122 results on '"Richard L. Wiener"'
Search Results
2. What’s reasonable? An experimental test of the reasonable officer standard
- Author
-
Trace C. Vardsveen and Richard L. Wiener
- Subjects
Sociology and Political Science ,Social Psychology ,Law - Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
3. Public support for sentencing reform: A policy-capturing experiment
- Author
-
Richard L. Wiener and Trace C. Vardsveen
- Subjects
Retributive justice ,Punishment ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Prison ,Policy capturing ,Criminals ,Judgment ,Policy ,Harm ,Humans ,Deterrence (legal) ,Crime ,Psychology ,Social psychology ,media_common ,Adjudication ,Criminal justice - Abstract
While research has shown magnitude of harm drives punishment decisions for crimes resulting in a prison sentence, many states impose probation rather than incarceration. A two-session experiment investigated how punishment type influences sentence length decisions. In session 1,347 participants answered online questions about their support for punishment justifications (i.e., retribution, deterrence, incapacitation, and rehabilitation). In session 2, the online participants read a randomly assigned scenario about a clerk who stole either a smaller or larger amount of money from his employer (magnitude of harm), which the employer was either likely or unlikely to detect (detection), and the clerk received either a term of prison or probation (type of punishment). Results revealed that magnitude of harm influenced punishment severity and sentence length judgments despite participants' self-reported support for retribution as a justification showing no influence. Punishment type also affected sentence length decisions. Furthermore, punishment severity judgments mediated the effect of the magnitude of harm on sentence length after controlling for punishment justifications but only in the probation condition, showing demand for harsher punishment was greater for probation. Thus, we concluded that the retribution motive is prevalent if offenders with a more severe crime receive probation rather than a prison sentence. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
4. Affective forecasting and ex-offender hiring decisions
- Author
-
Colin P. Holloway and Richard L. Wiener
- Subjects
education.field_of_study ,Social Psychology ,Recidivism ,Affective forecasting ,media_common.quotation_subject ,05 social sciences ,Population ,050109 social psychology ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Sample (statistics) ,050105 experimental psychology ,Criminal history ,Feeling ,Positive emotion ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Psychology ,education ,Negative emotion ,Social psychology ,media_common - Abstract
With over 600,000 Americans released from jails and prisons each year, it is increasingly important to understand the challenges that ex-offenders face when they attempt to reintegrate into society. Prior recidivism data indicate that over 80% of this population were rearrested within a decade of release, suggesting that the current social and economic environment is not conducive to successful reentry. The current research employs a simulation paradigm to explore how affective forecasting might predict hiring decisions for applicants with a criminal history. Using an online sample of adults (N = 322), an experimental hiring paradigm examined mock employment decisions for Black versus White applicants with or without a criminal history by measuring the degree to which applicant characteristics determined mock employer emotional forecasts and how those forecasts subsequently mediated simulated hiring outcomes. As expected, mock employers forecasted more intense negative emotion and less intense positive emotion when considering applicants who had a criminal history, and those forecasts predicted less favorable mock hiring outcomes for ex-offender applicants. Additionally, consistent with affective forecasting research, results showed that participants over-estimated the intensity of their negative emotion by forecasting more intense negative feelings than they experienced upon learning about the simulated hiring outcome. These findings suggest that evaluators generate emotional expectations about hiring applicants with a criminal history, which in turn account for their employment recommendations. Further, when evaluators utilize negative emotional predictions in a hiring decision they are likely relying on inaccurate information.
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
5. Exoffender housing stigma and discrimination
- Author
-
Megan C. Berry and Richard L. Wiener
- Subjects
Competence (law) ,Social discrimination ,Sociology and Political Science ,Social Psychology ,Criminal record ,Housing discrimination ,Stigma (botany) ,Professional competence ,Psychology ,Law ,Social psychology - Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
6. Custody Judgments, Ex‐offender Parents, and Best Interests of the Child
- Author
-
Richard L. Wiener and Julie Wertheimer
- Subjects
General Social Sciences ,Management, Monitoring, Policy and Law ,Criminology ,Best interests ,Psychology - Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
7. Criminal History, Sex, and Employment: Sex Differences in Ex‐Offender Hiring Stigma
- Author
-
Richard L. Wiener and Colin P. Holloway
- Subjects
General Social Sciences ,Stigma (botany) ,Management, Monitoring, Policy and Law ,Criminology ,Psychology ,Criminal history - Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
8. The Influence of Offender Race, Risk Level, and Participant Emotion on Juvenile Probation Case Judgments
- Author
-
Taylor Petty and Richard L. Wiener
- Subjects
Race (biology) ,Risk level ,General Social Sciences ,Juvenile ,Management, Monitoring, Policy and Law ,Psychology ,Developmental psychology - Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
9. The public's judgment of sex trafficked women: Blaming the victim?
- Author
-
Richard L. Wiener, Megan C. Berry, Jasmine Martinez, Julie Wertheimer, and Taylor Petty
- Subjects
Stereotyping ,Apprehension ,Sex trafficking ,Contempt ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Emotions ,Law enforcement ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Stereotype ,Morals ,humanities ,Police ,United States ,Competence (law) ,Blame ,Judgment ,medicine ,Humans ,Female ,medicine.symptom ,Psychology ,Social psychology ,media_common ,Sex work - Abstract
A victim-centered approach to fighting sex trafficking can result in apprehension and prosecution of traffickers and offer needed services to survivors. However, law enforcement officers frequently arrest sex-trafficking survivors for prostitution in accordance with state law. This study examined the psychology of public reactions and judgments of sex-trafficking survivors demonstrating the importance of situational factors, cognitive stereotypes, and moral emotions. Using Stereotype Content Modeling to measure the stereotypes that 762 community members held about prostitutes, we found shared stereotypes that were low in competence (i.e., capable and skilled) and warmth (i.e., good-natured and friendly). These participants later read modified case facts from United States v. Bell (United States v. Bell, 761 F.3d (8th Cir. 2014)) that varied survivor history of prostitution, vulnerability, and prostitution as a subsequent livelihood. Participants who stereotyped prostitutes as low in warmth and competence were the ones most certain police should arrest the survivor. Moral emotion analyses further showed that a survivor with no prior prostitution history and who came from a nonvulnerable background invoked disgust and contempt, which predicted a higher certainty of the arrest. Moral emotions fully mediated the relationship between the interacting case facts and arrest certainty for the trafficking survivor. Future directions and policy implications are discussed. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).
