism by which a grapheme-phoneme correspondence rule is applied to each grapheme in turn. However, several parallel-distributed processing ( PDP) models in English have simulated human nonword reading accuracy without a sequential mechanism. Interestingly, the Japanese psycholinguistic literature went partly in the same direction, but it has since concluded that a sequential parsing mechanism is required to reproduce human nonword reading accuracy. In this study, by manipulating the list composition (i.e., pure word/nonword list vs. mixed list), we demonstrated that past psycholinguistic studies in Japanese have overestimated human nonword reading accuracy. When the more fairly reevaluated human performance was targeted, a newly implemented Japanese PDP model simulated the target accuracy as well as the error patterns. These findings suggest that PDP models are a more parsimonious way of explaining reading across various languages. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
30. You Can't Recognize Two Words Simultaneously.
- Author
-
White, Alex L., Boynton, Geoffrey M., and Yeatman, Jason D.
- Subjects
- *
WORD recognition , *VOCABULARY , *LEXICAL access , *SEMANTICS , *PARALLEL processing - Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
31. A Power-Saving Adaptive Equalizer With a Digital-Controlled Self-Slope Detection.
- Author
-
Tu, Yo-Hao, Cheng, Kuo-Hsing, Lee, Man-Ju, and Liu, Jen-Chieh
- Subjects
- *
ELECTRONIC circuits , *EQUALIZERS (Electronics) - Abstract
When operating at a gigahertz-level frequency, a high-frequency signal is distorted and degraded through the channel. To meet the demand of low cost and the low power consumption for consumer electronic products, this paper proposes a power-saving adaptive equalizer with digital-controlled self-slope detection to compensate channel losses. Reducing and shutting down high-speed circuits in addition to digitization are the most effective methods for minimizing both power and cost. This study also proposes a serial processing for reducing one high-speed detection circuit. The main concept is to use a self-slope detection circuit, which compares two continuous serial slopes, instead of a previous detection circuit, which uses a slicer. After compensation, a shutdown mechanism switches OFF the control circuit to reduce power. A serial processing enables channeling the data through the same circuit and path to avoid swing balancing and mismatch problems. An experimental chip was implemented using 90-nm 1P9M CMOS technology. In the experiment, the equalizer is operated at a supply power of 1 V with 4.35 mW. The core area occupies of $120~\mu \text{m} \times 189~\mu \text{m}$ , and the peak-to-peak jitter measured at 5 Gb/s by using the PRBS31 pattern through a 1.5-m channel is 0.36 UI. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
32. Serial processing in the somatosensory system of macaques
- Author
-
Pons, T. P., Franzén, O., editor, Johansson, R., editor, and Terenius, L., editor
- Published
- 1996
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
33. What Happens in a Moment
- Author
-
Mark A Elliott and Anne eGiersch
- Subjects
time ,perceptual organization ,Simon effect ,Serial Processing ,psychological moment ,Psychology ,BF1-990 - Abstract
There has been evidence for the very brief, temporal quantization of perceptual experience at regular intervals below 100 ms for several decades. We briefly describe how earlier studies led to the concept of ‘psychological moment’ of between 50-60 ms duration. According to historical theories, within the psychological moment all events would be processed as co-temporal. More recently, a link with physiological mechanisms has been proposed, according to which the 50–60 ms psychological moment would be defined by the upper limit required by neural mechanisms to synchronize and thereby represent a snapshot of current perceptual event structure. However, our own experimental developments also identify a more fine-scaled, serialized process structure within the psychological moment. Our data suggests that not all events are processed as co-temporal within the psychological moment and instead, some are processed successively. This evidence questions the analog relationship between synchronized process and simultaneous experience and opens debate on the ontology and function of ‘moments’ in psychological experience.
