29 results on '"Ruitenberg MFL"'
Search Results
2. Food-related exploration across the menstrual cycle.
- Author
-
Nijboer ACS, Sellitto M, Ruitenberg MFL, Kerkkänen KIL, and Schomaker J
- Subjects
- Adult, Animals, Female, Humans, Adolescent, Young Adult, Middle Aged, Estradiol, Food, Exploratory Behavior, Menstrual Cycle, Progesterone
- Abstract
When deciding what to eat we constantly weigh different aspects of the options at hand and make trade-offs between exploiting opportunities with a known outcome (e.g., eating your usual meal) and exploring novel opportunities with a potentially better outcome (e.g., trying a new dish). Environmental factors, such as scarcity, have previously been shown to tip the balance towards either exploration or exploitation. Studies in animals have further linked female steroid hormones (including estradiol and progesterone) to exploratory behavior. Previous work in humans has suggested that food preferences and food intake also change over the menstrual cycle. However, it remains unknown whether exploratory behavior in food choices also changes across the menstrual phases in humans. In a rating phase, 112 adult women (age range 18-45 years) on or off hormonal birth control rated 40 food items on desirability. In the choice phase, participants made binary choices between food items. On some trials, a surprise box replaced one of the two food options, allowing us to examine exploratory choices. Women off hormonal birth control reported their average cycle length and time since the first day of their last menstruation. Analysis of the percentage surprise choices across the menstrual cycle indicated a small, but significant effect, with exploratory choice behavior tending to increase around the middle of/later during the cycle. This provides preliminary novel evidence that hormonal fluctuations are associated with food-related exploratory choice behavior. Importantly, these effects were observed beyond effects of other food-related factors. Future studies should investigate the nature of these effects using more direct hormonal measures., Competing Interests: Declaration of competing interest The authors declare no financial interests/personal relationships which may be considered as potential competing interests. If there are other authors, they declare that they have no known competing financial interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence the work reported in this paper., (Copyright © 2024 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Ltd.. All rights reserved.)
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
3. Dopamine system involvement in impulse control.
- Author
-
MacDonald HJ and Ruitenberg MFL
- Subjects
- Humans, Dopamine Agonists, Dopamine, Parkinson Disease
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
4. Don't be late! Postponing cognitive decline and preventing early unemployment in people with multiple sclerosis: a study protocol.
- Author
-
Aarts J, Saddal SRD, Bosmans JE, de Groot V, de Jong BA, Klein M, Ruitenberg MFL, Schaafsma FG, Schippers ECF, Schoonheim MM, Uitdehaag BMJ, van der Veen S, Waskowiak PT, Widdershoven GAM, van der Hiele K, and Hulst HE
- Subjects
- Humans, Quality of Life, Unemployment, Exercise, Randomized Controlled Trials as Topic, Multiple Sclerosis complications, Multiple Sclerosis therapy, Cognitive Dysfunction prevention & control
- Abstract
Background: Up to 65% of people with multiple sclerosis (PwMS) develop cognitive deficits, which hampers their ability to work, participating in day-to-day life and ultimately reducing quality of life (QoL). Early cognitive symptoms are often less tangible to PwMS and their direct environment and are noticed only when symptoms and work functioning problems become more advanced, i.e., when (brain) damage is already advanced. Treatment of symptoms at a late stage can lead to cognitive impairment and unemployment, highlighting the need for preventative interventions in PwMS., Aims: This study aims to evaluate the (cost-) effectiveness of two innovative preventative interventions, aimed at postponing cognitive decline and work functioning problems, compared to enhanced usual care in improving health-related QoL (HRQoL)., Methods: Randomised controlled trial including 270 PwMS with mild cognitive impairment, who have paid employment ≥ 12 h per week and are able to participate in physical exercise (Expanded Disability Status Scale < 6.0). Participants are randomised across three study arms: 1) 'strengthening the brain' - a lifestyle intervention combining personal fitness, mental coaching, dietary advice, and cognitive training; 2) 'strengthening the mind' - a work-focused intervention combining the capability approach and the participatory approach in one-on-one coaching by trained work coaches who have MS themselves; 3) Control group-receiving general information about cognitive impairment in MS and receiving care as usual. Intervention duration is four months, with short-term and long-term follow-up measurements at 10 and 16 months, respectively. The primary outcome measure of the Don't be late! intervention study will be HRQoL as measured with the 36-item Short Form. Secondary outcomes include cognition, work related outcomes, physical functioning, structural and functional brain changes, psychological functioning, and societal costs. Semi-structured interviews and focus groups with stakeholders will be organised to qualitatively reflect on the process and outcome of the interventions., Discussion: This study seeks to prevent (further) cognitive decline and job loss due to MS by introducing tailor-made interventions at an early stage of cognitive symptoms, thereby maintaining or improving HRQoL. Qualitative analyses will be performed to allow successful implementation into clinical practice., Trial Registration: Retrospectively registered at ClinicalTrials.gov with reference number NCT06068582 on 10 October 2023., (© 2024. The Author(s).)
