74 results on '"Reversal test"'
Search Results
2. Wisdom of the Established Pattern
- Author
-
Joel Janhonen
- Subjects
Pyschological ,Adaptive Value ,Reversal Test ,Cognitive Bias ,Status Quo ,Mental Patterns ,Medical philosophy. Medical ethics ,R723-726 ,Ethics ,BJ1-1725 - Abstract
Photo by 234988805© Niall Wiggan on Dreamstime INTRODUCTION This short essay explores the plausible wisdom of status quo bias and questions the attempt by Bostrom and Ord to negate its influence. Assuming that the standard role of reason in judgment-formation is only second to affect, I propose that attempts to study and counter cognitive biases should focus on the underlying emotional dispositions. With or without rational descriptions, such innate inclinations of our cognition likely contain some enduring adaptive value. Cognitive biases are deviations from reason-based judgment-formation. They are largely due to conflicts between our subjective image of the world and objective reality. Attaining an exhaustive comprehension of the complex interactions of natural and social worlds is impossible, so we have evolved to operate by relying on a built-in navigation system. Most of our choices we do not base on precise rational modeling but on a far more ancient response mechanism. Sense-making and decision making follow the order of their evolutionary emergence: intuition first, strategic reasoning second. According to Jonathan Haidt, even our moral reasoning mainly functions as a post-hoc confirmation of the preceding affective judgments.[1] The constructed rationalizations are our attempt to understand and unravel the mystery of our intuition and its pragmatic insights. I. Disposition to Resist Change Dispositions are enduring patterns of mental processing. As our inherent qualities, they direct our reactions toward changes transpiring around us. Rather than being an instance of dissonance within the cognitive system, influences like the status quo bias originate outside of it. Therefore, shifting the emphasis from cognitive to affective level gives us a better starting point for evaluating the basis and merit of each identified deviation pattern. Affective responses may appear irrational, yet these have in time become established for solid evolutionary reasons. This might not make them ethically or scientifically appropriate, but nonetheless, we should acknowledge the extensive contribution of these patterns to the success of our species. In his well-known bioethical writing on repugnance, Leon Kass defended the value of disgust that is experienced over perceived violations against nature.[2] He argued that this emotional impulse is an expression of nature’s wisdom, a deeper understanding of the world that human reason is unable to fully articulate. From the evolutionary standpoint, the importance of such intrinsic dispositions and the resulting affective judgments are indeed easy to defend.[3] Humans can attain a significant fitness advantage through reliable collaboration, which requires people to operate according to shared rules. The experienced revulsion toward norm violators gave us a built-in advocate for doing the right thing – even in the absence of authorities. Affect-based moral dispositions enacted a prosocial force over our ancestors, who later articulated and codified this acted-out morality. Therefore, our emotional impulses can be rich in pragmatic ethical wisdom. The intuition to resist change has a similar evolutionary origin. In the absence of a sufficient understanding of the indirect and extended consequences of an alteration, it is generally more advantageous to preserve the status quo. The emotional tendency to experience unease when deviating from conventions can manifest itself in a variety of contexts from politics to healthcare. Forms of affective sway over individual or societal attempts to be logical probably stem from the same roots as our moral sense. The resistance to violating the status quo is indeed often flavored with moral outrage toward the change. Psychologically, maintaining the status quo requires less effort, intention, and awareness.[4] This clearly makes it our standard setting and a considerable cognitive bias. In a sense, this psychological distaste for novelty safeguards against divergent and untested solutions to recurring problems. In the language of Kass, this predisposition is an expression of rarely articulated conventional wisdom.[5] The likelihood of triggering a catastrophe is minimal when following a long-ago-established tradition or pattern of behavior. On the contrary, unprecedented actions always carry unforeseeable consequences. It is likely that many yet-unidentified tendencies have indispensably influenced human decision making. In aggregate, the impact of each established sway must have proven advantageous, even if outcomes in isolated instances would not invariably be optimal or even beneficial. Such features of our cognitive processing can therefore contain adaptive value or natural wisdom, even if we have not identified them or defined their precise function. Therefore, an inability to articulate a rationale for an omission that seeks to maintain the status quo should not be considered indisputable evidence of a misstep. II. Detecting and Eliminating Irrational Stances In a paper that introduced the Reversal Test, Bostrom and Ord argued that it is improbable that the current state of affairs is the best possible one regarding any single parameter that could either be increased or decreased.[6] They suggest this proposed improbability justifies shifting the burden of proof to those in favor of preserving the status quo. According to their example, if someone is opposed to increasing as well as decreasing the average human intelligence, they need a good reason for why the current level is optimal. This thinking suggests that the present state of nature is incidental and has no wisdom or justificatory standing. The authors acknowledged the counterargument based on evolutionary adaptation but questioned the present adaptive value of the evolved human traits and tendencies. The undeniable advantage of reason-based decision making is its universal applicability, while intuitions are context specific. We did not evolve to perform in macroeconomics or quantum physics. Reliance on anything except systematic and rational decision making is therefore inappropriate when studying them, even if we would generally recognize our emotional responses to convey useful insights. Have the instincts that were shaped by a now vanished world become a handicap for scientific and ethical inquiry within our ultra-modern circumstance? III. Enduring Wisdom We can contend with this uncertainty over present adaptive value by either relying on the established patterns of judgment-formation before maladaptiveness is demonstrated or by insisting on fully rational conduct without any mental shortcuts or filtering of information. For me, it is not obvious where the onus to provide evidence on the enduring value of each deviation resides. Humans themselves have not fundamentally changed, so many of our social instincts may hold and there is a plausible pragmatic wisdom in suspicion toward societal change. Besides, human culture has built upon our evolved dispositions and adjusted them for new purposes. The value of an established deviation may remain unclear when an identified bias is interrogated only in relation to a specific parameter. Variables of psychology, society, and nature are not independent but intricately interconnected. The wisdom of our intuition might not reveal itself case by case – rather it resides in its broad undefined influence over humanity. Depending on the context, this influence should be protected against or granted an opportunity to protect us. CONCLUSION Reasons behind established conventions and practices may not become evident until some innovation alters the status quo. It may then be too late to prevent the unexpected downsides, downstream from the change. Similarly, the pragmatic value of natural tendencies that have shaped and defined our mental patterns can remain concealed until a disruption. Arguably, a stance or disposition contains wisdom if it accounts for the overarching pattern, even if its applicability in individual cases is questionable. Therefore, the bias toward preserving the status quo is like a natural caution to favor slow change over rapid. The psychological preference for the established pattern introduces a little inertia to the ever-changing parameters of our world. Rather than reasoning ourselves to a sensible midpoint between societal stagnation and upheaval, arriving there may partially depend on following our collective instinctive navigation system. - [1] Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: a social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological review, 108(4), 814–834. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295x.108.4.814 [2] Kass, L. (1998). The Wisdom of Repugnance: Why We Should Ban the Cloning of Humans. Val. U. L. Rev. 32(2), 679–705. https://scholar.valpo.edu/vulr/vol32/iss2/12 [3] Mameli, M. (2013). Meat made us moral: a hypothesis on the nature and evolution of moral judgment. Biol Philos 28, 903–931. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10539-013-9401-3 [4] Eidelman, S., & Crandall, C., 'CHAPTER 4 A Psychological Advantage for the Status Quo' (pp. 85–106), in Jost, J., Kay, A., and Thorisdottir H. (eds), Social and Psychological Bases of Ideology and System Justification (New York, 2009; online edn, Oxford Academic, 1 May 2009). https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195320916.003.004 [5] Kass, L. (1998). The Wisdom of Repugnance: Why We Should Ban the Cloning of Humans. Val. U. L. Rev. 32(2), 679–705. https://scholar.valpo.edu/vulr/vol32/iss2/12 [6] Bostrom, N., & Ord, T. (2006). The Reversal Test: Eliminating Status Quo Bias in Applied Ethics. In Ethics 116(4), 656–679. University of Chicago Press. https://doi.org/10.1086/505233