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
34. Serial vs. parallel models of attention in visual search: accounting for benchmark RT-distributions.
- Author
-
Moran, Rani, Zehetleitner, Michael, Liesefeld, Heinrich, Müller, Hermann, and Usher, Marius
- Subjects
- *
ATTENTION , *VISUAL perception , *COMPLEXITY (Philosophy) , *VISION , *APPERCEPTION - Abstract
Visual search is central to the investigation of selective visual attention. Classical theories propose that items are identified by serially deploying focal attention to their locations. While this accounts for set-size effects over a continuum of task difficulties, it has been suggested that parallel models can account for such effects equally well. We compared the serial Competitive Guided Search model with a parallel model in their ability to account for RT distributions and error rates from a large visual search data-set featuring three classical search tasks: 1) a spatial configuration search (2 vs. 5); 2) a feature-conjunction search; and 3) a unique feature search (Wolfe, Palmer & Horowitz Vision Research, 50(14), 1304-1311, 2010). In the parallel model, each item is represented by a diffusion to two boundaries (target-present/absent); the search corresponds to a parallel race between these diffusors. The parallel model was highly flexible in that it allowed both for a parametric range of capacity-limitation and for set-size adjustments of identification boundaries. Furthermore, a quit unit allowed for a continuum of search-quitting policies when the target is not found, with 'single-item inspection' and exhaustive searches comprising its extremes. The serial model was found to be superior to the parallel model, even before penalizing the parallel model for its increased complexity. We discuss the implications of the results and the need for future studies to resolve the debate. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
35. What Happens in a Moment.
- Author
-
Elliott, Mark A., Giersch, Anne, Herzog, Michael, and Sohn, Hansem
- Subjects
PSYCHOLOGICAL research ,SYNCHRONIZATION ,TIME measurements ,TASK performance - Abstract
There has been evidence for the very brief, temporal quantization of perceptual experience at regular intervals below 100 ms for several decades. We briefly describe how earlier studies led to the concept of "psychological moment" of between 50 and 60 ms duration. According to historical theories, within the psychological moment all events would be processed as co-temporal. More recently, a link with physiological mechanisms has been proposed, according to which the 50-60 ms psychological moment would be defined by the upper limit required by neural mechanisms to synchronize and thereby represent a snapshot of current perceptual event structure. However, our own experimental developments also identify a more fine-scaled, serialized process structure within the psychological moment. Our data suggests that not all events are processed as co-temporal within the psychological moment and instead, some are processed successively. This evidence questions the analog relationship between synchronized process and simultaneous experience and opens debate on the ontology and function of "moments" in psychological experience. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
36. Kindergarteners’ performance in a sound–symbol paradigm predicts early reading.
- Author
-
Horbach, Josefine, Scharke, Wolfgang, Cröll, Jennifer, Heim, Stefan, and Günther, Thomas
- Subjects
- *
KINDERGARTEN children , *BEGINNING reading , *SIGNS & symbols , *SHORT-term memory in children , *PHONOLOGY - Abstract
The current study examined the role of serial processing of newly learned sound–symbol associations in early reading acquisition. A computer-based sound–symbol paradigm (SSP) was administered to 243 children during their last year of kindergarten (T1), and their reading performance was assessed 1 year later in first grade (T2). Results showed that performance on the SSP measured before formal reading instruction was associated with later reading development. At T1, early readers performed significantly better than nonreaders in learning correspondences between sounds and symbols as well as in applying those correspondences in a serial manner. At T2, SSP performance measured at T1 was positively associated with reading performance. Importantly, serial application of newly learned correspondences at T1 explained unique variance in first-grade reading performance in nonreaders over and above other verbal predictors, including phonological awareness, verbal short-term memory, and rapid automatized naming. Consequently, the SSP provides a promising way to study aspects of reading in preliterate children. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
37. Parallel and serial processing in dual-tasking differentially involves mechanisms in the striatum and the lateral prefrontal cortex.