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
5. Don't be late! Timely identification of cognitive impairment in people with multiple sclerosis: a study protocol.
- Author
-
Waskowiak PT, de Jong BA, Uitdehaag BMJ, Saddal SRD, Aarts J, Roovers AAM, van Oirschot P, de Groot V, Schaafsma FG, van der Hiele K, Ruitenberg MFL, Schoonheim MM, Widdershoven GAM, van der Veen S, Schippers ECF, Klein M, and Hulst HE
- Subjects
- Humans, Quality of Life psychology, Reproducibility of Results, Cross-Sectional Studies, Cognition, Neuropsychological Tests, Multicenter Studies as Topic, Multiple Sclerosis complications, Multiple Sclerosis diagnosis, Multiple Sclerosis psychology, Cognitive Dysfunction diagnosis, Cognitive Dysfunction etiology
- Abstract
Background: Cognitive impairment occurs in up to 65% of people with multiple sclerosis (PwMS), negatively affecting daily functioning and health-related quality of life. In general, neuropsychological testing is not part of standard MS-care due to insufficient time and trained personnel. Consequently, a baseline assessment of cognitive functioning is often lacking, hampering early identification of cognitive decline and change within a person over time. To assess cognitive functioning in PwMS in a time-efficient manner, a BICAMS-based self-explanatory digital screening tool called the Multiple Screener
© , has recently been developed. The aim of the current study is to validate the Multiple Screener© in a representative sample of PwMS in the Netherlands. Additionally, we aim to investigate how cognitive functioning is related to psychological factors, and both work and societal participation., Methods: In this cross-sectional multicentre study, 750 PwMS (aged 18-67 years) are included. To obtain a representative sample, PwMS are recruited via 12 hospitals across the Netherlands. They undergo assessment with the Minimal Assessment of Cognitive Functioning in MS (MACFIMS; reference-standard) and the Multiple Screener© . Sensitivity, specificity, and predictive values for identifying (mild) cognitive impairment are determined in a subset of 300 participants. In a second step, the identified cut-off values are tested in an independent subset of at least 150 PwMS. Moreover, test-retest reliability for the Multiple Screener© is determined in 30 PwMS. Information on psychological and work-related factors is assessed with questionnaires., Discussion: Validating the Multiple Screener© in PwMS and investigating cognition and its determinants will further facilitate early identification and adequate monitoring of cognitive decline in PwMS., (© 2024. The Author(s).)- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
6. Classification of Mild Cognitive Impairment and Alzheimer's Disease Using Manual Motor Measures.
- Author
-
Koppelmans V, Ruitenberg MFL, Schaefer SY, King JB, Jacobo JM, Silvester BP, Mejia AF, van der Geest J, Hoffman JM, Tasdizen T, and Duff K
- Subjects
- Humans, Female, Male, Aged, Hand Strength physiology, Aged, 80 and over, Psychomotor Performance physiology, Amyloid beta-Peptides metabolism, Hippocampus pathology, Middle Aged, Positron-Emission Tomography methods, Neuropsychological Tests, Cognitive Dysfunction diagnosis, Cognitive Dysfunction classification, Cognitive Dysfunction physiopathology, Alzheimer Disease classification, Alzheimer Disease diagnosis, Alzheimer Disease physiopathology
- Abstract
Introduction: Manual motor problems have been reported in mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and Alzheimer's disease (AD), but the specific aspects that are affected, their neuropathology, and potential value for classification modeling is unknown. The current study examined if multiple measures of motor strength, dexterity, and speed are affected in MCI and AD, related to AD biomarkers, and are able to classify MCI or AD., Methods: Fifty-three cognitively normal (CN), 33 amnestic MCI, and 28 AD subjects completed five manual motor measures: grip force, Trail Making Test A, spiral tracing, finger tapping, and a simulated feeding task. Analyses included (1) group differences in manual performance; (2) associations between manual function and AD biomarkers (PET amyloid β, hippocampal volume, and APOE ε4 alleles); and (3) group classification accuracy of manual motor function using machine learning., Results: Amnestic MCI and AD subjects exhibited slower psychomotor speed and AD subjects had weaker dominant hand grip strength than CN subjects. Performance on these measures was related to amyloid β deposition (both) and hippocampal volume (psychomotor speed only). Support vector classification well-discriminated control and AD subjects (area under the curve of 0.73 and 0.77, respectively) but poorly discriminated MCI from controls or AD., Conclusion: Grip strength and spiral tracing appear preserved, while psychomotor speed is affected in amnestic MCI and AD. The association of motor performance with amyloid β deposition and atrophy could indicate that this is due to amyloid deposition in and atrophy of motor brain regions, which generally occurs later in the disease process. The promising discriminatory abilities of manual motor measures for AD emphasize their value alongside other cognitive and motor assessment outcomes in classification and prediction models, as well as potential enrichment of outcome variables in AD clinical trials., (© 2024 The Author(s). Published by S. Karger AG, Basel.)