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
3. In vivo and in vitro evidence for the inhibition of homogentisate solanesyltransferase by cyclopyrimorate.
- Author
-
Shino, Mamiko, Hamada, Takahiro, Shigematsu, Yoshio, and Banba, Shinichi
- Subjects
ARABIDOPSIS thaliana ,HERBICIDES ,CHEMICAL industry ,BIOSYNTHESIS ,EFFECT of herbicides on plants - Abstract
BACKGROUND: Cyclopyrimorate is a highly effective bleaching herbicide discovered by Mitsui Chemicals Agro, Inc. The target site was recently reported to be homogentisate solanesyltransferase (HST) in the plastoquinone (PQ) biosynthesis pathway on the basis of the number of intermediates in cyclopyrimorate‐treated plants and in vitro HST assays. Here, the target site of cyclopyrimorate was further explored using both in vivo and in vitro experiments. RESULTS: The cyclopyrimorate‐dependent bleaching effect on Arabidopsis thaliana was reversed by decyl PQ, suggesting that this symptom is attributable to the inhibition of PQ biosynthesis. Furthermore, homogentisate (HGA), a substrate of HST, weakly reversed the bleaching effect of cyclopyrimorate in a dose‐dependent manner. We expected that the weak reversal by HGA was due to competitive inhibition by cyclopyrimorate or des‐morpholinocarbonyl cyclopyrimorate (DMC), a metabolite of cyclopyrimorate in plants that exhibit higher HST‐inhibitory activity as compared to cyclopyrimorate. Kinetic analysis was therefore conducted using DMC. DMC inhibited HST competitively with respect to HGA, and was a mixed non‐competitive inhibitor with respect to the other substrate, farnesyl diphosphate. Moreover, neither cyclopyrimorate nor DMC inhibited 2‐methyl‐6‐phytyl‐1,4‐benzoquinone/2‐methyl‐6‐solanesyl‐1,4‐benzoquinone methyltransferase, which is located downstream of HST in the PQ biosynthesis pathway. CONCLUSIONS: The target site of cyclopyrimorate and DMC is HST, which is a novel target site for commercial herbicides. © 2019 Society of Chemical Industry [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
4. Natural Speed Observer for Nonsalient AC Motors
- Author
-
Jiahao Chen, Xin Yuan, Yuefei Zuo, Jie Mei, Christopher H. T. Lee, and School of Electrical and Electronic Engineering
- Subjects
Physics ,Observer (quantum physics) ,Bandwidth (signal processing) ,Estimator ,AC motor ,Sensorless Control ,Position (vector) ,Control theory ,Electrical and electronic engineering [Engineering] ,Waveform ,Unified Model ,Electrical and Electronic Engineering ,Reversal test ,Induction motor - Abstract
This letter addresses experimental validation of the reduced-order natural speed observer design for position sensorless drive with nonsalient permanent magnet synchronous motor. The natural speed observer and the active flux estimator are connected in a cascaded fashion, which results in a simple sensorless algorithm that needs only to tune one bandwidth parameter for flux estimation and one bandwidth parameter for speed observation. Experimental results of high speed reversal test, zero speed stopping test, and slow speed reversal test are included, where the practice of applying nonzero d-axis current at zero speed has shown to be effective for loaded zero speed stopping test, but it causes zero-speed lockedup at slow speed reversal with acceleration rate of 50 r/min/s. Four remedies are compared to improve the slow speed reversal test and the proposed method gives almost ramp actual speed waveform, nondiverging q-axis current and smooth transition in position waveform during slow zero speed crossing. A new dynamic expression of the active flux is proposed and with the aid of the active flux concept, the proposed sensorless algorithm is also applicable to various types of ac motors. National Research Foundation (NRF) Accepted version This work was supported by National Research Foundation (NRF) Singapore, under its NRF Fellowship under Grant no: NRF-NRFF12-2020-0003.
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
5. Revisiting the Paleomagnetic Reversal Test: A Bayesian Hypothesis Testing Framework for a Common Mean Direction.
- Author
-
Heslop, D. and Roberts, A. P.
- Subjects
- *
PALEOMAGNETISM , *CHEMICAL reactions , *CRYSTAL structure , *BAYESIAN analysis , *STRUCTURAL geology - Abstract
For over 60 years a suite of field tests has provided paleomagnetists with a means to evaluate the timing of magnetic remanence acquisition and subsequent remanence stability. These tests are crucial for detecting potential overprinting by secondary remanences and for assessing the fidelity of the geological information carried by paleomagnetic signals. The reversal test was developed to detect secondary remanent magnetizations that could potentially bias paleomagnetic reconstructions. More recently, the reversal test has been applied to a broader set of problems, which require statistical assessment of common or antipodal paleomagnetic directions. From a statistical standpoint, the reversal test must distinguish whether two sets of paleomagnetic directions originate from populations with a common mean. However, earlier work has demonstrated that the reversal test may be ambiguous for small numbers of observations because insufficient information is available to reject the null hypothesis of a common mean direction. Here we develop a Bayesian framework to estimate directly the probability that two Fisher‐distributed sets of directions originate from populations with a common mean. This framework can be used to consider data sets with common or different precisions and, thus, provides a fully probabilistic version of the parametric reversal test. Additionally, adoption of a Bayesian framework means that ambiguity associated with the lack of information provided by small numbers of observations is incorporated into the final probability estimate in a natural way. Our new Bayesian test is demonstrated with numerical examples and case studies. Key Points: Tests for a common mean paleomagnetic direction are placed into a Bayesian frameworkTest results naturally incorporate uncertainty when only small sets of observations are availableThe Bayesian test for a common mean direction is illustrated with a number of case studies [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
6. Kea (Nestor notabilis) show flexibility and individuality in within-session reversal learning tasks
- Author
-
Monika Laschober, Ludwig Huber, Roger Mundry, and Raoul Schwing
- Subjects
Original Paper ,Touchscreen ,Parrot cognition ,Pooling ,Individuality ,Flexibility (personality) ,Reversal Learning ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Cognition ,Reversal estimation ,Session (web analytics) ,Generalized linear mixed model ,Task (project management) ,Discrimination Learning ,Parrots ,Midsession reversal ,Animals ,Alternation (linguistics) ,Psychology ,Win-stay/lose-shift ,GLMM ,Reinforcement, Psychology ,Reversal test ,Ecology, Evolution, Behavior and Systematics ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
The midsession reversal paradigm confronts an animal with a two-choice discrimination task where the reward contingencies are reversed at the midpoint of the session. Species react to the reversal with either win-stay/lose-shift, using local information of reinforcement, or reversal estimation, using global information, e.g. time, to estimate the point of reversal. Besides pigeons, only mammalian species were tested in this paradigm so far and analyses were conducted on pooled data, not considering possible individually different responses. We tested twelve kea parrots with a 40-trial midsession reversal test and additional shifted reversal tests with a variable point of reversal. Birds were tested in two groups on a touchscreen, with the discrimination task having either only visual or additional spatial information. We used Generalized Linear Mixed Models to control for individual differences when analysing the data. Our results demonstrate that kea can use win-stay/lose-shift independently of local information. The predictors group, session, and trial number as well as their interactions had a significant influence on the response. Furthermore, we discovered notable individual differences not only between birds but also between sessions of individual birds, including the ability to quite accurately estimate the reversal position in alternation to win-stay/lose-shift. Our findings of the kea’s quick and flexible responses contribute to the knowledge of diversity in avian cognitive abilities and emphasize the need to consider individuality as well as the limitation of pooling the data when analysing midsession reversal data. Supplementary Information The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1007/s10071-021-01524-1.