- Author
-
Yildiz, Ali and Beste, Christian
- Subjects
- *
NEUROANATOMY , *PREFRONTAL cortex , *SIGNAL processing , *BASAL ganglia , *FUNCTIONAL magnetic resonance imaging , *PARALLEL processing - Abstract
The lateral prefrontal cortex and the basal ganglia are known to be important for response selection processes, also in dual-task situations. However, response selection in dual-task situations can be achieved using different modes ranging from a parallel selection to a more serial selection of responses. Nothing is known whether differences in these processing modes during dual-tasking have distinct functional neuroanatomical correlates. In this fMRI study we analyzed performance in a psychological refractory period paradigm. In this paradigm we design a PRP task where we vary the frequency of short and long stimulus onset asynchronies between the two tasks. Using mathematical constraints we interpret the effects of this manipulation with respect to processing modes ranging from more serial to more parallel response selection. Contrastingly these blocks showed that response selection in dual-tasking under the constraint of more parallel processing is mediated by mechanisms operating at the striatal level, while response selection under the constraint of more serial processing is mediated via mechanisms operating in the lateral prefrontal cortex. The results suggest that lateral prefrontal and striatal regions are 'optimized' for a certain processing modes in dual tasking. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
38. Hierarchical neural networks perform both serial and parallel processing.
- Author
-
Agliari, Elena, Barra, Adriano, Galluzzi, Andrea, Guerra, Francesco, Tantari, Daniele, and Tavani, Flavia
- Subjects
- *
ARTIFICIAL neural networks , *PARALLEL processing , *RECIPROCALS (Mathematics) , *COMPUTER simulation , *ELECTRICAL load , *COMPUTER multitasking - Abstract
In this work we study a Hebbian neural network, where neurons are arranged according to a hierarchical architecture such that their couplings scale with their reciprocal distance. As a full statistical mechanics solution is not yet available, after a streamlined introduction to the state of the art via that route, the problem is consistently approached through signal-to-noise technique and extensive numerical simulations. Focusing on the low-storage regime, where the amount of stored patterns grows at most logarithmical with the system size, we prove that these non-mean-field Hopfield-like networks display a richer phase diagram than their classical counterparts. In particular, these networks are able to perform serial processing (i.e. retrieve one pattern at a time through a complete rearrangement of the whole ensemble of neurons) as well as parallel processing (i.e. retrieve several patterns simultaneously, delegating the management of different patterns to diverse communities that build network). The tune between the two regimes is given by the rate of the coupling decay and by the level of noise affecting the system. The price to pay for those remarkable capabilities lies in a network’s capacity smaller than the mean field counterpart, thus yielding a new budget principle: the wider the multitasking capabilities, the lower the network load and vice versa. This may have important implications in our understanding of biological complexity. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
39. Is there a serial bottleneck in visual object recognition?
- Author
-
Cathleen M. Moore, Dina V. Popovkina, John Palmer, and Geoffrey M. Boynton
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Computer science ,Speech recognition ,media_common.quotation_subject ,divided attention ,Semantics ,050105 experimental psychology ,Article ,object perception ,03 medical and health sciences ,Judgment ,0302 clinical medicine ,Visual Objects ,Perception ,Feature (machine learning) ,Reaction Time ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Attention ,serial processing ,computer.programming_language ,media_common ,05 social sciences ,Cognitive neuroscience of visual object recognition ,Object (computer science) ,Sensory Systems ,Serial memory processing ,Ophthalmology ,Categorization ,Pattern Recognition, Visual ,Visual Perception ,Female ,computer ,Perceptual Masking ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery - Abstract
Divided attention has little effect for simple tasks, such as luminance detection, but it has large effects for complex tasks, such as semantic categorization of masked words. Here, we asked whether the semantic categorization of visual objects shows divided attention effects as large as those observed for words, or as small as those observed for simple feature judgments. Using a dual-task paradigm with nameable object stimuli, performance was compared with the predictions of serial and parallel models. At the extreme, parallel processes with unlimited capacity predict no effect of divided attention; alternatively, an all-or-none serial process makes two predictions: a large divided attention effect (lower accuracy for dual-task trials, compared to single-task trials) and a negative response correlation in dual-task trials (a given response is more likely to be incorrect when the response about the other stimulus is correct). These predictions were tested in two experiments examining object judgments. In both experiments, there was a large divided attention effect and a small negative correlation in responses. The magnitude of these effects was larger than for simple features, but smaller than for words. These effects were consistent with serial models, and rule out some but not all parallel models. More broadly, the results help establish one of the first examples of likely serial processing in perception.