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
7. Developmental and age differences in visuomotor adaptation across the lifespan.
- Author
-
Ruitenberg MFL, Koppelmans V, Seidler RD, and Schomaker J
- Subjects
- Humans, Male, Female, Aged, Child, Adolescent, Young Adult, Adult, Middle Aged, Cross-Sectional Studies, Adaptation, Physiological, Cognition, Psychomotor Performance, Longevity
- Abstract
In the present cross-sectional study, we examined age and sex differences in sensorimotor adaptation. We tested 253 individuals at a local science museum (NEMO Science Museum, Amsterdam). Participants spanned a wide age range (8-70 years old; 54% male), allowing us to examine effects of both development and healthy aging within a single study. Participants performed a visuomotor adaptation task in which they had to adapt manual joystick movements to rotated visual feedback. We assessed the rate of adaptation following the introduction of the visual perturbation (both for early and later stages of adaptation), and the rate of de-adaptation following its removal. Results showed reliable adaptation patterns which did not differ by sex. We observed a quadratic relationship between age and both early adaptation and de-adaptation rates, with younger and older adults exhibiting the fasted adaptation rates. Our findings suggest that both younger and older age are associated with poorer strategic, cognitive processes involved in adaptation. We propose that developmental and age differences in cognitive functions and brain properties may underlie these effects on sensorimotor functioning., (© 2023. The Author(s).)
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
8. Testing the dopamine overdose hypothesis in action control: A study in people with Parkinson's disease.
- Author
-
Ruitenberg MFL, Abrahamse EL, Santens P, and Notebaert W
- Subjects
- Humans, Dopamine metabolism, Dopamine therapeutic use, Neuropsychological Tests, Dopamine Agents pharmacology, Dopamine Agents therapeutic use, Cognition, Parkinson Disease drug therapy, Parkinson Disease complications
- Abstract
Prior work on patients with Parkinson's disease (PD) has shown that the administration of dopaminergic medication in the early to intermediate stages of PD benefits (motor) functions associated with the dopamine-depleted dorsal striatal circuitry but may 'overdose' and interfere with (cognitive) functions associated with the relatively intact ventral striatal circuitry. The present study aimed to elucidate this so-called dopamine overdose hypothesis for the action control domain. Using a within-subject design in a sample of 13 people with PD, we evaluated the effect of dopaminergic medication on two cognitive processes underlying goal-directed behaviour, namely action selection and initiation through event binding and conflict adaptation. We also investigated whether individual differences in the magnitude of medication effects were associated across these processes. Results showed no indications that dopaminergic medication affects action selection and initiation or conflict adaptation in PD patients. Additionally, we observed no correlations between both cognitive processes nor between individual differences in medication effects. Our findings do not support the notion that dopaminergic medication modulates action control processes, suggesting that the dopamine overdose hypothesis may only apply to a specific set of cognitive processes and should potentially be refined., (© 2022 The Authors. Journal of Neuropsychology published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd on behalf of The British Psychological Society.)
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
9. Novelty-induced memory boosts in humans: The when and how.
- Author
-
Lorents A, Ruitenberg MFL, and Schomaker J
- Abstract
Novel information potentially signals danger or reward and behavioral and psychophysiological studies have suggested that the brain prioritizes its processing. Some effects of novelty even go beyond the stimulus itself. Studies in animals have robustly shown that exposure to novel stimulation can promote memory for information presented before or after this exposure. Research regarding effects of novelty on memory in humans is lagging, but in the last few years, several studies have emerged that suggest that memory-facilitating effects of novelty also exist in humans. Here, we provide a comprehensive overview of these studies. We identified several factors that have been shown to influence whether novelty promotes memory or not, including the timing between the novel experience and the learning events, the involvement with the novel material, and population characteristics (such as clinical diagnosis or age). Finally, we link the behavioral findings to potential neurobiological mechanisms and discuss the relevance of specific findings in light of potential clinical and educational applications that could leverage novelty to improve memory., Competing Interests: The authors declare no competing interests., (© 2023 The Authors.)
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
10. Memory's penumbra in the older or pathological brain.
- Author
-
Schomaker J, Ruitenberg MFL, and Takeuchi T
- Subjects
- Humans, Brain, Memory
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
11. Delayed and More Variable Unimanual and Bimanual Finger Tapping in Alzheimer's Disease: Associations with Biomarkers and Applications for Classification.
- Author
-
Koppelmans V, Ruitenberg MFL, Schaefer SY, King JB, Hoffman JM, Mejia AF, Tasdizen T, and Duff K
- Subjects
- Humans, Amyloid beta-Peptides, Biomarkers, Alzheimer Disease psychology, Cognitive Dysfunction psychology, Amyloidosis
- Abstract
Background: Despite reports of gross motor problems in mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and Alzheimer's disease (AD), fine motor function has been relatively understudied., Objective: We examined if finger tapping is affected in AD, related to AD biomarkers, and able to classify MCI or AD., Methods: Forty-seven cognitively normal, 27 amnestic MCI, and 26 AD subjects completed unimanual and bimanual computerized tapping tests. We tested 1) group differences in tapping with permutation models; 2) associations between tapping and biomarkers (PET amyloid-β, hippocampal volume, and APOEɛ4 alleles) with linear regression; and 3) the predictive value of tapping for group classification using machine learning., Results: AD subjects had slower reaction time and larger speed variability than controls during all tapping conditions, except for dual tapping. MCI subjects performed worse than controls on reaction time and speed variability for dual and non-dominant hand tapping. Tapping speed and variability were related to hippocampal volume, but not to amyloid-β deposition or APOEɛ4 alleles. Random forest classification (overall accuracy = 70%) discriminated control and AD subjects, but poorly discriminated MCI from controls or AD., Conclusions: MCI and AD are linked to more variable finger tapping with slower reaction time. Associations between finger tapping and hippocampal volume, but not amyloidosis, suggest that tapping deficits are related to neuropathology that presents later during the disease. Considering that tapping performance is able to differentiate between control and AD subjects, it can offer a cost-efficient tool for augmenting existing AD biomarkers.