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
7. Examining the effect of stress on the flexible updating of avoidance responses
- Author
-
Tom Smeets, Conny W.E.M. Quaedflieg, Marleen M. Rijkeboer, Pauline Dibbets, Anke Lemmens, Medical and Clinical Psychology, Section Clinical Psychology, RS: FPN CPS III, RS: FPN NPPP I, and Section Neuropsychology
- Subjects
acute stress ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Hydrocortisone ,avoidance behavior ,MAST ,Reversal Learning ,Audiology ,Extinction, Psychological ,Task (project management) ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,Stress (linguistics) ,Avoidance Learning ,medicine ,Extinction, Psychological/physiology ,Humans ,Acute stress ,Association (psychology) ,Reversal test ,030304 developmental biology ,Balance (ability) ,0303 health sciences ,Avoidance Learning/physiology ,General Neuroscience ,Extinction ,Affect ,Psychological/physiology ,Stress induction ,Aversive Stimulus ,Psychology ,noradrenergic activity ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery - Abstract
Acute stress has been found to impair the flexible updating of stimulus-outcome associations. However, there is a lack of studies investigating the effect of acute stress on the flexible updating of stimulus-response associations, like active avoidance responses. The current study used an avoidance reversal learning paradigm to address this question. Sixty-one participants learned that a red dot was associated with an aversive sound, whereas a green dot was not (Pavlovian Acquisition phase). Next, they were trained to avoid the aversive stimulus by selectively pressing a button in response to the red, but not the green, dot (Avoidance Acquisition phase). Subsequently, participants either underwent a stress induction task or a no-stress control task. The flexible updating of expectancies of the US and avoidance responses were assessed after reversal of the original contingencies (Reversal Test). Acute stress did not impair the flexible updating of avoidance responses during the Reversal Test. In contrast, results showed that in the stress group the expectancies of the aversive sound were more in accordance with the reversed contingencies compared to the ratings of control participants. Additionally, cortisol responders avoided less often in comparison to cortisol non-responders. Increased noradrenergic activity in stressed participants was related to impairments in the flexible updating of avoidance responses after contingency reversal, while this association was absent in the control participants. In conclusion, our results suggest that the autonomic response might account for shifting the balance towards inflexible updating of stimulus-outcome awareness while stress does not impair flexible updating of avoidance responses.
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
8. Introduction
- Author
-
Vogt, Arthur, Barta, János, Vogt, Arthur, and Barta, János
- Published
- 1997
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
9. Comparison of Reversal Test Pictures among Three Groups of Students: Normal, Education Mental Retarded and Students with Learning Disabilities in Tehran
- Author
-
Mohammad Reza Koushesh, Hadi Bahrami, Akbar Biglarian, Akbr Forougheddin-A'dl, Laleh Same-Siahkalroudi, and Hamid Reza Karimi
- Subjects
Reversal test ,Dyslexia ,Mental retarded ,Normal students ,Therapeutics. Pharmacology ,RM1-950 - Abstract
Objective: Riversal visual perception discrimination test is one of the dyslexia diagnostic tests in children which can be performed in the group (group-based) and it is reliable to detect these disorders in students of the primary schools especially those who spend their first educational weeks or months. The aim of this survey is comparison of Riversal test pictures among three groups of students: normal, educable mental retarded students and students with learning disabilities, aged 8-12 years old that were under coverage of Tehran Welfare Department. Materials & Methods: This Comparative cross – sectional study has performed on 150 girls and boys of mentioned groups that were selected by simple randomize selection. Results: The findings suggested that there was significant difference between surveyed groups (P=0.001). The highest scores were related to normal students and the lowest scores to educable mental retarded. The interval of negative scores of educable mental retarded from normal students was more than that of between educable mental retarded and learning disabilities. Conclusion: This survey indicates that students with learning disabilities (dyslexia) have problems in their visual perception and this test can help to diagnose and determine abnormal children as soon as possible in order to better treatment.
- Published
- 2007
10. The reversal test, status quo bias, and opposition to human cognitive enhancement.
- Author
-
Clarke, Steve
- Subjects
- *
BURDEN of proof , *PRIMA facie evidence , *COGNITION , *DELUSIONS , *PHILOSOPHERS , *ARGUMENT - Abstract
Bostrom and Ord’s reversal test has been appealed to by many philosophers to substantiate the charge that preferences for status quo options are motivated by status quo bias. I argue that their characterization of the reversal test needs to be modified, and that their description of the burden of proof it imposes needs to be clarified. I then argue that there is a way to meet that burden of proof which Bostrom and Ord fail to recognize. I also argue that the range of circumstances in which the reversal test can be usefully applied is narrower than they recognize. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
11. Naloxone Impairs Concurrent but Not Sequential Flavor Aversion: Resorting to a Flexible/Explicit Learning.
- Author
-
Agiiera, Antonio D. R., Bernal, Antonio, and Puerto, Amadeo
- Subjects
- *
NALOXONE , *TASTE aversion , *IMPLICIT learning , *REVERSAL theory (Psychology) , *NEUROTRANSMITTERS , *DRUG administration , *THERAPEUTICS - Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
12. The magnificent seven : A proposal for modest revision of the Van der Voo (1990) quality index
- Author
-
Meert, Joseph G., Pivarunas, Anthony F., Evans, David A. D., Pisarevsky, Sergei A., Pesonen, Lauri J., Li, Zheng-Xiang, Elming, Sten-Ake, Miller, Scott R., Zhang, Shihong, Salminen, Johanna M., Department of Physics, and Department of Geosciences and Geography
- Subjects
Q-Index ,APPARENT POLAR WANDER ,1171 Geosciences ,Paleomagnetism ,PALEOMAGNETIC DIRECTIONS ,SEDIMENTARY-ROCKS ,DIAGENETIC XENOTIME ,Reliability ,114 Physical sciences ,REVERSAL TEST ,Database ,U-PB GEOCHRONOLOGY ,DHARWAR CRATON ,REMAGNETIZATION CIRCLES ,FOLD TEST ,R-Index ,MAGNETIC-ANISOTROPY - Abstract
Thirty years ago, Rob Van der Voo proposed an elegant and simple system for evaluating the quality of paleomagnetic data. As a second-year Ph.D. student, the lead author remembers Rob waxing philosophical about the need to have an appropriate, but not overly rigid evaluation system. The end result was a 7-point system that assigned a (1) or (0) for any paleomagnetic result based on objective criteria. The goal was never to reject or blindly accept any particular result, but merely to indicate the degree of quality for any paleomagnetic pole. At the time, the global paleomagnetic database was burgeoning and it was deemed useful to rank older paleo magnetic results with the newer data being developed in modern laboratories. Van der Voo's, 1990 paper launched a silent revolution in paleomagnetism. Researchers began to evaluate their data against those seven criteria with the anticipation that reviewers would be similarly critical. Today, paleomagnetism is a mature science. Our methods, analyses, and results are more sophisticated than they were 30 years ago. Therefore, we feel it is appropriate to revisit the Van der Voo (1990) criteria in light of those developments. We hope to honor the intention of the original paper by keeping the criteria simple and easy to evaluate while also acknowledging the advances in science. This paper aims to update the criteria and modernize the process. We base our changes on advances in paleomagnetism and geochronology with a faithful adherence to the simplicity of the original publication. We offer the "Reliability" or "R" index as the next generation of the Van der Voo "Quality" or "Q" index. The new R-criteria evaluate seven different information items for each paleomagnetic pole including age, statistical requirements, identification of magnetic carriers, field tests, structural integrity, presence of reversals and an evaluation for possible remagnetization.