- Published
- 2021
10. The objective prong in sexual harassment: What is the standard?
- Author
-
Richard L. Wiener and Trace C. Vardsveen
- Subjects
Adult ,Employment ,Interview ,050109 social psychology ,Social Environment ,Reasonable person ,Judgment ,Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,Humans ,Interpersonal Relations ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Objectification ,Social Behavior ,Workplace ,General Psychology ,0505 law ,Social perception ,05 social sciences ,Psychiatry and Mental health ,Sexualization ,Sexual Harassment ,Social Perception ,Vignette ,050501 criminology ,Harassment ,Female ,Sexual objectification ,Psychology ,Law ,Social psychology - Abstract
In Title VII sexual harassment jurisprudence, U.S. courts use a 2-prong subjective-objective test to determine the viability of a sexual harassment claim: The complainant must show that the employer's conduct was unwelcome and sufficiently severe or pervasive to alter the conditions of employment and create an abusive working environment because of the complainant's sex from both the complainant's perspective (subjective prong) and a reasonable person's perspective (objective prong). This online study used a diverse national sample (361 MTurk Community Members) to investigate whether people apply the objective prong in a uniform manner, as the law assumes, or show predictable differences. Participants read a vignette about a female interviewee's allegations of sexual harassment following from severe, mild, or no sexual objectification by a male interviewer during a job interview. The interviewee claimed that she was either harassed or not by the interviewer during the interaction, as well as claiming to enjoy or reject sexualization. Participants made judgments about whether the interviewer's behavior was sexually harassing from the interviewee's and a reasonable person's perspective. Overall, participants' sex and enjoyment of sexualization moderated their judgments of sexual harassment when considering the situation from both points of view, demonstrating that there is no convergence on a unified standard for evaluating whether specific behavior is sexually harassing. Drawing comparisons to obscenity law, we argue that the use of data to form social fact evidence may help decision makers in hostile work environment cases to apply a more uniform understanding of what is hostile and abusive. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2018 APA, all rights reserved).
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
11. Abuse history and culpability judgments: Implications for battered spouse syndrome
- Author
-
Colin P. Holloway and Richard L. Wiener
- Subjects
Sociology and Political Science ,Social Psychology ,Battered spouse syndrome ,05 social sciences ,050501 criminology ,Domestic violence ,Criminology ,Self defense ,Psychology ,Law ,0505 law ,Culpability - Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
12. Validation and Application of the LS/CMI in Nebraska Probation
- Author
-
Trace C. Vardsveen, Richard L. Wiener, Alisha Caldwell Jimenez, and R. Hazel Delgado
- Subjects
Predictive validity ,Race ethnicity ,Level of service ,05 social sciences ,Applied psychology ,Case management ,Pathology and Forensic Medicine ,050501 criminology ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Psychology ,Law ,General Psychology ,050104 developmental & child psychology ,0505 law - Abstract
This study examined the validity of the Level of Service/Case Management Inventory (LS/CMI), as probation officers in the state of Nebraska use the tool. Study 1 evaluated the predictive validity of the LS/CMI by examining 19,344 probationer records over a 5.5-year period (January 2007-July 2013), and found that the LS/CMI total risk score demonstrated moderate predictive validity. Consistent with past findings, logistic regression showed that the total risk score predicted recidivism (return to probation) differently for nonminorities than for minorities. Furthermore, minorities scored higher than nonminorities on seven of the eight criminogenic factors. Study 2, a true randomized experiment, explored probation officer bias as an explanation for these findings, and found that training increased officers’ ratings of scores in some LS/CMI domains and decreased ratings in others. Most importantly, there was no evidence that officers demonstrated racial bias in administering the LS/CMI survey when scoring Black versus White target clients.
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
13. How old is old in allegations of age discrimination? The limitations of existing law
- Author
-
Richard L. Wiener and Katlyn S. Farnum
- Subjects
Decision Making ,Legislation as Topic ,050109 social psychology ,PsycINFO ,Stereotype content model ,Ageism ,Competence (law) ,Judgment ,Race (biology) ,Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,Civil Rights ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,General Psychology ,0505 law ,Plaintiff ,Judicial Role ,05 social sciences ,Age Factors ,Causality ,Supreme court ,Psychiatry and Mental health ,Law ,050501 criminology ,Psychology ,Decision model ,Social psychology - Abstract
Under Title VII, courts may give a mixed motive instruction allowing jurors to determine that defendants are liable for discrimination if an illegal factor (here: race, color, religion, sex, or national origin) contributed to an adverse decision. Recently, the Supreme Court held that to conclude that an employer discriminated against a worker because of age, the Age Discrimination in Employment Act, unlike Title VII of the Civil Rights Act of 1964, requires "but for" causality, necessitating jurors to find that age was the determinative factor in an employer's adverse decision regarding that worker. Using a national online sample (N = 392) and 2 study phases, 1 to measure stereotypes, and a second to present experimental manipulations, this study tested whether older worker stereotypes as measured through the lens of the Stereotype Content Model, instruction type (but for vs. mixed motive causality), and plaintiff age influenced mock juror verdicts in an age discrimination case. Decision modeling in Phase 2 with 3 levels of case orientation (i.e., proplaintiff, prodefendant, and neutral) showed that participants relied on multiple factors when making a decision, as opposed to just 1, suggesting that mock jurors favor a mixed model approach to discrimination verdict decisions. In line with previous research, instruction effects showed that mock jurors found in favor of plaintiffs under mixed motive instructions but not under "but for" instructions especially for older plaintiffs (64- and 74-year-old as opposed to 44- and 54-year-old-plaintiffs). Most importantly, in accordance with the Stereotype Content Model theory, competence and warmth stereotypes moderated the instruction effects found for specific judgments. The results of this study show the importance of the type of legal causality required for age discrimination cases. (PsycINFO Database Record
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
14. Equal Protection versus Free Speech Rights: When Gains Loom Larger than Losses
- Author
-
Richard L. Wiener and Katherine M. K. Kimble
- Subjects
Property (philosophy) ,Punishment ,LOOM ,media_common.quotation_subject ,05 social sciences ,050109 social psychology ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Context (language use) ,050105 experimental psychology ,Intimidation ,Free speech ,Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,Argument ,Prospect theory ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Psychology ,Social psychology ,computer ,media_common ,computer.programming_language - Abstract
Summary This paper examines the tension between equal protection and free speech in the hate speech context through a prospect theory lens. Two hundred and fifty-four participants recruited through Amazon's Mechanical Turk read a First Amendment free speech or Fourteenth Amendment equal protection argument framed to endorse the protections gained by each right, the losses avoided by each right, or the security provided by each right. Results showed gain-framing was more persuasive than loss-framing. Participant race and constitutional principle influenced punishment invoked for cross burning but not destruction of property or trespassing. Participants who received a positive framed equal protection argument believed the target would experience stronger negative emotions, particularly under low intimidation. Furthermore, participants receiving a positive frame speech argument believed the target would be less willing to support suppression. Unlike previous research, which suggests an equal protection principle default, this study demonstrated an impact of framed statements on decisions.Copyright © 2016 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
15. Do You See What I See? The Consequences of Objectification in Work Settings for Experiencers and Third Party Predictors
- Author
-
Jill Allen, Katlyn S. Farnum, Sarah J. Gervais, Richard L. Wiener, and Katherine M. K. Kimble
- Subjects
Third party ,Affective forecasting ,05 social sciences ,General Social Sciences ,050109 social psychology ,Management, Monitoring, Policy and Law ,Social issues ,050105 experimental psychology ,Work performance ,Harassment ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Objectification ,Sexual objectification ,Psychology ,Practical implications ,Social psychology - Abstract
Sexual objectification is a significant problem that permeates all areas of women's lives including the workplace. This research examines the impact of sexual objectification on women in work settings by integrating objectification, sexual harassment, and affective forecasting theories. We used a laboratory analogue that included undergraduate women who actually experienced objectification during a work interview (i.e., experiencers) and third-party predictors (including female and male undergraduates as well as female and male community workers) who anticipated the effects of objectification (i.e., predictors). We measured actual and anticipated emotions, performance, and sexual harassment following objectification. We found that both mild and severe objectification caused weaker positive affect, stronger negative affect, worse work performance, and higher sexual harassment judgments, but these effects were primarily driven by predictors anticipating worse outcomes following objectification compared to what experiencers actually reported. We also found that experiencers’ responses to objectification were moderated by benevolent sexism with women lower in benevolent sexism responding more similarly to predictors relative to women higher in benevolent sexism. Both experiencers and predictors evaluated interviewers who engaged in objectification equally negatively. Finally, we explored differences between predictors who were female and male undergraduate students versus community workers and found that these parties anticipated different consequences, depending on worker status and gender. Implications for sexual objectification, sexual harassment, and affective forecasting theories as well as practical implications for policy and law are discussed.