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
12. Effects of exploring a novel environment on memory across the lifespan.
- Author
-
Schomaker J, Baumann V, and Ruitenberg MFL
- Subjects
- Adolescent, Aged, Child, Hippocampus, Humans, Mental Recall, Dopamine, Longevity
- Abstract
Exploration of a novel environment has been shown to promote memory formation in healthy adults. Studies in animals have suggested that such novelty-induced memory boosts are mediated by hippocampal dopamine. The dopaminergic system is known to develop and deteriorate over the lifespan, but so far, the effects of novelty on memory across the lifespan have not yet been investigated. In the current study, we had children, adolescents, younger, and older adults (n = 439) explore novel and previously familiarized virtual environments to pinpoint the effects of spatial novelty on declarative memory in humans across different age groups. After exploration, words were presented while participants performed a deep or shallow encoding task. Incidental memory was quantified in a surprise test. Results showed that participants in the deep encoding condition remembered more words than those in the shallow condition, while novelty did not influence this effect. Interestingly, however, children, adolescents and younger adults benefitted from exploring a novel compared to a familiar environment as evidenced by better word recall, while these effects were absent in older adults. Our findings suggest that the beneficial effects of novelty on memory follow the deterioration of neural pathways involved in novelty-related processes across the lifespan., (© 2022. The Author(s).)
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
13. Neural correlates of risky decision making in Parkinson's disease patients with impulse control disorders.
- Author
-
Ruitenberg MFL, Koppelmans V, Wu T, Averbeck BB, Chou KL, and Seidler RD
- Subjects
- Decision Making physiology, Humans, Impulsive Behavior physiology, Reward, Disruptive, Impulse Control, and Conduct Disorders complications, Disruptive, Impulse Control, and Conduct Disorders etiology, Parkinson Disease
- Abstract
Some patients with Parkinson's disease (PD) experience impulse control disorders (ICDs), characterized by deficient voluntary control over impulses, drives, or temptations regarding excessive hedonic behavior. The present study aimed to better understand the neural basis of impulsive, risky decision making in PD patients with ICDs by disentangling potential dysfunctions in decision and outcome mechanisms. We collected fMRI data from 20 patients with ICDs and 28 without ICDs performing an information gathering task. Patients viewed sequences of bead colors drawn from hidden urns and were instructed to infer the majority bead color in each urn. With each new bead, they could choose to either seek more evidence by drawing another bead (draw choice) or make an urn-inference (urn choice followed by feedback). We manipulated risk via the probability of bead color splits (80/20 vs. 60/40) and potential loss following an incorrect inference ($10 vs. $0). Patients also completed the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS) to assess impulsivity. Patients with ICDs showed greater urn choice-specific activation in the right middle frontal gyrus, overlapping the dorsal premotor cortex. Across all patients, fewer draw choices (i.e., more impulsivity) were associated with greater activation during both decision making and outcome processing in a variety of frontal and parietal areas, cerebellum, and bilateral striatum. Our findings demonstrate that ICDs in PD are associated with differences in neural processing of risk-related information and outcomes, implicating both reward and sensorimotor dopaminergic pathways., (© 2022. The Author(s), under exclusive licence to Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature.)
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
14. The subjective evaluation of task switch cues is related to voluntary task switching.
- Author
-
Vermeylen L, Braem S, Notebaert W, and Ruitenberg MFL
- Subjects
- Humans, Individuality, Motor Activity, Psychomotor Performance, Reaction Time, Attention, Cues
- Abstract
Task switching refers to the effortful mental process of shifting attention between different tasks. While it is well-established that task switching usually comes with an objective performance cost, recent studies have shown that people also subjectively evaluate task switching as negative. An open question is whether this affective evaluation of task switching is also related to actual decision making. In this pre-registered study, we therefore examined whether individual differences in the negative evaluation of task switch cues are related to less voluntary task switching. To this end, participants first performed a cued task switching paradigm where abstract cues signaled task transitions (repetition or alternation). In a second phase, these transition cues were used as prime stimuli in an affective priming procedure to assess participants' affective evaluation of task switching. In a third phase, participants were allowed to freely choose whether to switch or repeat tasks. We found that a more negative evaluation of task switching cues was related to lower switch rates in the voluntary task switching phase. This finding supports neuroeconomic theories of value-based decision making which suggest that people use their subjective value of control to decide whether to engage in (different) tasks., (Copyright © 2022 The Authors. Published by Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.)