- Published
- 2020
13. Revisiting the Paleomagnetic Reversal Test: A Bayesian Hypothesis Testing Framework for a Common Mean Direction
- Author
-
Andrew P. Roberts and David Heslop
- Subjects
Paleomagnetism ,010504 meteorology & atmospheric sciences ,010502 geochemistry & geophysics ,01 natural sciences ,Bayesian statistics ,Geophysics ,Common mean ,Space and Planetary Science ,Geochemistry and Petrology ,Statistics ,Earth and Planetary Sciences (miscellaneous) ,Bayesian hypothesis testing ,Reversal test ,Geology ,0105 earth and related environmental sciences - Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
14. Memory in wild mountain chickadees from different elevations: comparing first-year birds with older survivors
- Author
-
Dovid Y. Kozlovsky, Angela M. Pitera, Eli S. Bridge, Carrie L. Branch, Maria C. Tello-Ramos, and Vladimir V. Pravosudov
- Subjects
0106 biological sciences ,Cognitive ecology ,biology ,Interference theory ,Cognition ,biology.organism_classification ,010603 evolutionary biology ,01 natural sciences ,Intraspecific competition ,Task (project management) ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,Poecile ,Juvenile ,Animal Science and Zoology ,Reversal test ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Ecology, Evolution, Behavior and Systematics ,Demography - Abstract
Understanding both inter- and intraspecific variation in animals' cognitive abilities is one of the central goals of cognitive ecology. We developed a field system for testing spatial learning in wild chickadees using radio frequency identification (RFID)-enabled feeders that allowed us to track individuals across multiple years. Mountain chickadees, Poecile gambeli, inhabit a continuous montane gradient, and individuals inhabiting higher elevations experience harsher winters than those at lower elevations. Previous studies found that chickadees at higher elevations cached more food and demonstrated better spatial memory, but they performed worse during reversal learning than chickadees at lower elevations. Here, we employed spatial learning, reversal learning and memory retention tasks to compare elevation-related performance of first-year juvenile birds with that of adults that had survived at least 1 year. Chickadees from high elevation performed better in the initial learning task but worse in the reversal task than birds from low elevation. There were no differences between first-year birds and adults in the initial learning task, but adults performed significantly better in the reversal test. First-year birds also made more errors associated with the initial target, which suggests higher levels of proactive interference. There were no significant differences between elevations or between juvenile and adults in memory performance after a 16-day retention. After retention, chickadees did not discriminate between the feeders that provided food during the initial task or during the reversal task prior to retention. These results are also consistent with the effects of proactive interference, as birds should have only visited the most recently rewarding feeder. Our findings suggest that the ability to quickly learn changing information is critical for chickadees at both elevations as surviving adults did better in the reversal task than first-year birds. Our results also suggest that selection may favour better reversal learning abilities associated with lower levels of proactive interference.
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
15. PERSPECTIVES ON THE PREVENTION OF LEARNING DIFFICULTIES RELATED TO LANGUAGE STRUCTURE.
- Author
-
Haţegan, Carolina and Gâlgău, Adela
- Subjects
PERSPECTIVE (Art) ,MORPHEMICS ,SPEECH research ,LANGUAGE & languages ,LEARNING ability - Abstract
Copyright of Studia Universitatis Babes-Bolyai, Psychologia-Paedagogia is the property of Babes-Bolyai University, Cluj-Napoca, Romania and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use. This abstract may be abridged. No warranty is given about the accuracy of the copy. Users should refer to the original published version of the material for the full abstract. (Copyright applies to all Abstracts.)
- Published
- 2011
16. Comparison of different protocols of Morris water maze in cognitive impairment with heart failure
- Author
-
Wenkun Cheng, Tong Li, Baofu Wang, Tao Yang, Mingjing Zhao, Yizhou Zhao, Jingjing Yang, Lei Wang, Ziwen Lu, Yang Li, Qi Qiu, and Aiming Wu
- Subjects
Cardiac function curve ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Spatial Learning ,Morris water navigation task ,Reversal Learning ,Water maze ,050105 experimental psychology ,working memory ,lcsh:RC321-571 ,Rats, Sprague-Dawley ,03 medical and health sciences ,Behavioral Neuroscience ,0302 clinical medicine ,Cognition ,Morris Water Maze Test ,Internal medicine ,Medicine ,Animals ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Cognitive Dysfunction ,Latency (engineering) ,lcsh:Neurosciences. Biological psychiatry. Neuropsychiatry ,Reversal test ,Original Research ,cognitive impairment ,Heart Failure ,Behavior, Animal ,business.industry ,Working memory ,05 social sciences ,medicine.disease ,Rats ,Memory, Short-Term ,Heart failure ,Cardiology ,business ,Morris water maze ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery - Abstract
Aim This study aimed to find a more sensitive and systematic behavioral evaluation protocol to evaluate the cognitive impairment in rats with heart failure (HF). Methods and results An HF rat model was built by ligating the left anterior descending coronary artery. The cardiac function and structure were detected using echocardiography. Myocardial histopathological changes were observed by nitro blue tetrazolium and hematoxylin–eosin staining. The cognitive functions were evaluated using the acquisition task, probe trial, reversal test, and matching‐to‐sample test of the Morris water maze. In the probe trial, the number of times the rats in the model group crossed the platform site significantly decreased compared with that in the sham group. In the reversal test, the average latency was significantly longer in the sham group compared with the model group in the first trial but was shorter in the second and third trials. In the matching‐to‐sample test, the average latency of Trial1 increased significantly in the model group compared with the sham group, while no obvious difference was observed in Trial2. Therefore, the difference in the average latency between Trial1 and Trial2 of the model group was significantly larger. Conclusions The cognitive impairment in rats with HF mainly reflected in the long‐term and working memory, spatial learning, and reversal learning ability. The probe trial and reversal test in the water maze may be more sensitive and preferred to evaluate cognitive function after HF. These findings would provide a brief evaluation protocol for further studies on the relationship between cognitive function and HF., This study aimed to find a more sensitive and systematic behavioral evaluation protocol to evaluate the cognitive impairment in rats with heart failure (HF).The cognitive impairment in rats with HF mainly reflected in the long‐term and working memory, spatial learning and reversal learning ability.
- Published
- 2019
17. A deeper insight in predicting the effect of voltage polarity reversal on HVDC cables
- Author
-
Giovanni Mazzanti, Antonio Battaglia, Massimo Marzinotto, and A. Battaglia, M. Marzinotto, G. Mazzanti
- Subjects
010302 applied physics ,Physics ,Voltage polarity ,voltage polarity reversal, HVDC cables, Line Commutated Converters, LCC HVDC systems ,020209 energy ,Current source converter ,02 engineering and technology ,Relaxation time constant ,01 natural sciences ,Power flow ,Control theory ,0103 physical sciences ,0202 electrical engineering, electronic engineering, information engineering ,Heuristic procedure ,Reversal test - Abstract
The reversal of voltage polarity is essential in HVDC cable systems with Current Source Converters (CSC), since it enables to revert the direction of the power flow. In a previous paper by the authors a life model for HVDC cables subjected to voltage polarity reversal was developed and applied for predicting the life of HVDC cables subjected to a voltage polarity reversal test. In that model, a heuristic procedure was developed for determining the relaxation time constant for the redistribution of space charges within the insulation after voltage polarity reversal. In this paper the model is refined according to a novel approach based on manufacturers’ experience with MIND HVDC cables.