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
16. Stereotype Content Model, Causal Models, and Allegations of Age Discrimination: Should the Law Change?
- Author
-
Katlyn S. Farnum and Richard L. Wiener
- Subjects
Plaintiff ,media_common.quotation_subject ,05 social sciences ,General Social Sciences ,050109 social psychology ,Management, Monitoring, Policy and Law ,Stereotype content model ,Causality ,Supreme court ,Competence (law) ,Action (philosophy) ,Jury ,Law ,0502 economics and business ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Psychology ,Social psychology ,050203 business & management ,media_common ,Causal model - Abstract
In 2009, the Supreme Court decided, in Gross v. FBL, that the law governing age discrimination should be interpreted in a stricter manner than before. After Gross, age discrimination claims are tried under but for causality in which the plaintiff's age must be the direct cause of the adverse action, as opposed to mixed motive causality allowable under Title VII, in which age would only have to be a motivating factor. Previous research has shown that but for instructions lead to more pro-defendant verdicts, regardless of case strength, as compared to mixed motive instructions. This study reports on a simulated jury experiment that sought to uncover the influence of stereotypes concerning older workers on juror verdicts in an age discrimination case. Older worker stereotypes were assessed using the Stereotype Content Model's warmth and competence dimensions. In line with previous research, participants were more likely to find for the defendant under but for instructions, as compared to mixed motive. Further, mock jurors’ stereotypes predicted their verdicts, but only under the but for instructions, suggesting that jurors rely on stereotypes when they are limited in the case facts they can consider. Implications for policy changes and future research directions are discussed.
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
17. Dehumanization of older people: The evaluation of hostile work environments
- Author
-
Richard L. Wiener, Sarah J. Gervais, Gwenith D. Nuss, and Ena Brnjic
- Subjects
Derogation ,Sociology and Political Science ,Social Psychology ,Interview ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Hostility ,Affect (psychology) ,Dehumanization ,Perception ,medicine ,medicine.symptom ,Older people ,Path analysis (statistics) ,Psychology ,Law ,Social psychology ,media_common - Abstract
This research examined how people judge hostile work environments in which members of a work setting derogate other workers because they are older and presumed to be incompetent based on a psycholegal model incorporating negative affect and dehumanization (Wiener, Gervais, Brnjic, & Nuss, 2014). Specifically, we conducted a study in which a national sample of community participants read a scenario in which an interviewer did or did not derogate an older worker in a work interview. Respondents predicted affect (negative and positive), rated dehumanization (animalistic and mechanistic), and assessed hostile work environment (legal and impact). Consistent with hypotheses, derogation of the older worker caused more negative affect, more animalistic and mechanistic dehumanization, and more hostile work environment perceptions. Further, consistent with the mechanisms posited by our psycholegal model, path analysis revealed that negative affect and mechanistic dehumanization explained the relation between age derogation and hostile work environment judgments. The paper ends with a discussion of implications for organizations, law, and psychology, as well as directions for future research.
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
18. Anticipated affect and sentencing decisions in capital murder
- Author
-
Richard L. Wiener, Juan Cangas, and Leah C. Georges
- Subjects
Sociology and Political Science ,Social Psychology ,Homicide ,Affective forecasting ,Capital (economics) ,Legal Decisions ,Criminology ,Psychology ,Affect (psychology) ,Law ,Adjudication - Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
19. The psychology of jury decision making in age discrimination claims
- Author
-
Richard L. Wiener and Katlyn S. Farnum
- Subjects
Sociology and Political Science ,Social Psychology ,Jury ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Psychology ,Law ,Social psychology ,media_common ,Age discrimination - Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
20. Eye of the beholder: Effects of perspective and sexual objectification on harassment judgments
- Author
-
Sarah J. Gervais, Richard L. Wiener, Jill Allen, and Allissa Marquez
- Subjects
Sociology and Political Science ,Social Psychology ,Affective forecasting ,Perspective (graphical) ,Developmental psychology ,Reciprocity (social psychology) ,Psychological Theory ,Harassment ,Sexual objectification ,Objectification ,Psychology ,Law ,Social psychology ,Job interview - Abstract
Social Analytic Jurisprudence instructs researchers to study the reciprocal relations between law and peoples’ lives by developing empirical descriptions of legal assumptions about human behavior. The present work introduced a new experimental paradigm to test some of those assumptions by studying the impact of sexual objectification in a simulated job interview on performance, sexual harassment judgments, and emotions for women who experienced (akin to complainants), observed (akin to coworkers or witnesses), or predicted (akin to investigators, EEOC officers, mediators, jurors, or judges,) the impact of sexual objectification. Consistent with hypotheses, sexual objectification resulted in worse performance predictions, more sexual harassment, and more negative emotion compared with the control condition, but these effects were moderated by perspective. Overall, predictors estimated worse performance, more sexual harassment, and more negative emotion in response to sexual objectification than observers and experiencers. Anticipated negative emotion and self-referencing explained the effects for predictors. Theoretical and practical implications for legal and psychological theory on sexual harassment, objectification, and affective forecasting are discussed.
- Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
21. Gone Fishing: I–O Psychologists' Missed Opportunities to Understand Marginalized Employees' Experiences With Discrimination
- Author
-
Michelle R. Hebl, Cody B. Cox, Richard L. Wiener, Enrica N. Ruggs, Laura G. Barron, Charlie L. Law, and Mark V. Roehling
- Subjects
Legal protection ,Social Psychology ,business.industry ,Field (Bourdieu) ,Industrial and organizational psychology ,Public relations ,Workplace discrimination ,Psychology ,business ,Applied Psychology - Abstract
This article focuses attention on research examining workplace discrimination against employees from marginalized groups. We particularly consider the experiences of seven different groups of marginalized individuals, some of which have legal protection and some of which do not but all of whom we feel have been overlooked by the field of industrial–organizational (I–O) psychology. We briefly describe the importance of studying each group and then delineate the brief amount of research that has been conducted. Finally, we make recommendations for I–O psychologists in terms of research and advocacy. Overall, we argue that I–O psychologists are missing an opportunity to be at the forefront of understanding and instigating changes that would result in maximizing the fairness and optimization of these often forgotten employees and their experiences in the workplace.
- Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
22. Differences in the eyes of the beholders: The roles of subjective and objective judgments in sexual harassment claims
- Author
-
Katherine M. K. Kimble, Richard L. Wiener, Jill Allen, Katlyn S. Farnum, Gwenith D. Nuss, and Sarah J. Gervais
- Subjects
Employment ,Male ,media_common.quotation_subject ,050109 social psychology ,PsycINFO ,Reasonable person ,Developmental psychology ,Judgment ,Young Adult ,Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,Perception ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Objectification ,Students ,General Psychology ,0505 law ,media_common ,Affective forecasting ,05 social sciences ,Perspective (graphical) ,Psychiatry and Mental health ,Harm ,Sexual Harassment ,050501 criminology ,Harassment ,Female ,Psychology ,Law ,Social psychology - Abstract
In 2 studies, we found support for current sexual harassment jurisprudence. Currently, the courts use a 2-prong test to determine the viability of a sexual harassment claim: that the adverse treatment is sufficiently severe or pervasive to alter conditions of employment based on a protected class from the perspective of the individual complainant (subjective prong) and from the perspective of a reasonable person (objective prong). In Experiment 1, trained male undergraduate research assistants administered sequential objectifying gazes and comments to undergraduate female research participants. We found that the pervasive objectification delivered by multiple men (compared with 1 man) did not elicit more negative emotion or harm the experiencers' task performance, although it did lead them to make increased judgments of sexual harassment. In Experiment 2, observers (who viewed a recording of an experiencer's interactions with the male research assistants) and predictors (who read a protocol describing the facts of the interaction) anticipated the female targets would experience negative emotions, show impaired performance, as well as find more evidence in the interaction of sexual harassment. Observers' judgments mirrored those of the experiencers' while predictors' judgments demonstrated affective forecasting errors. Predictors were more likely to anticipate more negative emotion, worse performance, and greater likelihood of sexual harassment. Overall, these studies demonstrate the impact and importance of considering perceptions of sexual harassment from multiple perspectives and viewpoints. (PsycINFO Database Record
- Published
- 2016
23. The Angry Juror: Sentencing Decisions in First-Degree Murder
- Author
-
Leah C. Georges, Stacie R. Keller, and Richard L. Wiener
- Subjects
media_common.quotation_subject ,Human factors and ergonomics ,Poison control ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Anger ,Affect (psychology) ,Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,Homicide ,Injury prevention ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,Psychology ,Social psychology ,Sentence ,media_common ,Adjudication - Abstract
A great deal of research in legal decision making has overlooked the influence of affect on the decision-making process. The present study measured the fluctuation of emotions across five time points of a capital trial and tested the overall relationship between changes in emotion and sentencing decisions. The results showed that across all participants, anger initially increased and then decreased during the course of a capital punishment trial. Most importantly, the more individual mock jurors' anger increased during any stage of the trial, the more likely they were to assign a death sentence. Furthermore, when jurors' anger increased, they rated mitigating factors presented by the defense as weaker and the weaker mitigation mediated the relationship between change in anger and sentencing. The paper ends with a discussion of theoretical explanations and policy implications. Copyright © 2012 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Language: en
- Published
- 2012
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
24. Gender policing: Harassment judgments when men target other men
- Author
-
Richard L. Wiener, Nolt Nicholson, Sidney Bennett, and Carrie Cheloha
- Subjects
Sociology and Political Science ,Social Psychology ,Harassment ,Person–environment fit ,Gender distribution ,Human Males ,Psychology ,Law ,Social psychology ,Gender policing - Published
- 2012
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
25. Mock Jury Research: Where Do We Go from Here?
- Author
-
Daniel A. Krauss, Richard L. Wiener, and Joel D. Lieberman
- Subjects
Psychological research ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Validity ,Construct validity ,External validity ,Psychiatry and Mental health ,Clinical Psychology ,Jury ,Statistical conclusion validity ,Generalizability theory ,Internal validity ,Psychology ,Law ,Social psychology ,media_common - Abstract
This paper reviews the four types of validity that make up Cook and Campbell's traditional approach for social science research in general and psychological research in particular: internal validity, statistical conclusion validity, external validity, and construct validity. The most important generalizability threat to the validity of jury research is not likely a selection main effect (i.e., the effect of relying solely on undergraduate mock jurors) but is more likely the interaction of sample with construct validity factors. Researchers who try to capture the trial process with experimental paradigms may find that undergraduate mock jurors react differently to those efforts than do more representative community samples. We illustrate these issues with the seven papers that make up this volume, and conclude by endorsing Diamond's call for a two-stage research process in which findings with samples of convenience gradually add more realistic trial processes and representative samples to confirm the initial findings and increase the research program's credibility.
- Published
- 2011
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
26. Effects of Mortality Salience on Capital Punishment Sentencing Decisions
- Author
-
Richard L. Wiener and Megan Beringer Jones
- Subjects
Social Psychology ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Terror management theory ,Criminology ,humanities ,Jury ,Capital (economics) ,Mortality salience ,behavior and behavior mechanisms ,Capital punishment ,Psychology ,human activities ,Social psychology ,Applied Psychology ,media_common - Abstract
Despite the empirical success of Terror Management Theory (TMT) and the possibility that capital jurors may be unable to escape mortality salience effects, no studies have applied TMT to capital punishment trials. This research asked mock jurors to focus on their own death, the victim's death, or the defendant's death while reviewing a capital trial. Mock jurors who focused on the victim's or defendant's death (but not their own death) relied more on their attitudes toward the death penalty to make sentencing decisions than did other participants. These results raise questions about the usefulness of TMT in understanding capital jury decision making.
- Published
- 2011
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
27. Narrowly Tailored Actuarial Models for Affirmative Action in Higher Education
- Author
-
Richard L. Wiener and Evelyn M. Maeder
- Subjects
Affirmative action ,Consistency (negotiation) ,Actuarial science ,Higher education ,business.industry ,Selection (linguistics) ,General Social Sciences ,Management, Monitoring, Policy and Law ,Psychology ,business ,Supreme court ,Diversity (business) - Abstract
Previous research discusses the superiority of actuarial models over clinical models in a number of areas related to accuracy and consistency in decision making. The current project sought to develop an actuarial candidate selection model for affirmative action in higher education that would achieve the goal of diversity by assigning points for a number of diversity-related characteristics in addition to standard academic admission criteria. Two experiments showed that participants who used actuarial models selected applicants with more markers of academic success and greater diversity using factors favored by recent U.S. Supreme Court cases. The second experiment showed that an unweighted actuarial model also helped decision makers select more minority student candidates and that it produced higher ratings of procedural fairness. The article discusses how an actuarial model might pass Constitutional muster in light of recent Supreme Court cases.