- Published
- 2021
40. Serial processing in scotopic vision: from upshift to the scotopic band
- Author
-
Spivak, Oleg and Thier, Peter (Prof. Dr.)
- Subjects
Monkey ,genetic structures ,fixation ,Scotopic vision ,serial processing ,sense organs ,saccades ,Affen , Dämmerungssehen , Fixierung , Kaskade - Abstract
It has been known for decades that haplorrhine primates and other non-mammalian foveate vertebrates explore visual scenes by making fast eye movements that allow them to shift the image of an object of interest into the fovea. High spatial resolution accommodated by the fovea can be used to scrutinize the object of interest during a period of steady foveal fixation of the object’s image, interrupted only briefly by different types of miniature eye movements, too small to jeopardize the image position within the confines of the fovea. In this dissertation I address a peculiar aspect of the object fixation of macaque monkeys observed under conditions of full darkness. Macaque monkeys exhibit an upward shift of gaze (for short, ‘upshift’) when asked to fixate a tiny fixation spot in an otherwise dark environment. Given the advantages of foveal vision, the upshift that moves the fovea away from the object of interest, the fixation point, must appear paradoxical. The upshift was first described by Snodderly (1987) in rhesus monkeys and rediscovered and investigated in detail by Barash et al. (1998) who studied it in cynomolgus macaques. Barash et al. (1998) demonstrated that the upshift depended solely on the background luminance and not on the contrast between target and background. Barash and coworkers could also show that the upshift increased with training on the fixation and typically started within seconds after turning the background from bright to dark. The view that the upshift was a hitherto undescribed illumination-dependent fixation offset not related to known features of the systems controlling saccades and fixation, was put into a question by Goffart et al. (2006). This group claimed that the upshift is nothing but a luminance dependent disbalance of the saccadic system for the vertical. However, based on this hypothesis, the upshift should be abolished soon after the onset of fixation. To critically test this hypothesis, we carried out a first study in which we trained rhesus monkeys to fixate small targets on the screen. The monkeys fixated a target in two conditions: with bright or dark background. We investigated the time course of the difference between fixation in darkness and fixation in bright conditions that is the actual upshift. We showed that dark-background-dependent upshift persisted during at least during two seconds of fixation. Additionally, the size of the upshift depended on fixation location: fixation in the lower visual field resulted in larger upshifts whereas fixation in the upper visual field demonstrated smaller upshifts. These results clearly indicated that the upshift cannot merely be a consequence of a dysbalanced saccadic system causing hypermetric upward and hypometric downward saccades as both should be corrected within a few milliseconds, ultimately establishing foveal fixation. Anecdotal observations seemed to suggest that the upshift might differ between monkeys. In an attempt to consolidate this impression and, moreover, to identify a cause explaining eventual differences, we embarked on study of a very large sample of 14 monkeys from two species. We tested the monkeys in the same task that we had used in the preceding study. The results were very clear: all monkeys in our sample had upshift and the monkeys lacked systematic horizontal deviation during fixation in darkness. However, the monkeys tested exhibited substantial differences as to the size of the upshift. The monkeys in our large sample differed by the level of ‘habituation’: dark habituated (monkeys that had been previously trained in tasks in full darkness with small bright stimuli) and bright-habituated (monkeys that had been trained in tasks with large bright stimuli without control for full darkness of experimental setup). We showed that the size of the upshift largely reflects the extent to which a monkey is habituated to work in the dark. Dark habituated monkeys with mostly belonged to 2 the group with higher upshift whereas bright habituated monkeys were very likely to demonstrate lower upshift. Species differences (cynomolgus vs. rhesus macaques) were not found. In seeking to explain the upshift, we resorted to the geometry of the rod and cone densities, which constitute a hard