- Published
- 2019
18. Impairments in reversal learning following short access to cocaine self-administration
- Author
-
Amber L. LaCrosse, Brooke Jackson, Lori A. Knackstedt, Mark D. Namba, and Allison R. Bechard
- Subjects
0301 basic medicine ,Male ,Sucrose ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Drug-Seeking Behavior ,Context (language use) ,Reversal Learning ,Self Administration ,Toxicology ,Spatial memory ,Article ,Extinction, Psychological ,Rats, Sprague-Dawley ,03 medical and health sciences ,Cocaine-Related Disorders ,0302 clinical medicine ,Cocaine ,Dopamine Uptake Inhibitors ,Reward ,Medicine ,Animals ,Pharmacology (medical) ,Reversal test ,media_common ,Pharmacology ,Environmental enrichment ,business.industry ,Addiction ,Abstinence ,Cognitive training ,Rats ,Psychiatry and Mental health ,030104 developmental biology ,Anesthesia ,business ,Self-administration ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery - Abstract
Background Cocaine use disorder is characterized by compulsive drug-seeking that persists long into abstinence. Work using rodent models of cocaine addiction has found evidence for reversal learning deficits 21 days after non-contingent cocaine administration and 60 days after self-administration. Here we sought to determine if a deficit in reversal learning is present 3–4 weeks after cessation of cocaine self-administration, when relapse to cocaine-seeking is robust. Conversely, we hypothesized that reversal learning training would protect against relapse, similar to other forms of environmental enrichment. Methods Male rats underwent short access (ShA, 2 h/10d) or long access (LgA, 1 h/7d then 6 h/10d) cocaine self-administration, followed by 21–29 days of abstinence. During abstinence, a subset of rats underwent training in a plus-maze that required an egocentric strategy to earn a sucrose reward. Following response acquisition and retention, the ability to reverse the spatial navigation strategy was tested. Results Total trials to criteria and total errors made did not differ between the groups during response acquisition, retention, or reversal. On the first reversal test, ShA rats performed better than LgA and control rats. ShA rats’ performance worsened over time. There were no effects of cognitive training or length of cocaine access on context-primed relapse of cocaine-seeking. Conclusions The present data indicate that perhaps LgA cocaine self-administration does not produce adaptations to regions mediating context-primed relapse as it does for cocaine and cocaine-associated cue-induced reinstatement of drug-seeking. A time-dependent deficit in reversal learning was found only in ShA rats. Reversal learning training did not protect against cocaine relapse.
- Published
- 2018
19. Improved Tests for Forecast Comparisons in the Presence of Instabilities
- Author
-
Luis F. Martins and Pierre Perron
- Subjects
Statistics and Probability ,Applied Mathematics ,05 social sciences ,Autocorrelation ,Scale (descriptive set theory) ,Monotonic function ,Variance (accounting) ,01 natural sciences ,Power (physics) ,010104 statistics & probability ,0502 economics and business ,Econometrics ,A priori and a posteriori ,0101 mathematics ,Statistics, Probability and Uncertainty ,Null hypothesis ,Reversal test ,050205 econometrics ,Mathematics - Abstract
Of interest is comparing the out-of-sample forecasting performance of two competing models in the presence of possible instabilities. To that effect, we suggest using simple structural change tests, sup-Wald and UDmax as proposed by Andrews (1993) and Bai and Perron (1998), for changes in the mean of the loss-differences. Giacomini and Rossi (2010) proposed a Aiuctuations test and a one-time reversal test also applied to the loss-differences. When properly constructed to account for potential serial correlation under the null hypothesis to have a pivotal limit distribution, it is shown that their tests have undesirable power properties, power that can be low and non-increasing as the alternative gets further from the null hypothesis. The good power properties they reported is simply an artifact of imposing a priori that the loss di§erentials are serially uncorrelated and using the simple sample variance to scale the tests. On the contrary, our statistics are shown to have higher monotonic power, especially the UDmax version. We use their empirical examples to show the practical relevance of the issues raised.
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
20. Multilateral Sato–Vartia index for international comparisons of prices and real expenditures
- Author
-
D. S. Prasada Rao and Naohito Abe
- Subjects
Economics and Econometrics ,Transitive relation ,Index (economics) ,Logarithm ,05 social sciences ,International comparisons ,International Comparison Program ,Number theory ,0502 economics and business ,Econometrics ,050207 economics ,Reversal test ,Finance ,050205 econometrics ,Mathematics - Abstract
The Sato–Vartia (SV) index for bilateral price comparisons has impressive analytical properties and is used intensively in recent international trade and macroeconomic analyses. We show that the SV index is only one of many logarithmic indices that satisfy the factor reversal test discussed in index number theory. In this paper we propose several transitive multilateral versions of the SV index but note that these do not satisfy factor reversal test. We derive closed form expressions for the transitive logarithmic indices and empirically implement the new indices for cross-country price comparisons using World Bank data from the 2011 International Comparison Program.
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
21. Plasticity in Inferotemporal Cortex-Amygdala-Lateral Hypothalamus Axis during Operant Behavior of the Monkey
- Author
-
Ono, Taketoshi, Fukuda, Masaji, Nishijo, Hisao, Nakamura, Kiyomi, Woody, Charles D., editor, Alkon, Daniel L., editor, and McGaugh, James L., editor
- Published
- 1988
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
22. Automated test of behavioral flexibility in mice using a behavioral sequencing task in IntelliCage
- Author
-
Shigetomo Suyama, Toshihiro Endo, Wataru Miyazaki, Hans-Peter Lipp, Masaki Kakeyama, Toshihiko Yada, David P. Wolfer, Kuniko Shimazaki, Yan Zhang, Fumihiko Maekawa, Asahi Haijima, Chiharu Tohyama, Vootele Voikar, and Yukari Uemura
- Subjects
Automation, Laboratory ,Male ,Mice, Inbred ICR ,Flexibility (personality) ,Reversal Learning ,Cognition ,Serial Learning ,Task (project management) ,Test (assessment) ,Developmental psychology ,Discrimination Learning ,Mice, Inbred C57BL ,Mice ,Behavioral Neuroscience ,Rapid acquisition ,Reward ,Species Specificity ,Mice, Inbred DBA ,Models, Animal ,Animals ,Discrimination learning ,Test protocol ,Psychology ,Reversal test ,Neuroscience - Abstract
There has been a long-standing need to develop efficient and standardized behavioral test methods for evaluating higher-order brain functions in mice. Here, we developed and validated a behavioral flexibility test in mice using IntelliCage, a fully automated behavioral analysis system for mice in a group-housed environment. We first developed a "behavioral sequencing task" in the IntelliCage that enables us to assess the learning ability of place discrimination and behavioral sequence for reward acquisition. In the serial reversal learning using the task, the discriminated spatial patterns of the rewarded and never-rewarded places were serially reversed, and thus, mice were accordingly expected to realign the previously acquired behavioral sequence. In general, the tested mice showed rapid acquisition of the behavioral sequencing task and behavioral flexibility in the subsequent serial reversal stages both in intra- and inter-session analyses. It was found that essentially the same results were obtained among three different laboratories, which confirm the high stability of the present test protocol in different strains of mice (C57BL/6, DBA/2, and ICR). In particular, the most trained cohort of C57BL/6 mice achieved a markedly rapid adaptation to the reversal task in the final phase of the long-term serial reversal test, which possibly indicated that the mice adapted to the "reversal rule" itself. In conclusion, the newly developed behavioral test was shown to be a valid assay of behavioral flexibility in mice, and is expected to be utilized in tests of mouse models of cognitive deficits.