- Published
- 2010
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
28. Dealing with mentally ill domestic violence perpetrators: A therapeutic jurisprudence judicial model
- Author
-
Bruce J. Winick, Aryn Emmert, Leah Skovran Georges, Richard L. Wiener, and Anthony Castro
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Domestic Violence ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Substance-Related Disorders ,Population ,Poison control ,Context (language use) ,Criminology ,Pathology and Forensic Medicine ,Mentally Ill Persons ,Dangerous Behavior ,Ambulatory Care ,medicine ,Humans ,Mental Competency ,Cooperative Behavior ,Psychiatry ,education ,Problem Solving ,Mental health court ,education.field_of_study ,Judicial Role ,business.industry ,Mental Disorders ,Socialization ,Public Assistance ,Therapeutic jurisprudence ,Mental illness ,medicine.disease ,United States ,Psychiatry and Mental health ,Intervention (law) ,Spouse Abuse ,Commitment of Mentally Ill ,Domestic violence ,Female ,Interdisciplinary Communication ,business ,Case Management ,Law - Abstract
People suffering from mental illness are increasingly referred to the domestic violence court. Yet the typical diversion programs available, including batterer's intervention programs, are inappropriate for those with serious mental illness. As a result, the Miami-Dade Domestic Violence Court has developed a new approach for dealing with this population that applies mental health court techniques in domestic violence court. This article will describe and discuss this pioneering model. It also will situate this model within the context of other problem-solving courts and discuss how the court uses principles and approaches of therapeutic jurisprudence. The paper presents some preliminary data that describe the social and legal characteristics of 20 defendants in the Domestic Violence Mental Health Court followed over a two year period between 2005 and 2007.
- Published
- 2010
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
29. A testable theory of problem solving courts: Avoiding past empirical and legal failures
- Author
-
Richard L. Wiener, Anthony Castro, Leah Skovran Georges, and Bruce J. Winick
- Subjects
Domestic Violence ,Substance-Related Disorders ,business.industry ,Process (engineering) ,Mental Disorders ,Emotions ,Socialization ,Procedural justice ,Therapeutic jurisprudence ,Outcome (game theory) ,United States ,Pathology and Forensic Medicine ,Psychiatry and Mental health ,Outcome and Process Assessment, Health Care ,Health care ,Humans ,Criticism ,Crime ,Sociology ,business ,Law ,Problem Solving ,Social cognitive theory ,Law and economics - Abstract
Recent years have seen a proliferation of problem solving courts designed to rehabilitate certain classes of offenders and thereby resolve the underlying problems that led to their court involvement in the first place. Some commentators have reacted positively to these courts, considering them an extension of the philosophy and logic of Therapeutic Jurisprudence, but others show concern that the discourse surrounding these specialty courts has not examined their process or outcomes critically enough. This paper examines that criticism from historical and social scientific perspectives. The analysis culminates in a model that describes how offenders are likely to respond to the process as they engage in problem solving court programs and the ways in which those courts might impact subsequent offender conduct. This Therapeutic Jurisprudence model of problem solving courts draws heavily on social cognitive psychology and more specifically on theories of procedural justice, motivation, and anticipated emotion to offer an explanation of how offenders respond to these programs. We offer this model as a lens through which social scientists can begin to address the concern that there is not enough critical analysis of the process and outcome of these courts. Applying this model to specialty courts constitutes an important step in critically examining the contribution of problem solving courts.
- Published
- 2010
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
30. Complainant behavioral tone, ambivalent sexism, and perceptions of sexual harassment
- Author
-
Erin M. Richter, Richard L. Wiener, Ryan J. Winter, Evelyn M. Maeder, Roni Reiter-Palmon, and Amy M. Humke
- Subjects
Plaintiff ,Sociology and Political Science ,Social Psychology ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Ambivalence ,Obedience ,Reasonable person ,Ambivalent sexism ,Perception ,Harassment ,Situational ethics ,Psychology ,Law ,Social psychology ,media_common - Abstract
Previous research has examined the impact of the law on decisions made about social sexual interactions in the workplace in the context of a variety of individual difference variables including gender of the observer and sexist attitudes, as well as situational factors including legal standard and prior exposure to aggressive and submissive complainants. The current study continued this line of inquiry by testing whether hostile or benevolent sexist attitudes behaved differently under manipulated exposure to aggressive and submissive complainants. Full-time workers watched 1 videotape in which aggressive, submissive, or neutral (i.e., businesslike) women complained that male coworkers sexually harassed them; then, participants viewed a second complainant who always acted in a neutral behavioral tone. In the first case, participants high in hostile sexism who took a reasonable person perspective (but not those with a reasonable woman point of view) and all men who viewed an aggressive complainant found less evidence of harassment. With the second set of allegations, female workers who were exposed to a submissive complainant in the first case found less evidence of harassment against the neutral complainant, suggesting that exposure to a submissive complainant triggered some type of victim blaming in female workers. Policy and training implications are discussed.
- Published
- 2010
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
31. Justice, Conflict and Wellbeing : Multidisciplinary Perspectives
- Author
-
Brian H. Bornstein, Richard L. Wiener, Brian H. Bornstein, and Richard L. Wiener
- Subjects
- Well-being, Justice, Social conflict, Social justice, Conflict (Psychology)
- Abstract
Justice, conflict and wellbeing are large topics that occupy researchers from a variety of disciplines, as well as laypeople and policy makers. The three concepts are closely connected: conflict often (though not always) impairs wellbeing, whereas justice often (though not always) enhances it; perceived injustice is a common source of conflict, at multiple levels and calls for justice are a common response to conflict. In addition, each construct has subtypes, such as distributive and procedural justice, individual and group conflict and physical and psychological wellbeing. Although there are established traditions of research on the topics in multiple disciplines, there is little cross-fertilization across disciplines. This volume brings together researchers from social, clinical and educational psychology; law and political science. The unifying theme is how injustice and conflict pose threats to wellbeing, at the micro (individual) and macro (groups and societies) levels. Multi- and interdisciplinary research are at the vanguard of science in the twenty-first century and the present work applies multi and interdisciplinary perspectives to the important real-world topics of justice, conflict and wellbeing.
- Published
- 2014
32. Does a Truck Driver See What a Nurse Sees? The Effects of Occupation Type on Perceptions of Sexual Harassment
- Author
-
Ryan J. Winter, Evelyn M. Maeder, and Richard L. Wiener
- Subjects
Gender Studies ,Social Psychology ,Occupation type ,Social perception ,Perception ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,Harassment ,Psychology ,Social psychology ,Truck driver ,media_common ,Clinical psychology - Abstract
Previous researchers have demonstrated differential prevalence of harassment of men and women in men’s traditional, women’s traditional, and neutral occupation types, but they have not looked at differences in harassment judgments among these occupations. Our hypotheses rely on the assumption that people who have observed frequent sexual harassment of others in their work environments react differently to new cases than do those who have witnessed fewer of these episodes in the past. Participants watched videos of two cases and made judgments about the presence or absence of sexual harassment. We categorized the participants based on their occupations—men’s traditional, women’s traditional, and neutral, and compared responses among groups. A gender by occupation type interaction emerged. Men in men’s traditional occupation types provided the highest ratings of harassment, followed by men in neutral occupations; men in women’s traditional occupation types provided the lowest ratings. Conversely, women in women’s traditional occupations provided the highest ratings of harassment, followed by women in neutral occupations; women in men’s traditional occupations provided the lowest ratings of sexual harassment.