bound for the resolution of the percept. Cones peak in the fovea; extrafoveally, cone density decreases as eccentricity increases. Rods are absent in the foveola. On going dorsally from the foveola, rod density increases, until reaching a peak in a location called dorsal rod peak, or rod hotspot (Packer et al., 1989; Wickler and Rakic, 1990; Wickler et al., 1990). We therefore started with the hypothesis that if any retinal location replaces the fovea in scotopic vision, it would be the rod hotspot. We therefore expected that the vertical component of the line of gaze would be distributed bimodally, one mode reflecting the fovea and the other the rod hotspot. However, the results did not corroborate this hypothesis. Upshift height varied a lot, from monkey to monkey, from condition to condition, even within session. Eventually we were led to an alternative hypothesis, that of a scotopic band. We now suggest that at any epoch during scotopic vision there might be a scotopic center, located dorsally to the fovea. The scotopic center replaces the fovea. During fixations, target images are projected on the scotopic center, not on the fovea. Saccadic trajectories show that saccades shift the target’s image directly to the scotopic center. Therefore, not all saccades foveate. Scotopic saccades do not foveate. The relative weight of the scotopic center in the evolution of saccades remains open. In any case, scotopic saccades come with their own sensorimotor transformations, as do scotopic fixations. Scotopic center is only the beginning of the scotopic analogy for high-acuity vision. Unlike the close, tight fovea of photopic vision, the scotopic center moves along a line extending dorsally from immediately dorsal to the fovea. We call this the scotopic band. The presence of the band makes scotopic sensorimotor transformations more complex than photopic because they rely on a parameter – the scotopic band setting, the current location of the scotopic center on the band. Scotopic band is set primarily according to the ambient light, reflected in the laboratory as background luminance. Increasing luminance results in lower upshift, or, equivalently, more ventral setting. However, other factors heavily influence the scotopic setting too. To mention but 2: habituation to darkness increases the upshift, that is, makes the setting more dorsal; and so do also threshold task conditions. Thus, we suggest that ultimately, the scotopic band setting reflects the statistics of the scene and the monkey’s task. The computational needs change gradually, and so do the anatomy (and physiology). Therefore, scotopic band setting is not limited to the two endpoints but occupy the points in between.
- Published
- 2021
41. Serial processing in primary and secondary somatosensory cortex: A DCM analysis of human fMRI data in response to innocuous and noxious electrical stimulation.
- Author
-
Khoshnejad, Mina, Piché, Mathieu, Saleh, Soha, Duncan, Gary, and Rainville, Pierre
- Subjects
- *
SOMATOSENSORY cortex , *FUNCTIONAL magnetic resonance imaging , *NEURAL stimulation , *BRAIN imaging , *DATA analysis - Abstract
The anatomy of the somatosensory system allows both serial and parallel information flow but the conditions involving each mode of processing is a matter of debate. In this functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study, cutaneous electrical stimulation was applied to human volunteers at three intensities (low-innocuous, moderate-noxious and high-noxious) to investigate interactions between contralateral primary and secondary somatosensory cortices (S1c and S2c), and between contralateral and ipsilateral S2 (S2c and S2i), using dynamic causal modeling (DCM). Our results are consistent with serial processing with a key role of the direct input to S1c for all three intensity levels. The more intense stimulus also induced significantly more interactions between S2i and S2c, consistent with an increase in inter-hemispheric integration associated with the additional recruitment of nociceptive inputs. However, stronger pain reports were also associated with reduced information flow from S1c to S2c at both the moderate (r = -0.81, p = 0.004) and the high stimulation level (r = -0.63, p = 0.037). These findings suggest that the connectivity pattern driven by innocuous inputs is modified by the additional activation of nociceptive afferents. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
42. Parallel processing in language production.
- Author
-
Melinger, Alissa, Branigan, Holly P., and Pickering, Martin J.