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
15. Novelty exposure induces stronger sensorimotor representations during a manual adaptation task.
- Author
-
Ruitenberg MFL, Koppelmans V, Seidler RD, and Schomaker J
- Subjects
- Acclimatization, Adaptation, Physiological, Humans, Movement, Dopamine, Psychomotor Performance
- Abstract
Active exploration of novel spatial environments enhances memory for subsequently presented explicit, declarative information in humans. These effects have been attributed to novelty promoting dopamine release via mesolimbic dopaminergic pathways in the brain. As procedural motor learning has been linked to dopamine as well, we predict that novelty effects extend to this domain. To test this hypothesis, the present study examined whether spatial novelty exploration benefits subsequent sensorimotor adaptation. Participants explored either two different virtual environments (i.e., novelty condition; n = 210) or two identical environments (i.e., familiar condition; n = 253). They then performed a manual adaptation task in which they had to adapt joystick movements to a visual perturbation. We assessed the rate of adaptation following the introduction of this perturbation, and the rate of deadaptation following its removal. While results showed reliable adaptation patterns and similar adaptation rates across both conditions, individuals in the novelty condition showed slower deadaptation. This suggests that exposure to spatial novelty induced stronger sensorimotor representations during adaptation, potentially through novelty-induced dopaminergic effects in mesocortical and/or nigrostriatal pathways. Novelty exposure may be employed to promote motor learning on tasks that require precision movements in altered sensory contexts, for example, in astronauts moving in microgravity or patients with impaired motor processing., (© 2021 The Authors. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences published by Wiley Periodicals LLC on behalf of New York Academy of Sciences.)
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
16. Trait Anxiety as a Risk Factor for Impulse Control Disorders in de novo Parkinson's Disease.
- Author
-
Waskowiak P, Koppelmans V, and Ruitenberg MFL
- Subjects
- Aged, Anxiety diagnosis, Anxiety etiology, Humans, Prospective Studies, Risk Factors, Disruptive, Impulse Control, and Conduct Disorders complications, Disruptive, Impulse Control, and Conduct Disorders etiology, Parkinson Disease complications, Parkinson Disease diagnosis, Parkinson Disease epidemiology
- Abstract
Background: In addition to the well-known motor symptoms, patients with Parkinson's disease (PD) also frequently experience disabling non-motor symptoms including impulse control disorders (ICDs). ICDs are characterized by a loss of voluntary control over impulses, drives, or temptations regarding excessive hedonic behavior., Objective: The present study examined whether depression and anxiety in de novo PD patients predict the prospective development of ICDs., Methods: We selected 330 de novo PD patients from the Parkinson's Progression Markers Initiative database who were free of ICDs at the start of the study. ICD presence at baseline and follow-up assessments was evaluated via the shortened version of the Questionnaire for Impulsive-Compulsive Disorders (QUIP-S). Baseline depression and anxiety were measured via the Geriatric Depression Scale (GDS-15) and State-Trait-Anxiety Inventory (STAI-Y), respectively., Results: A total of 149 participants (45.2%) developed an ICD at follow-up and average time of ICD onset was 35 months after baseline. Results of a Cox regression analysis showed that STAI-Y scores but not GDS-15 scores significantly predicted ICD presence. Specifically, scores reflecting higher trait anxiety were associated with an increased risk of developing an ICD. This effect was not confounded by age, gender or UPDRS motor score. We also replicated the well-established result that dopamine agonist use is predictive of ICDs., Conclusion: Our findings indicate that higher anxiety levels in de novo PD patients represent a risk factor for ICD development during the course of the disorder. This highlights the need for early and routine based anxiety screening in these patients.
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
17. The role of dopamine in action control: Insights from medication effects in Parkinson's disease.
- Author
-
Ruitenberg MFL, van Wouwe NC, Wylie SA, and Abrahamse EL
- Subjects
- Animals, Corpus Striatum, Dopamine Agents therapeutic use, Humans, Inhibition, Psychological, Dopamine, Parkinson Disease drug therapy
- Abstract
Parkinson's disease (PD) is a neurological disorder associated primarily with overt motor symptoms. Several studies show that PD is additionally accompanied by impairments in covert cognitive processes underlying goal-directed motor functioning (e.g., action planning, conflict adaptation, inhibition), and that dopaminergic medication may modulate these action control components. In this review we aim to leverage findings from studies in this domain to elucidate the role of dopamine (DA) in action control. A qualitative review of studies that investigated the effects of medication status (on vs. off) on action control in PD suggests a component-specific role for DA in action control, although the expression of medication effects depends on characteristics of both the patients and experimental tasks used to measure action control. We discuss these results in the light of findings from other research lines examining the role of DA in action control (e.g., animal research, pharmacology), and recommend that future studies use multi-method, within-subject approaches to model DA effects on action control across different components as well as underlying striatal pathways (ventral vs. dorsal)., (Copyright © 2021 The Author(s). Published by Elsevier Ltd.. All rights reserved.)
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
18. Cognition in Motion: Evidence for Intact Action Control With Healthy Aging.
- Author
-
Ruitenberg MFL and Koppelmans V
- Subjects
- Adult, Age Factors, Aged, 80 and over, Bayes Theorem, Executive Function, Female, Humans, Male, Problem Solving, Reaction Time, Cognition physiology, Healthy Aging physiology, Healthy Aging psychology, Motor Skills, Physical Functional Performance, Psychomotor Performance
- Abstract
Objectives: Healthy aging is associated with impairments in motor functioning. Such functioning is not limited to the physical execution of actions, but also involves cognitive processes that allow for goal-directed behavior. The present study examined whether aging affects 2 of such cognitive components that control motor functioning, namely action planning and action adaptation, and whether age effects are associated across components., Method: A group of 103 participants aged 18-82 years performed 2 tasks that have previously been linked to action planning and adaptation, respectively., Results: Despite observations that aging was associated with slower and less accurate responses, Bayesian models showed evidence indicating that older age was not associated with poorer action planning and conflict adaptation., Discussion: These findings challenge the view that healthy aging is associated with a general deficit in motor functioning and suggest that some cognitive aspects of motor control may be relatively spared., (© The Author(s) 2020. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of The Gerontological Society of America.)