- Published
- 2007
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
33. How people evaluate others with social anxiety disorder: A comparison to depression and general mental illness stigma
- Author
-
Kristin N. Anderson, Richard L. Wiener, Jordan A. Blenner, Andrew B. Jeon, and Debra A. Hope
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Social inhibition ,Social stigma ,Social Stigma ,Young Adult ,Prevalence of mental disorders ,Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,medicine ,Humans ,Sex Distribution ,Psychiatry ,Depressive Disorder, Major ,Stereotyping ,Social anxiety ,Mental illness ,medicine.disease ,Anxiety Disorders ,Psychiatry and Mental health ,Psychological Distance ,Schizophrenia ,Anxiety ,Major depressive disorder ,Regression Analysis ,Female ,Psychology (miscellaneous) ,medicine.symptom ,Psychology ,Clinical psychology - Abstract
Despite the availability of effective interventions, most individuals with social anxiety disorder do not seek treatment. Given their fear of negative evaluation, socially anxious individuals might be especially susceptible to stigma concerns, a recognized barrier for mental health treatment. However, very little is known about the stigma specific to social anxiety disorder. In a design similar to Feldman and Crandall (2007), university undergraduate students read vignettes about target individuals with a generic mental illness label, major depressive disorder, and social anxiety disorder. Subjects rated each of 3 people in the vignettes on social distance and 17 dimensions including dangerousness, heritability and prevalence of the disorder, and gender ratio. Results indicated that being male and not having experience with mental health treatment was associated with somewhat greater preferred social distance. Multiple regression analyses revealed that being embarrassed by the disorder and dangerousness predicted social distance across all 3 vignettes. The vignette for social anxiety disorder had the most complex model and included work impairment, more common among women, and more avoidable. These results have implications for understanding the specific aspects of the stigma associated with social anxiety disorder. Public service messages to reduce stigma should focus on more accurate information about dangerousness and mental illness, given this is an established aspect of mental illness stigma. More nuanced messages about social anxiety might be best incorporated into the treatment referral process and as part of treatment.
- Published
- 2015
34. Generic Prejudice in the Law: Sexual Assault and Homicide
- Author
-
Lucy Arnot, Brian Redmond, Richard L. Wiener, and Ryan J. Winter
- Subjects
Social Psychology ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Victimology ,Impartiality ,Criminology ,Cognitive bias ,Sexual abuse ,Jury ,Homicide ,Social cognition ,Psychology ,Social psychology ,Applied Psychology ,Prejudice (legal term) ,media_common - Abstract
This article distinguishes generic prejudice, prejudgments about classes of cases and defendants, from other forms of juror bias that undermine the impartiality principle of the Constitution. It develops an experimental method to study 2 types of generic prejudice (biases based upon specific charges, and biases based upon crime categories) and tests the role of cognitive resource theory and simple attention models in explaining when generic prejudice is most likely to be a threat to jury decisions. Results of 2 experiments using undergraduate research participants as mock jurors demonstrate that generic prejudice is more likely to be found in sexual assault cases relative to homicide cases, but that general adjudicative bias favoring the prosecution or the defense across crime categories is more likely to be found in homicide cases. There was some support for cognitive resource theory as an explanation for evaluations in sexual assault cases, and simple attention models for evaluations in homicide cases.
- Published
- 2006
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
35. Introduction to the Special Issue on Emotion in Legal Judgment and Decision Making
- Author
-
Richard L. Wiener and Brian H. Bornstein
- Subjects
Punitive damages ,Jury nullification ,Emotion work ,Legal psychology ,Psychiatry and Mental health ,Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,Damages ,Crime scene ,Psychology ,Legal practice ,Law ,Social psychology ,General Psychology ,Culpability - Abstract
In the last decade, researchers have shown burgeoning interest in issues at the intersection of emotion and law. Given the longstanding interest in emotion among social (and other) psychologists, most of this research has come from a psychological perspective, but it also includes work with a legal, sociological, philosophical, and neuroscience flavor. The issues are theoretical as well as practical, influencing both psychological theories of emotion and matters of legal practice and policy. The law adopts a double standard in its treatment of emotion. In some areas, the law explicitly addresses emotion as a legitimate consideration, but in other areas, the law denies emotion any role in legal decision making. For example, legal analysis requires decision makers to consider the emotional reactions of others when classifying certain offenses for purposes of criminal culpability (e.g., “hate crimes” and “crimes of passion”), awarding damages for emotional injuries (e.g., mental suffering and emotional distress), and allowing jurors’ moral judgments to influence certain consequential decisions such as punitive damages, capital sentencing, and jury nullification. At the same time, the courts make what may be untenable presumptions when they require factfinders to ignore their affective states and evaluate evidence, such as gory crime scene photographs, dispassionately. Thus, the field of law and emotion is ripe for scholarly exploration.
- Published
- 2006
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
36. The More You See It, the More You Know It: Memory Accessibility and Sexual Harassment Judgments
- Author
-
Ryan J. Winter, Richard L. Wiener, Amy M. Voss, and Lucy Arnot
- Subjects
Gender Studies ,Social Psychology ,Recall ,Availability heuristic ,Injury prevention ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,Harassment ,Poison control ,Human factors and ergonomics ,Psychology ,Social psychology ,Suicide prevention ,Occupational safety and health - Abstract
The influence of the availability of personally known and media known sexual harassment victims and harassers on perceptions of social sexual workplace conduct was examined. Male and female full-time workers evaluated two videotapes that depict sexual harassment in the workplace. Results indicated that perceived likelihood of harassment and discrimination increased as participants recalled more examples of harassment victims whom they personally knew. In some instances, recall of victims in the media had a similar influence. As predicted, the influence of availability was stronger for men than for women. Similar findings resulted from the analyses of the unwelcomeness, severity, and pervasiveness of the conduct; however, legal standard, gender, or case often moderated this relationship. The implications of these findings are discussed.