- Abstract
Following the Sixth International Workshop on Language Production (Edinburgh, UK, Sept., 2010), this special issue presents a collection of contributions concerned with a wide range of representational and processing components. In the present article, we review the evidence for parallel processing at different levels within the production system with the aim of identifying any generalisation or common characteristics that might underpin a robust model of language production. Our review synergises with the other articles of the special issue. After reviewing the literature, we conclude that the evidence for parallelism is stronger at some levels than at others and it is premature to take a strong stand for a unified principle that applies equally to all components of the production system. Following our review, we introduce the other articles represented within this special issue. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
43. Event-related potential N270 delayed and enhanced by the conjunction of relevant and irrelevant perceptual mismatch.
- Author
-
Bennett, Matthew A., Duke, Philip A., and Fuggetta, Giorgio
- Subjects
- *
EVOKED potentials (Electrophysiology) , *SENSORY perception , *AVERSIVE stimuli , *SELECTIVITY (Psychology) , *DISTRACTION , *COGNITION - Abstract
Event-related potential studies using delayed match-to-sample tasks have demonstrated the presence of two components, N270 and N400, possibly reflecting the sequential processing of multiple sources of endogenous mismatch. To date, studies have only investigated mismatch between a single cue and target. In this study, we used distractor stimuli to investigate the effect of a secondary source of mismatch distinct from the task-relevant stimulus. Subjects performed two paradigms in which the cue and target could match or mismatch. In one paradigm, task-irrelevant distractors were added-producing a source of task-irrelevant perceptual mismatch. A mismatch-triggered negativity was elicited in both paradigms, but was delayed and enhanced in magnitude in the distractors present paradigm. It is suggested that the distractors may differentially affect mismatch responses through the generation of a task-irrelevant mismatch response. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
44. Characterizing the dynamics of mental representations: the temporal generalization method.
- Author
-
King, J-R. and Dehaene, S.
- Subjects
- *
MENTAL representation , *TEMPORAL integration , *FUNCTIONAL magnetic resonance imaging , *NEURON analysis , *STIMULUS generalization , *BRAIN imaging - Abstract
Highlights: [•] MVPA is often used to detect static fMRI activation patterns. [•] Adapting MVPA to time-resolved signals can characterize the dynamics of neural responses. [•] The temporal generalization matrix reveals a repertoire of canonical brain dynamics. [Copyright &y& Elsevier]
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
45. Serial Processing
- Author
-
Volkmar, Fred R., editor
- Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
46. Serial and strategic memory processes in goal-directed selective remembering.
- Author
-
Murphy, Dillon H., Schwartz, Shawn T., and Castel, Alan D.
- Subjects
- *
MEMORY , *COGNITIVE ability , *CONTROL (Psychology) , *WORD frequency , *MOTIVATION (Psychology) , *LEARNING , *RESEARCH funding , *GOAL (Psychology) - Abstract
People often rely on habitual, serial processing when presented with to-be-learned information. We tested how strategic processing can override more bottom-up, serial processes when remembering information by having participants study a list of word triads (e.g., "dollar phone pizza"). Participants' goal was manipulated by maximizing either (i) their recall for each of the studied words or (ii) their total score associated with recalling certain words in each triad that were more valuable (worth more points) to engage either serial or strategic processing and retrieval mechanisms. Results revealed that when learners were told to maximize their total recall, they frequently engaged in serial remembering-remembering guided by an item's location within the study phase (i.e., words were retrieved according to a habitual reading bias). However, when words were paired with point values that counted towards participants' scores if recalled, participants were not only selective for high-value words but also attempted to overcome the tendency to engage in serial remembering; instead, they appeared to engage in strategic remembering whereby retrieval is guided by value. Thus, to maximize memory utility, it may be beneficial to override habitual processes and initiate retrieval with high-value words, and when making recall transitions, to recall high-value words together. Importantly, when certain to-be-remembered words were more valuable than their neighbors, participants still demonstrated some serial processing of the to-be-remembered words, indicating that even when engaging in strategic memory, some habitual processes can persist. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