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
19. Cognitive and Affective Theory of Mind in Healthy Aging.
- Author
-
Ruitenberg MFL, Santens P, and Notebaert W
- Subjects
- Adolescent, Adult, Aged, Female, Humans, Male, Middle Aged, Reaction Time, Young Adult, Aging psychology, Cognition, Healthy Aging, Theory of Mind
- Abstract
Background: Previous studies on the effect of healthy aging on Theory of Mind (ToM) have produced mixed results. A possible explanation may be that different ToM components and types of inference have not systematically been considered. This study examined the effect of aging on ToM by assessing both first and second order cognitive and affective components within a single task., Methods: We compared performance of young ( M = 18.3y) and older adults ( M = 61.0y) on the Yoni task. This task allows for a within-subject assessment of both first and second order cognitive and affective ToM., Results: We observed that older adults had longer reaction times than young adults across cognitive and affective first order items. For second order items, this age difference was larger for affective than cognitive items. Results showed no indications that these findings could be explained by age differences in speed/accuracy trade-offs., Conclusion: Our findings suggest that decision processes underlying ToM are slower in older adults on both first and second order inferences, but that age differences in these processes between cognitive and affective ToM are selective to second order inferences. We propose that the observed age differences may be associated with cortical and mental changes that occur with aging.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
20. Neural Dedifferentiation across the Lifespan in the Motor and Somatosensory Systems.
- Author
-
Cassady K, Ruitenberg MFL, Reuter-Lorenz PA, Tommerdahl M, and Seidler RD
- Subjects
- Adult, Aged, Female, Functional Neuroimaging, Humans, Magnetic Resonance Imaging, Male, Middle Aged, Neural Pathways, Young Adult, Aging physiology, Brain physiology, Psychomotor Performance physiology, Touch Perception physiology
- Abstract
Age-related declines in sensorimotor performance have been linked to dedifferentiation of neural representations (i.e., more widespread activity during task performance in older versus younger adults). However, it remains unclear whether changes in neural representations across the adult lifespan are related between the motor and somatosensory systems, and whether alterations in these representations are associated with age declines in motor and somatosensory performance. To investigate these issues, we collected functional magnetic resonance imaging and behavioral data while participants aged 19-76 years performed a visuomotor tapping task or received vibrotactile stimulation. Despite one finding indicative of compensatory mechanisms with older age, we generally observed that 1) older age was associated with greater activity and stronger positive connectivity within sensorimotor and LOC regions during both visuomotor and vibrotactile tasks; 2) increased activation and stronger positive connectivity were associated with worse performance; and 3) age differences in connectivity in the motor system correlated with those in the somatosensory system. Notwithstanding the difficulty of disentangling the relationships between age, brain, and behavioral measures, these results provide novel evidence for neural dedifferentiation across the adult lifespan in both motor and somatosensory systems and suggest that dedifferentiation in these two systems is related., (© The Author(s) 2020. Published by Oxford University Press. All rights reserved. For permissions, please e-mail: journals.permissions@oup.com.)
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
21. Learning to be in control involves response-specific mechanisms.
- Author
-
Ruitenberg MFL, Braem S, Du Cheyne H, and Notebaert W
- Subjects
- Adolescent, Attention physiology, Female, Humans, Male, Young Adult, Learning physiology, Photic Stimulation methods, Reaction Time immunology, Stroop Test
- Abstract
Conflict adaptation refers to our ability to modulate our attention in line with changing situational demands, so we can engage in goal-directed behavior. While there is ample evidence demonstrating that such adaptation in conflict tasks can be captured using different response modalities, it remains unknown whether these effects rely on domain-general mechanisms applied to different response modalities, or are the result of more inherently response-specific processes. Here, we used an individual-differences approach to evaluate whether conflict adaptation in two highly similar tasks using different response modalities are related. Specifically, participants performed two versions of a Stroop task, one in which they responded via key presses and one in which they responded via mouse movements. In both tasks, we manipulated the item-specific proportion of (in)congruent trials (80% vs. 20% congruent). This allowed us to evaluate the item-specific proportion congruency (ISPC) effect, a hallmark indicator of conflict adaptation. ISPC effects were observed in both response modalities. However, we found no indications that individual differences in the ISPC effects of the two response modalities were related. This raises the question whether findings from studies on conflict adaptation measured by different modalities can reliably be compared. Furthermore, these results suggest that response modality plays a more integrative role in these adaptive processes, rather than being the mere output of a domain-general control mechanism. This is consistent with contingency learning accounts of the ISPC effect and associative learning models of cognitive control.