- Published
- 2005
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
37. Debtor education, financial literacy, and pending bankruptcy legislation
- Author
-
Karen Gross, Richard L. Wiener, Susan Block-Lieb, and Corinne Baron-Donovan
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Health Knowledge, Attitudes, Practice ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Financial Management ,New York ,Accounting ,Legislation ,Education ,Surveys and Questionnaires ,medicine ,Humans ,Psychiatry ,Bankruptcy ,business.industry ,Debtor ,Middle Aged ,Psychiatry and Mental health ,Clinical Psychology ,Balance (accounting) ,Educational Status ,Financial literacy ,Female ,business ,Psychology ,Law ,Program Evaluation - Abstract
This paper reports on an evaluation of a financial education-training program for residents of New York who had filed for bankruptcy. Over 400 individuals divided into three groups (trained debtors, untrained debtors, and non-debtors) completed identical questionnaires approximately three months apart. Trained debtors took the pretest before training and the post-test after training. Results revealed that trained debtors' financial knowledge increased after training compared with untrained and non-debtors. Trained debtors showed more negative attitudes towards unnecessary spending compared with the other two groups and reported less intention to buy than non-debtors reported. Self-reported behaviors showed significant changes in the desired direction for trained debtors' use of credit cards (i.e. number owned, purchases, and balance amount), paying bills, budgeting, and borrowing from predatory lenders. Implications for pending legislation are discussed. Copyright © 2005 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
- Published
- 2005
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
38. The death penalty in the United States: A crisis of conscience
- Author
-
Craig Haney and Richard L. Wiener
- Subjects
Sociology and Political Science ,Social Psychology ,Homicide ,Law ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Legal Decisions ,Capital punishment ,Psychology ,Conscience ,media_common - Published
- 2004
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
39. Death is different: An editorial introduction to the theme issue
- Author
-
Craig Haney and Richard L. Wiener
- Subjects
Sociology and Political Science ,Social Psychology ,Punishment ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Capital punishment ,Criminology ,Psychology ,Law ,media_common ,Theme (narrative) - Published
- 2004
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
40. Guided jury discretion in capital murder cases: The role of declarative and procedural knowledge
- Author
-
Ben Morasco, Shannon M. Rauch, Ryan J. Winter, Karen Kadela, Melanie Rogers, Linda E. Hurt, Amy A. Hackney, Hope Seib, Richard L. Wiener, and Laura Warren
- Subjects
Descriptive knowledge ,Sociology and Political Science ,Social Psychology ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Jurisprudence ,Deliberation ,Procedural knowledge ,Discretion ,Jury ,Law ,Jury instructions ,Psychology ,media_common ,Adjudication - Abstract
Saint Louis UniversityThis article analyzes whether state-approved jury instructions adequately guide jurydiscretion in the penalty phase of first-degree murder trials. It examines EighthAmendment jurisprudence regarding guided jury discretion, emphasizing the use of“empirical factors” to examine the quality of state-approved instructions. Psycho-logical research and testimony on the topic of the comprehensibility of juryinstructions are reviewed. Data from a recently completed simulation with 80deliberating juries showed that current instructions do not adequately convey theconcepts and processes essential to guiding penalty phase judgments. An additionalsimulation with 20 deliberating juries demonstrated that deliberation alone does notcorrect for jurors’ errors in comprehension. The article concludes with recommen-dations for policy and future research.
- Published
- 2004
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
41. The effects of prior workplace behavior on subsequent sexual harassment judgments
- Author
-
Melanie Rogers, Ryan J. Winter, Richard L. Wiener, and Lucy Arnot
- Subjects
Male ,Decision Making ,Poison control ,Federal Government ,Suicide prevention ,Occupational safety and health ,Sex Factors ,Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,Injury prevention ,Humans ,Workplace ,General Psychology ,Behavior ,Plaintiff ,Judicial Role ,Data Collection ,Human factors and ergonomics ,United States ,Legal psychology ,Psychiatry and Mental health ,Sexual Harassment ,Harassment ,Female ,Psychology ,Law ,Social psychology ,Prejudice - Abstract
A dual processing model of sexual harassment judgments predicted that the behavior of a complainant in a prior case would influence evaluations in an unrelated subsequent case. In the first of two experimental scenarios depicting social-sexual conduct at work, the female complainant's conduct was manipulated to be aggressive, submissive, ambiguous, or neutral. Half of the participants were asked to reflect upon the first scenario after reading it and before answering responsibility questions. The other half simply reviewed the scenario and answered the questions. When the complainant acted aggressively, her behavior in the first scenario caused men who reflected on the fact pattern to find less evidence of harassment. Most interestingly, an aggressive complainant observed in the first scenario caused participants (especially women) to rate lower the likelihood that a neutral complainant in a second independent case was the victim of gender discrimination. Across cases, men found less evidence of harassment than did women.
- Published
- 2004
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
42. Problem Solving Courts : Social Science and Legal Perspectives
- Author
-
Richard L. Wiener, Eve M. Brank, Richard L. Wiener, and Eve M. Brank
- Subjects
- Courts of special jurisdiction, Therapeutic jurisprudence
- Abstract
In order to make the criminal court system more effective there has been a growing trend to have courts participate in what is essentially a rehabilitation strategy. Such courts are often referred to as “problem-solving” because they are working on root causes of criminal behavior as part of the dispensation of justice. This major shift in the role of the courts means that the court works closely with prosecutors, public defenders, probation officers, social workers, and other justice system partners to develop a strategy that pressures offenders to complete a treatment program which will ultimately, hopefully prevent recidivism. Research has shown that this kind of strategy has a two-fold benefit. It has been successful in helping offenders turn their lives around which leads to improved public safety and the ultimate saving of public funds. This book is the first to focus exclusively on problem solving courts, and as such it presents an overview of the rationale and scientific evidence for such courts as well as individual sections on the key areas in which these courts are active. Thus there is specific attention paid to domestic violence, juvenile criminality, mental health, and more. Throughout, research findings are incorporated into general discussions of these courts operate and ideally what they are trying to accomplish. There is also discussion of how such courts should evolve in the future and the directions that further research should take.
- Published
- 2013
43. The psychology of telling murder stories: do we think in scripts, exemplars, or prototypes?
- Author
-
M.L.S. Richard L. Wiener Ph.D., Amy A. Hackney, Tracey L. Richmond, Shannon M. Rauch, and Hope M. Seib
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Poison control ,computer.software_genre ,Computer security ,Sociology ,Jury ,Memory ,Homicide ,Cluster Analysis ,Humans ,General knowledge ,Narrative ,media_common ,Analysis of Variance ,Jurisprudence ,Missouri ,Media studies ,Psychiatry and Mental health ,Clinical Psychology ,Scripting language ,Imagination ,Female ,Psychology ,Law ,computer ,Storytelling - Abstract
According to the story model of Pennington and Hastie, jurors collect information at trial and modify it with general knowledge to create case stories. Schank and Ableson argue that human memory is organized to tell and understand stories. However, Finkel and Groscup questioned the use of manipulated, experimenter-constructed narratives to demonstrate the existence of multiple prototypical crime stories. We interviewed 76 jury eligible, death qualified citizens and asked them to imagine a first-degree murder scenario, describing the events that led to the killing. We coded the presence of dichotomous variables in the resulting stories and identified at least three shared story prototypes using cluster and profile analysis. We conclude that people do not store crime stories as simple prototypes and comment on the implications of this finding for legal decision-making. Copyright © 2002 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
- Published
- 2002
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
44. Gender differences in evaluating social-sexual conduct in the workplace
- Author
-
Brenda L. Russell, Richard L. Wiener, R. Kelley Mannen, and Linda E. Hurt
- Subjects
media_common.quotation_subject ,Poison control ,Human factors and ergonomics ,Suicide prevention ,Occupational safety and health ,Psychiatry and Mental health ,Clinical Psychology ,Respondent ,Injury prevention ,Harassment ,Assertiveness ,Psychology ,Law ,Social psychology ,media_common - Abstract
Qualitative interviews exploring gender differences in perceptions of sexual harassment were conducted with 100 full-time St. Louis area employees. Women more than men reported that telling dirty/sexual jokes was a non-harassing behavior, qualified behaviors as harassing when they happened in the workplace, and considered behaviors as non-harassing when the man's intentions were not harmful. Men more than women reported that requesting a date was a non-harassing behavior, qualified behaviors as harassing when the woman did not welcome the behavior, and considered behaviors as non-harassing when they did not violate workplace norms. Logistic regression analysis predicted the respondent gender with 86% accuracy. Finally, concept mapping suggested that when women think about harassers they are concerned with power and social aptitude, while men seem to be more concerned about the responsibility and psychological adjustment of perpetrators of sexual harassment. When women think about victims of harassment they are concerned with a woman's assertiveness and work effectiveness, while men are more concerned with the psychological state of the woman and how provocative she is when they think about victims of sexual harassment.