47. Serial Processing
- Author
-
Kreutzer, Jeffrey S., editor, DeLuca, John, editor, and Caplan, Bruce, editor
- Published
- 2011
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
48. Parallel processing and the human mind: Re-understanding consciousness with James Joyce's Ulysses.
- Author
-
Hogan, Patrick Colm
- Subjects
- *
PARALLEL processing , *CONSCIOUSNESS , *COGNITION , *BIOLOGICAL neural networks , *BRAIN physiology , *COGNITION in literature - Abstract
Research on neural models for cognition suggests that thought is far from a simply serial process. Nonetheless, there has been relatively little work on which parameters govern just what aspects of thought are parallel and what are serial. Clearly, speech as such is serial. In consequence, interior monologue (understood as subvocalised speech) is serial. Moreover, stream of consciousness - mental experience not confined to subvocalised speech - must be articulated in serial form in a novel. Due to this constraint on representation, it seems that novelists commonly imagine that stream of consciousness itself really is serial. Joyce, however, developed a sense of parallel cognitive processing in the course of Ulysses. Specifically, in the 'Wandering Rocks' episode, he explored spatiotemporally parallel events in complex social systems. In the following chapter, 'Sirens,' he in effect transferred this treatment of external parallelism to the human mind, systematically developing cognitive parallelism in his representation of Leopold Bloom. This development was perhaps reinforced by ideas of harmony and counterpoint associated with the episode's musical model. Understanding Joyce's exploration of parallel and serial processes in thought is important not only for what it tells us about Ulysses. It is also important for what it contributes to our understanding of cognitive parallelism. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
49. Eye movements in reading: Some theoretical context.
- Author
-
Radach, Ralph and Kennedy, Alan
- Subjects
- *
EYE movements , *READING , *WORD recognition , *EYE movement measurements , *INFORMATION processing ,WRITING - Abstract
The study of eye movements has proven to be one of the most successful approaches in research on reading. In this overview, it is argued that a major reason for this success is that eye movement measurement is not just a methodology—the control of eye movements is actually part and parcel of the dynamics of information processing within the task of reading itself. Some major developments over the last decade are discussed with a focus on the issue of spatially distributed word processing and its relation to the development of reading models. The survey ends with a description of two newly emerging trends in the field: the study of continuous reading in non-Roman writing systems and the broadening of the scope of research to encompass individual differences and developmental issues. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
50. Serial and parallel processing in reading: Investigating the effects of parafoveal orthographic information on nonisolated word recognition.
- Author
-
Dare, Natasha and Shillcock, Richard
- Subjects
- *
READING , *ORTHOGRAPHY & spelling , *LEXICAL access , *WORD recognition , *EYE movements , *VISION - Abstract
We present a novel lexical decision task and three boundary paradigm eye-tracking experiments that clarify the picture of parallel processing in word recognition in context. First, we show that lexical decision is facilitated by associated letter information to the left and right of the word, with no apparent hemispheric specificity. Second, we show that parafoveal preview of a repeat of wordnat wordn +1 facilitates reading of wordnrelative to a control condition with an unrelated word at wordn +1. Third, using a version of the boundary paradigm that allowed for a regressive eye movement, we show no parafoveal “postview” effect on reading wordnof repeating wordnat wordn –1.Fourth, we repeat the second experiment but compare the effects of parafoveal previews consisting of a repeated wordnwith a transposed central bigram (e.g.,caotforcoat) and a substituted central bigram (e.g.,ceitforcoat), showing the latter to have a deleterious effect on processing wordn, thereby demonstrating that the parafoveal preview effect is at least orthographic and not purely visual. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.