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
22. Age-Related Reductions in Tactile and Motor Inhibitory Function Start Early but Are Independent.
- Author
-
Ruitenberg MFL, Cassady KE, Reuter-Lorenz PA, Tommerdahl M, and Seidler RD
- Abstract
Aging is associated with declines in motor and somatosensory function. Some of these motor declines have been linked to age-related reductions in inhibitory function. Here we examined whether tactile surround inhibition also changes with age and whether these changes are associated with those in the motor domain. We tested a group of 56 participants spanning a wide age range (18-76 years old), allowing us to examine when age differences emerge across the lifespan. Participants performed tactile and motor tasks that have previously been linked to inter- and intra-hemispheric inhibition in the somatosensory and motor systems. The results showed that aging is associated with reductions in inhibitory function in both the tactile and motor systems starting around 40 years of age; however, age effects in the two systems were not correlated. The independent effects of age on tactile and motor inhibitory function suggest that distinct mechanisms may underlie age-related reductions in inhibition in the somatosensory and motor systems.
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
23. Commentary: Age Differentiation within Gray Matter, White Matter, and between Memory and White Matter in an Adult Life Span Cohort.
- Author
-
Ruitenberg MFL and Cassady KE
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
24. The effect of dopaminergic medication on conflict adaptation in Parkinson's disease.
- Author
-
Ruitenberg MFL, Abrahamse EL, Santens P, and Notebaert W
- Subjects
- Aged, Dopamine Agents, Female, Humans, Intelligence Tests, Male, Middle Aged, Motor Skills drug effects, Neuropsychological Tests, Psychomotor Performance drug effects, Stroop Test, Adaptation, Psychological drug effects, Antiparkinson Agents adverse effects, Antiparkinson Agents therapeutic use, Conflict, Psychological, Dopamine Agonists adverse effects, Dopamine Agonists therapeutic use, Parkinson Disease drug therapy, Parkinson Disease psychology
- Abstract
Parkinson's disease (PD) is a neurological disorder associated primarily with motor symptoms such as tremor, slowness of movement, and difficulties with gait and balance. Most patients take dopaminergic medication to improve their motor functions. Previous studies reported indications that such medication can impair higher cognitive functions (cf. dopamine overdose hypothesis). In the present study, we examined the effect of medication status on conflict adaptation. PD patients performed a Stroop task in which we manipulated the proportion of congruent and incongruent items, thereby allowing us to explore conflict adaptation. The use of mouse movements allowed us to examine the action dynamics of conflict adaptation in PD, and their sensitivity to dopaminergic medication. Each patient performed the same task twice: once without making changes to their regular medication regime, and once after overnight withdrawal from their medication. Results showed that medication improved mouse movements and alleviated motor symptoms. Moreover, patients' mouse movements were modulated as a function of the proportion congruency manipulation, revealing conflict adaptation in PD, which was unaffected by medication status. The present study extends earlier work on conflict adaptation in PD where reduced transient (trial-by-trial) conflict adaptation was observed ON compared to OFF medication (Duthoo et al., 2013, Neuropsychology, 27, 556). Our findings suggest that more sustained cognitive control processes may not be sensitive to dopamine overdose effects., (© 2017 The British Psychological Society.)
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
25. Neural correlates of multi-day learning and savings in sensorimotor adaptation.
- Author
-
Ruitenberg MFL, Koppelmans V, De Dios YE, Gadd NE, Wood SJ, Reuter-Lorenz PA, Kofman I, Bloomberg JJ, Mulavara AP, and Seidler RD
- Subjects
- Adult, Behavior, Brain anatomy & histology, Brain physiology, Brain Mapping, Female, Humans, Magnetic Resonance Imaging, Male, Middle Aged, Task Performance and Analysis, Adaptation, Physiological, Learning physiology, Psychomotor Performance physiology
- Abstract
In the present study we evaluated changes in neural activation that occur over the time course of multiple days of sensorimotor adaptation, and identified individual neural predictors of adaptation and savings magnitude. We collected functional MRI data while participants performed a manual adaptation task during four separate test sessions over a three-month period. This allowed us to examine changes in activation and associations with adaptation and savings at subsequent sessions. Participants exhibited reliable savings of adaptation across the four sessions. Brain activity associated with early adaptation increased across the sessions in a variety of frontal, parietal, cingulate, and temporal cortical areas, as well as various subcortical areas. We found that savings was positively associated with activation in several striatal, parietal, and cingulate cortical areas including the putamen, precuneus, angular gyrus, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC), and cingulate motor area. These findings suggest that participants may learn how to better engage cognitive processes across days, potentially reflecting improvements in action selection. We propose that such improvements may rely on action-value assignments, which previously have been linked to the dACC and striatum. As correct movements are assigned a higher value than incorrect movements, the former are more likely to be performed again.