- Published
- 2011
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
23. Forecast comparisons in unstable environments
- Author
-
Barbara Rossi and Raffaella Giacomini
- Subjects
Economics and Econometrics ,Heteroscedasticity ,Exchange rate ,Statistics ,Econometrics ,Economics ,Contrast (statistics) ,Sample (statistics) ,Random walk ,Stability (probability) ,Reversal test ,Social Sciences (miscellaneous) ,Statistical hypothesis testing - Abstract
We propose new methods for comparing the out-of-sample forecasting performance of two competing models in the presence of possible instabilities. The main idea is to develop a measure of the relative local forecasting performance for the two models, and to investigate its stability over time by means of statistical tests. We propose two tests (the Fluctuation test and the One-Time Reversal test) that analyze the evolution of the models' relative performance over historical samples. In contrast to previous approaches to forecast comparison, which are based on measures of global performance, we focus on the entire time path of the models' relative performance, which may contain useful information that is lost when looking for the model that forecasts best on average. We apply our tests to the analysis of the time variation in the out-of-sample forecasting performance of monetary models of exchange rate determination relative to the random walk. Copyright © 2010 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
- Published
- 2010
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
24. Selective extra-dimensional set shifting deficit in a knock-in mouse model of Huntington's disease
- Author
-
H. Betteridge, Rebecca C. Trueman, Lesley Jones, Stephen B. Dunnett, and Simon Philip Brooks
- Subjects
Male ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Recall ,Human studies ,General Neuroscience ,Cognitive flexibility ,Cognition ,Audiology ,medicine.disease ,Mice, Mutant Strains ,Mice ,Discrimination, Psychological ,Huntington Disease ,Huntington's disease ,Gene knockin ,Mental Recall ,Mutation ,medicine ,Animals ,Female ,Knock in mouse ,Psychology ,Neuroscience ,Reversal test - Abstract
People with early-stage Huntington's disease have been found to have a specific deficit in performing an extra-dimensional shift. To date no evidence of this deficit has been identified in transgenic or knock-in rodent models of the disease. The aim of the present paper then, was to test whether homozygous knock-in mice derived from the Hdh(CAG(150)) mouse line were impaired in any of five 2-choice discrimination tasks (simple, compound, compound reversal, intra-dimensional shift and extra-dimensional shift), and whether these mice were impaired at recalling these tasks on the following day. On the extra-dimensional shift task the Hdh(CAG(150)) homozygous mice required a greater number of trials to reach criteria than mice and the percentage of correct choices within the trials was also significantly reduced compared with the animals. For the recall tasks, a deficit for recalling the compound reversal test was found in the Hdh(CAG(150)) homozygous mice for both number of trials required to reach criteria and percentage of correct choices within the trials. Recall for the intra-dimensional shift task was also impaired in these animals when measured by the percentage of correct choices. Our results demonstrate a pronounced deficit in the Hdh(CAG(150)) mice not only on extra-dimensional shift performance in agreement with human studies, but also on recall tasks for both the compound reversal and the intra-dimensional shift tasks.
- Published
- 2006
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
25. Better composite environmental performance index
- Author
-
Satyendra Nath Chakrabartty
- Subjects
Core (game theory) ,Identification (information) ,Index (economics) ,Computer science ,Component (UML) ,Statistics ,General Earth and Planetary Sciences ,Environmental Performance Index ,Reversal test ,Scaling ,Confidence interval ,General Environmental Science - Abstract
After reviewing existing environmental performance indices the paper suggests an index of overall environmental performance in terms of geometric mean and avoiding scaling or finding weights for component indicators which may be independent or have correlations with different degrees. The suggested index satisfies many desired properties including monotonically increasing, time reversal test, formation of chain indices, independent of change of scale, reduction of substitutability between component indicators, etc. and facilitates further statistical analysis like estimation, confidence interval and test of significance and identifies the critical areas requiring corrective measures. Such identification is important from a policy point of view. Separate indices for core areas or drivers can be constructed easily by clumping relevant indicators pertaining to each core area. The method helps to reflect path of improvement registered by a country or a region over time. Such paths may provide new criteria for comparison of countries across time.
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
26. Effects of a Flavor-Placement Reversal Test after Different Modalities of Taste Aversion Learning
- Author
-
Amadeo Puerto, Cristina Mediavilla, and Filomena Molina
- Subjects
Male ,Taste ,Time Factors ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,medicine.medical_treatment ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Stimulus (physiology) ,Discrimination Learning ,Behavioral Neuroscience ,medicine ,Animals ,Rats, Wistar ,Reversal test ,Saline ,Flavor ,Proprioception ,Rats ,Smell ,Space Perception ,Taste aversion ,Cues ,Aversive Stimulus ,Psychology ,Neuroscience - Abstract
Taste aversion learning is induced through two different behavioral procedures: a short-term or concurrent (two-daily flavors) and a long-term or sequential (one-daily flavor) procedure. For the concurrent group of animals, two gustatory/olfactory stimuli are presented separately but at the same time on a daily basis. One is paired with simultaneous intragastric administration of hypertonic NaCl and the other with physiological saline. For the sequential group, the two stimuli are presented on alternate days, one of them followed by intragastric injection of the aversive stimulus and the other by saline, both after a delay of 15 min. The two groups learned the task, but when they were subjected to a flavor-placement reversal test only the sequential group was successful in achieving it. In a second experiment, three groups of animals had to learn concurrent or sequential discrimination tasks (with either simultaneous or delayed administration of the visceral stimulus) using only spatial/proprioceptive cues. The data show that none of the groups learned them under these conditions. The results are discussed in terms of the different modalities of learning. Short-term and long-term taste aversion learning are different in the anatomical structures involved, the number of trials required for acquisition and, as shown in this paper, flexibility.
- Published
- 2001
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
27. Selection-Based Versus Topography-Based Responding: An Important Distinction for Stimulus Equivalence?
- Author
-
David A. D. Polson and Joseph A. Parsons
- Subjects
Stimulus equivalence ,050103 clinical psychology ,medicine.medical_specialty ,05 social sciences ,Contrast (statistics) ,Articles ,Audiology ,Acquisition rate ,Repeated testing ,medicine ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,050102 behavioral science & comparative psychology ,Psychology ,Social psychology ,Reversal test ,Selection (genetic algorithm) - Abstract
English-speaking subjects were taught 16 English-French word pairs. Within any given trial, one word from each pair was presented; for eight items, subjects were to select its counterpart from an array of words in the other language (selection-based training), and for the other eight items there was no array and subjects were to type its counterpart (topography-based training). In Experiment 1, all items were trained from French to English, and later, interspersed no-feedback probe trials tested for the emergence of the reversed relations. Half of the eight selection-trained items were tested in the selection-based mode and half were tested in the topography-based mode; similarly, half of the eight topography-trained items were tested in the selection-based mode and half were tested in the topography-based mode. On the first reversal test trial, all 7 subjects scored 100% correct for the selection-tested items; in contrast, 5 of the 7 subjects scored 0% or near 0% correct for the topography-tested items, which improved to varying degrees with repeated testing. The training response mode affected neither acquisition rate nor reversal test trial performance. In Experiment 2, all items were tested in the topography-based mode only, and subjects were exposed to nine consecutive reversal test trials prior to interspersed probe testing. Improved accuracy across reversal test trials was not observed until the conditions of probe testing were instated, an indication that reexposure to the trained relations was a crucial component of delayed emergence. In Experiment 3, all items were trained from English to French, resulting in subjects typing a familiar rather than an unfamiliar word on reversal test trials. Accuracy on reversal test trials was considerably better than in the previous two experiments. We discuss the implications of our findings for stimulus equivalence research.