- Published
- 1999
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
45. Advances in sexual harassment research, theory, and policy
- Author
-
Richard L. Wiener and Barbara A. Gutek
- Subjects
Sociology and Political Science ,Social Psychology ,Policy making ,Psychological research ,Law ,Legal Decisions ,Harassment ,Sociology ,Criminology ,Research findings ,Affect (psychology) ,Theme (narrative) - Abstract
The study of sexual harassment is an active and exciting arena for both legal scholars and psychologists and other social scientists. Legal rulings affect the course of psychological research, and research findings in turn inform legal scholars and the courts. This special theme issue brings togethe
- Published
- 1999
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
46. An interdisciplinary approach to understanding social sexual conduct at work
- Author
-
Linda E. Hurt and Richard L. Wiener
- Subjects
Sociology and Political Science ,Social Psychology ,Work (electrical) ,Applied psychology ,Legal Decisions ,Engineering ethics ,Psychology ,Law ,Adjudication - Published
- 1999
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
47. The Role of Declarative and Procedural Knowledge in Capital Murder Sentencing1
- Author
-
Craig A. Bauer, Melody S. Sadler, Tracy M. Sargent, Richard L. Wiener, Linda E. Hurt, and Susan L. Thomas
- Subjects
Descriptive knowledge ,Social Psychology ,Constitution ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Poison control ,Orderliness ,Procedural knowledge ,Law ,Capital (economics) ,Active listening ,Jury instructions ,Psychology ,Social psychology ,media_common - Abstract
This paper identifies rational orderliness and moral appropriateness as 2 norms that the United States Constitution requires for sentencing in capital murder trials. The courts convey these norms directly to jurors through jury instructions in the penalty phase of capital murder trials. To follow the instructions, jurors require accurate declarative knowledge (rules of law) and procedural knowledge (processes required to execute the rules) of state and federal sentencing law. Undergraduate mock jurors showed low accuracy for both types of knowledge after reviewing and listening to pattern jury instructions. Participants failed to offset aggravating factors with mitigating circumstances as the Missouri Approved Jury Instructions direct. The less knowledge that participants mastered about mitigation, the more certain they were of invoking the death penalty.
- Published
- 1998
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
48. Research report: A preliminary analysis of medical futility decisionmaking: law and professional attitudes
- Author
-
Sandra H. Johnson, Jesse A. Goldner, David T. Eton, Vincent P. Gibbons, and Richard L. Wiener
- Subjects
Government ,medicine.medical_specialty ,business.industry ,Poison control ,Human factors and ergonomics ,Judicial opinion ,Suicide prevention ,Occupational safety and health ,Psychiatry and Mental health ,Clinical Psychology ,Family medicine ,Respondent ,Health care ,medicine ,Psychology ,business ,Law - Abstract
Judicial decisions reviewed in this article indicate that courts have taken two disparate approaches to disputes over futility of treatment. To explore whether a consensus on medical futility is developing among hospitals, the authors conducted a nationwide survey of health care professionals at hospitals. Respondents assigned importance ratings to factors used in recent futility decisions made at their institutions. The resulting importance ratings showed significant variation by characteristics of the institution (comparing respondents from for-profit, not-for-profit, and government hospitals) and by profession of the respondent (comparing physicians and nurses). The respondents' judgments endorsed three distinct strategies for making futility decisions (i.e., emphasis on the patient's decision preferences, providing for the patient and family, and adhering to objective medical and social norms).
- Published
- 1998
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
49. Justice, Conflict and Wellbeing
- Author
-
Brian H. Bornstein and Richard L. Wiener
- Subjects
Political science ,Conflict resolution ,Arbitration ,Social conflict ,Therapeutic jurisprudence ,Objectification ,Criminology ,Distributive justice ,Dehumanization ,Alternative dispute resolution ,Social psychology - Abstract
Part I: Distributive justice: All else being equal: Overcoming the egalitarian norm.- Cutting your cake and having it too: Or, is equality a distributive justice principle?.- Part II: Objectification and discrimination.- A psycholegal model of hostile environments: The role of dehumanization.- Exploring the dark matter of objectification.- Part III: Justice and conflict involving people with mental illness.- Therapeutic jurisprudence and recovery from severe and disabling mental illness.- Mental illness, dangerousness, and police power interventions in pursuit of justice and well-being.- That's What Friends Are For: Mentors, LAP Lawyers, Therapeutic Jurisprudence, and Clients with Mental Illness.- Part IV: Political conflict, policy solutions, and citizens' wellbeing.- After violent conflict: Justice, wellbeing, and international criminal courts.- Complexity of accountability for mass atrocity.- Part V: Justice outside of court: Alternative dispute resolution.- Advancements in arbitral immunity and judicial review of arbitral awards create ethical loopholes in arbitration.- Retired to greener pastures: The public costs of private judging.- Part VI: Bringing wellbeing to children in conflict: Recess in primary school: The disjuncture between educational policy and scientific research.- Playground conflict: Everyday opportunities for children to manage conflict.
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
50. Perceptions of sexual harassment: The effects of gender, legal standard, and ambivalent sexism
- Author
-
Charles Gasper, Brenda L. Russell, Kelley Mannen, Richard L. Wiener, and Linda E. Hurt
- Subjects
Male ,Logic ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Hostility ,Reasonable person ,Conflict, Psychological ,Sex Factors ,Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,Surveys and Questionnaires ,Perception ,medicine ,Humans ,Women ,General Psychology ,media_common ,Likelihood Functions ,Men ,Test (assessment) ,Legal psychology ,Psychiatry and Mental health ,Ambivalent sexism ,Attitude ,Sexual Harassment ,Harassment ,Female ,medicine.symptom ,Factor Analysis, Statistical ,Psychology ,Prejudice ,Law ,Social psychology - Abstract
This research tests the possibility that the reasonable woman as compared to the reasonable person test of hostile work environment sexual harassment interacts with hostile and benevolent sexist beliefs and under some conditions triggers protectionist attitudes toward women who complain of sexual harassment. We administered to a sample of undergraduates the ambivalent sexism inventory along with the fact patterns in two harassment cases and asked them to make legally relevant decisions under either the reasonable woman or person standard. We found that those high in hostile sexism, and women, found more evidence of harassment. However, those high in benevolent sexism did not exhibit the hostile sexism effects. Although men were less sensitive to the reasonable woman standard than women, under some conditions the reasonable woman standard enabled both genders to find greater evidence of harassment. The results are discussed from the perspectives of law and psychology.
- Published
- 1997
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.