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
26. Multi-day Adaptation and Savings in Manual and Locomotor Tasks.
- Author
-
Ruitenberg MFL, De Dios YE, Gadd NE, Wood SJ, Reuter-Lorenz PA, Kofman I, Bloomberg JJ, Mulavara AP, and Seidler RD
- Subjects
- Adult, Female, Humans, Male, Memory, Short-Term physiology, Middle Aged, Visual Fields physiology, Adaptation, Physiological physiology, Individuality, Learning physiology, Locomotion physiology, Psychomotor Performance physiology
- Abstract
Using an individual differences approach, we evaluated whether manual and locomotor adaptation are associated in terms of adaptation and savings across days, and whether they rely on shared underlying mechanisms involving visuospatial working memory or visual field dependence. Participants performed a manual and a locomotor adaptation task during 4 separate test sessions over a 3-month period. Reliable adaptation and savings were observed for both tasks. It was further found that higher visuospatial working memory performance and lower visual field dependence scores were associated with faster learning in the manual and locomotor tasks, respectively. Moreover, adaptation rates were correlated between the 2 tasks in the final test session, suggesting that people may gradually be learning something generalizable about the adaptation process.
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
27. Early and late indications of item-specific control in a Stroop mouse tracking study.
- Author
-
Bundt C, Ruitenberg MFL, Abrahamse EL, and Notebaert W
- Subjects
- Adolescent, Adult, Biomechanical Phenomena, Executive Function, Female, Hand, Humans, Inhibition, Psychological, Male, Reaction Time, Self-Control, Stroop Test, Young Adult, Conflict, Psychological, Motor Activity
- Abstract
Previous studies indicated that cognitive conflict continues to bias actions even after a movement has been initiated. The present paper examined whether cognitive control also biases actions after movement initiation. To this end, we had participants perform a Stroop task in which we manipulated the item-specific proportion of (in)congruent trials (80% congruent vs. 20% congruent). Importantly, participants responded via mouse movements, allowing us to evaluate various movement parameters: initiation times, movement times, and movement accuracy. Results showed that mouse movements were faster and more accurate during congruent trials compared to incongruent trials. Moreover, we observed that this congruency effect was larger for 80% congruent compared to 20% congruent items, which reflects item-specific cognitive control. Notably, when responses were initiated very fast - rendering virtually no time for stimulus processing before movement onset - this item-specific control was observed only in movement times. However, for relatively slow initiated responses, item specific control was observed both in initiation and in movement times. These findings demonstrate that item-specific cognitive control biases actions before and after movement initiation., Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
28. Impulsivity in Parkinson's Disease Is Associated With Alterations in Affective and Sensorimotor Striatal Networks.
- Author
-
Ruitenberg MFL, Wu T, Averbeck BB, Chou KL, Koppelmans V, and Seidler RD
- Abstract
A subset of patients with Parkinson's disease (PD) experiences problems with impulse control, characterized by a loss of voluntary control over impulses, drives, or temptations regarding excessive hedonic behavior. The present study aimed to better understand the neural basis of such impulse control disorders (ICDs) in PD. We collected resting-state functional connectivity and structural MRI data from 21 PD patients with ICDs and 30 patients without such disorders. To assess impulsivity, all patients completed the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale and performed an information-gathering task. MRI results demonstrated substantial differences in neural characteristics between PD patients with and without ICDs. Results showed that impulsivity was linked to alterations in affective basal ganglia circuitries. Specifically, reduced frontal-striatal connectivity and GPe volume were associated with more impulsivity. We suggest that these changes affect decision making and result in a preference for risky or inappropriate actions. Results further showed that impulsivity was linked to alterations in sensorimotor striatal networks. Enhanced connectivity within this network and larger putamen volume were associated with more impulsivity. We propose that these changes affect sensorimotor processing such that patients have a greater propensity to act. Our findings suggest that the two mechanisms jointly contribute to impulsive behaviors in PD.
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
29. Sequence learning in Parkinson's disease: Focusing on action dynamics and the role of dopaminergic medication.
- Author
-
Ruitenberg MFL, Duthoo W, Santens P, Seidler RD, Notebaert W, and Abrahamse EL
- Subjects
- Aged, Biomechanical Phenomena, Female, Hand physiopathology, Humans, Learning physiology, Male, Middle Aged, Motor Skills physiology, Neuropsychological Tests, Parkinson Disease physiopathology, Parkinson Disease psychology, Antiparasitic Agents therapeutic use, Dopamine Agents therapeutic use, Learning drug effects, Motor Skills drug effects, Parkinson Disease drug therapy
- Abstract
Previous studies on movement sequence learning in Parkinson's disease (PD) have produced mixed results. A possible explanation for the inconsistent findings is that some studies have taken dopaminergic medication into account while others have not. Additionally, in previous studies the response modalities did not allow for an investigation of the action dynamics of sequential movements as they unfold over time. In the current study we investigated sequence learning in PD by specifically considering the role of medication status in a sequence learning task where mouse movements were performed. The focus on mouse movements allowed us to examine the action dynamics of sequential movement in terms of initiation time, movement time, movement accuracy, and velocity. PD patients performed the sequence learning task once on their regular medication, and once after overnight withdrawal from their medication. Results showed that sequence learning as reflected in initiation times was impaired when PD patients performed the task ON medication compared to OFF medication. In contrast, sequence learning as reflected in the accuracy of movement trajectories was enhanced when performing the task ON compared to OFF medication. Our findings suggest that while medication enhances execution processes of movement sequence learning, it may at the same time impair planning processes that precede actual execution. Overall, the current study extends earlier findings on movement sequence learning in PD by differentiating between various components of performance, and further refines previous dopamine overdose effects in sequence learning., (Copyright © 2016 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.)
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.