- Published
- 2000
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
28. Mitochondrial Diversity and the Reversal Test
- Author
-
Madeline Bannon and Matthew H. Haber
- Subjects
Issues, ethics and legal aspects ,Argumentative ,Genetic diversity ,Sparrow ,Evolutionary biology ,Health Policy ,biology.animal ,Genetic variation ,Biology ,Value (mathematics) ,Reversal test ,Diversity (business) - Abstract
In “Imposing Genetic Diversity,” Robert Sparrow (2015) offers a means to assess the value of genetic diversity. He proposes a number of thought experiments that draw on a particular argumentative s...
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
29. Imposing Genetic Diversity: An Imposition on Reproductive Freedom
- Author
-
Michelle J. Bayefsky
- Subjects
Issues, ethics and legal aspects ,Genetic diversity ,Sparrow ,Evolutionary biology ,Health Policy ,biology.animal ,Genetic variation ,Biology ,Reversal test - Abstract
In his target article, Sparrow (2015) argues that we ought to be wary of our positive intuitions toward genetic diversity because they result in problematic conclusions when a “reversal test” is ap...
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
30. Dissociable effects of 5-HT2C receptor antagonism and genetic inactivation on perseverance and learned non-reward in an egocentric spatial reversal task
- Author
-
Peter G. Clifton, Elizabeth M. Somerville, and Simon R. O. Nilsson
- Subjects
Male ,Indoles ,lcsh:Medicine ,Aminopyridines ,Reversal Learning ,Choice Behavior ,Discrimination Learning ,03 medical and health sciences ,Mice ,0302 clinical medicine ,Cognition ,Reward ,Task Performance and Analysis ,Receptor, Serotonin, 5-HT2C ,Animals ,Discrimination learning ,Valence (psychology) ,lcsh:Science ,Reversal test ,030304 developmental biology ,Mice, Knockout ,0303 health sciences ,Multidisciplinary ,Behavior, Animal ,lcsh:R ,Cognitive flexibility ,Novelty ,Repeated measures design ,BF0180 ,5-HT2C receptor ,Mice, Inbred C57BL ,Space Perception ,Serotonin 5-HT2 Receptor Antagonists ,lcsh:Q ,Psychology ,Neuroscience ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Research Article - Abstract
Cognitive flexibility can be assessed in reversal learning tests, which are sensitive to modulation of 5-HT2C receptor (5-HT2CR) function. Successful performance in these tests depends on at least two dissociable cognitive mechanisms which may separately dissipate associations of previous positive and negative valence. The first is opposed by perseverance and the second by learned non-reward. The current experiments explored the effect of reducing function of the 5-HT2CR on the cognitive mechanisms underlying egocentric reversal learning in the mouse. Experiment 1 used the 5-HT2CR antagonist SB242084 (0.5 mg/kg) in a between-groups serial design and Experiment 2 used 5-HT2CR KO mice in a repeated measures design. Animals initially learned to discriminate between two egocentric turning directions, only one of which was food rewarded (denoted CS+, CS−), in a T- or Y-maze configuration. This was followed by three conditions; (1) Full reversal, where contingencies reversed; (2) Perseverance, where the previous CS+ became CS− and the previous CS− was replaced by a novel CS+; (3) Learned non-reward, where the previous CS− became CS+ and the previous CS+ was replaced by a novel CS-. SB242084 reduced perseverance, observed as a decrease in trials and incorrect responses to criterion, but increased learned non-reward, observed as an increase in trials to criterion. In contrast, 5-HT2CR KO mice showed increased perseverance. 5-HT2CR KO mice also showed retarded egocentric discrimination learning. Neither manipulation of 5-HT2CR function affected performance in the full reversal test. These results are unlikely to be accounted for by increased novelty attraction, as SB242084 failed to affect performance in an unrewarded novelty task. In conclusion, acute 5-HT2CR antagonism and constitutive loss of the 5-HT2CR have opposing effects on perseverance in egocentric reversal learning in mice. It is likely that this difference reflects the broader impact of 5HT2CR loss on the development and maintenance of cognitive function.
- Published
- 2013
31. The reversal test: An examination of secondary directions
- Author
-
Gary R. Scott and S. A. Hotes
- Subjects
Magnetization ,Paleomagnetism ,Geophysics ,Demagnetizing field ,General Earth and Planetary Sciences ,Magnitude (mathematics) ,Antipodal point ,Mineralogy ,Biasing ,Geometry ,Normal ,Reversal test ,Geology - Abstract
Paleomagnetic data frequently display bipolar data that are not exactly antipodal. One cause of this biasing is the addition of another later (secondary) magnetization that cannot be removed by laboratory demagnetization experiments. The magnitude of this hidden secondary magnetization can be calculated when only two values are known : the non-antipodal angle (measured), and the angle between the Normal direction (measured) and secondary direction (measured or assumed). Furthermore, a minimum magnitude of the secondary magnetization can be calculated with knowledge only of the non-antipodal angle. Applied to three published data sets (sandstone, limestone and diorite intrusion) with non-antipodal angles of 11°, 28° and 11° respectively ; a secondary component is calculated at 25%, 28% and 16% of the characteristic magnetization remaining after laboratory cleaning. Near surface alteration (weathering) appears to be the source of secondary magnetization in two of these cases. A directionally consistent, biasing effect of a few degrees is made on the mean direction as compared to the conventional calculation of averaging the non-antipodal directions.
- Published
- 1996
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
32. Errorless learning of a conditional temporal discrimination
- Author
-
Armando Machado, Joana Arantes, and Universidade do Minho
- Subjects
medicine.medical_specialty ,Reinforcement Schedule ,Stimulus generalization ,Color vision ,Social Sciences ,Reversal Learning ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Audiology ,Stimulus (physiology) ,Errorless ,Choice Behavior ,050105 experimental psychology ,Developmental psychology ,Discrimination Learning ,Behavioral Neuroscience ,Orientation ,medicine ,Key peck ,Animals ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,050102 behavioral science & comparative psychology ,Discrimination learning ,Timing ,Columbidae ,10. No inequality ,Reversal test ,Research Articles ,Science & Technology ,05 social sciences ,Association Learning ,Time perception ,Conditional discrimination ,Memory, Short-Term ,Fading ,Generalization, Stimulus ,Practice, Psychological ,Sample size determination ,Time Perception ,Errorless learning ,Conditioning, Operant ,Pigeons ,Psychology ,Color Perception - Abstract
In the present study we extended errorless learning to a conditional temporal discrimination. Pigeons’ responses to a left–red key after a 2-s sample and to a right–green key after a 10-s sample were reinforced. There were two groups: One learned the discrimination through trial and error and the other through an errorless learning procedure. Then, both groups were presented with three types of tests. First, they were exposed to intermediate durations between 2 s and 10 s, and given a choice between both keys (stimulus generalization test). Second, a delay from 1 s to 16 s was included between the offset of the sample and the onset of the choice keys (delay test). Finally, pigeons learned a new discrimination in which the stimuli were switched (reversal test). Results showed that pigeons from the Errorless group made significantly fewer errors than those in the Trial-and-Error group. Both groups performed similarly during the stimulus generalization test and the reversal test, but results of the delay test suggested that, on long stimulus trials, responding in the errorless training group was less disrupted by delays., This research was presented in part at the 33rd Annual Convention of the Association of Behavioral Analysis (ABA) in San Diego, California. The authors were supported by the Portuguese Foundation for Science and Technology (FT). The authors thank Herbert S. Terrace for his valuable thoughts at the 31st Annual Meeting of the Society for the Quantitative Analyses of Behavior (SQAB) about this research.
- Published
- 2011
33. Index-Number Tests and the Common-Scaling Social Cost-of-Living Index
- Author
-
Krishna Pendakur and David Donaldson
- Subjects
Economics and Econometrics ,Index (economics) ,Social cost ,jel:D63 ,Function (mathematics) ,jel:C43 ,Homothetic transformation ,social cost of living, aggregation, welfare economics ,Value (economics) ,Econometrics ,Economics ,Reversal test ,Scaling ,Mathematical economics ,Social Sciences (miscellaneous) ,Reciprocal - Abstract
For a change in prices, the common-scaling social cost-of-living index is the equal scaling of every individual's expenditure level needed to restore the level of social welfare to its pre-change value. This index does not, in general, satisfy two standard index-number tests. The reversal test requires the index value for the reverse change to be the reciprocal of the original index. And the circular test requires the product of index values for successive price changes to be equal to the index value for the whole change. We show that both tests are satisfied if and only if the Bergson-Samuelson indirect social-welfare function is homothetic in prices, a condition which does not require individual preferences to be homothetic. If individual preferences are homothetic, however, stronger conditions on the Bergson-Samuelson indirect must be satisfied. Given these results, we ask whether the restrictions are empirically reasonable and find, in the case that individual preferences are not homothetic, that the restrictions make little difference to estimates of the index.
- Published
- 2010
34. Jordan Left-Right Reversal Test: An analysis of visual reversals in children and significance for reading problems
- Author
-
Susan G. Jordan and Brian T. Jordan
- Subjects
Male ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Dysfunctional family ,Test validity ,Neuropsychological Tests ,Developmental psychology ,Discrimination Learning ,Dyslexia ,Reading (process) ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,medicine ,Humans ,Attention ,Psychological testing ,Child ,Reversal test ,media_common ,Test (assessment) ,Form Perception ,Psychiatry and Mental health ,Pattern Recognition, Visual ,Pediatrics, Perinatology and Child Health ,Learning disability ,Laterality ,Female ,medicine.symptom ,Psychology - Abstract
The Jordan Left-Right Reversal Test (JLRRT) was first published in 1974 (1) as a convenient measure of symbol reversals in children. A current revision of the test, based on 3,000 children, showed error scores to be inversely related to age and sex. Learning disabled children and a group of below average readers made significantly more errors, indicating that visual reversals are dysfunctional for reading skills.
- Published
- 1990
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
35. The Case Against Cognitive Enhancement: Responding to the Reversal Test
- Author
-
Agar, Nick, Monro, Alice, Agar, Nick, and Monro, Alice
- Abstract
In this thesis I argue against the use of genetic technologies to enhance human cognitive capacities. More specifically, I respond to Nick Bostrom and Toby Ord's "Reversal Test", which they use to argue in favour of genetic cognitive enhancement. The Reversal Test is a burden of proof challenge designed to diagnose status quo bias in arguments against enhancement. By noting that most of those who oppose raising intelligence would also oppose lowering intelligence, the Reversal Test puts the onus on opponents of enhancement to explain why both increases and decreases in our cognitive capacity would be worse than the status quo (our current level of intelligence). Bostrom and Ord claim that if no good reasons can be provided, this indicates that the opposition to enhancement is influenced by status quo bias. Since cognitive biases cannot provide a moral reason against enhancement, opposition to genetic cognitive enhancement shown to be affected by status quo bias can accordingly be discounted. The aim of my thesis, then, is to overcome the Reversal Test' s burden of proof challenge by showing that my reasons for opposing cognitive enhancement are not influenced by status quo bias. However, I do not argue that enhanced intelligence could not be beneficial to the individual. Instead, I claim that the probable unequal distribution of enhancements between the best- and worst-off would be likely to cause serious injustices to those who are unable to afford them.
- Published
- 2008
36. Effect of intrapartum meperidine on behavior of 3- to 12-month-old infant rhesus monkeys
- Author
-
Mari S. Golub and James M. Donald
- Subjects
Male ,Meperidine ,Neurological disorder ,Discrimination Learning ,Cognition ,Pregnancy ,Task Performance and Analysis ,medicine ,Animals ,Effects of sleep deprivation on cognitive performance ,Discrimination learning ,Reversal test ,Labor, Obstetric ,Behavior, Animal ,business.industry ,medicine.disease ,Macaca mulatta ,Anesthesia ,Pediatrics, Perinatology and Child Health ,Toxicity ,Gestation ,Conditioning, Operant ,Female ,Opiate ,business ,Developmental Biology - Abstract
Potential long-term effects of intrapartum meperidine were studied in rhesus monkey infants whose dams received 0,2, or 3 mg/kg meperidine, i.v., during labor (n = 5, 5, 3). Spontaneous behavior and cognitive performance were evaluated at 3–12 months of age. Observation of spontaneous behaviors indicated less age-related increase in quiet activities in drug-exposed infants. In the discrimination reversal test, drug-exposed infants had more balks (p = 0.008) and fewer correct choices (p = 0.008) during initial phases of the first reversal. Due to sex differences in the delayed alternation test, evaluation of drug effects on short-term memory was not possible. In the continuous performance test, drug-exposed infants performed better (NS) and had fewer omission errors (p = 0.034) during the second half of the test period. These initial findings suggest that short-term opiate exposure during labor can alter later behavior of infant monkeys.
- Published
- 1995
37. Time Reversal Invariance in the beta-Decays of A = 8 Nuclei
- Author
-
E. M. Henley and S. Ying
- Subjects
Physics ,Nuclear and High Energy Physics ,Nuclear Theory ,Scattering ,Phase (waves) ,FOS: Physical sciences ,Alpha particle ,Electron ,Nuclear Theory (nucl-th) ,Positron ,medicine.anatomical_structure ,Coulomb ,medicine ,Atomic physics ,Reversal test ,Nucleus - Abstract
We examine time reversal invariance in the beta-decays of 8B(2+) and 8Li(2+) to 8Be(2+) in detail, with particular attention to final state interactions of the two alpha particles from the decay of 8Be, and of the electron (positron) with the daughter nucleus. A R-matrix formulation is used, with the initial state described by a shell model. The R-matrix parameters are obtained by fitting the alpha-\alpha scattering phase shifts and allowed beta-decay rates. Because the nuclear final state interaction effects on the time reversal test come from second forbidden beta-decays, they are small and can be minimized by a suitable choice of kinematic conditions. The e(+/-)-nucleus Coulomb interaction induced T-violation effects depend on the first forbidden operators., Comment: 64 pages, latex, 26 figures (available upon request)
- Published
- 1994
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
38. Word Reversal Test: A new measure of interference and cognitive efficiency
- Author
-
M.D. Franzen, J.D. Petrick, K.L. Wilhelm, and M. McCue
- Subjects
Psychiatry and Mental health ,Clinical Psychology ,Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology ,Computer science ,Measure (physics) ,Cognitive efficiency ,General Medicine ,Interference (wave propagation) ,Reversal test ,Word (computer architecture) ,Cognitive psychology - Published
- 1995
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
39. A Commentary on the Jordan Left-Right Reversal Test
- Author
-
Brian T. Jordan
- Subjects
Visual perception ,Learning disability ,General Engineering ,medicine ,General Earth and Planetary Sciences ,Test validity ,medicine.symptom ,Psychology ,Reversal test ,General Environmental Science ,Developmental psychology ,Cognitive psychology - Published
- 1976
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
40. Directional Awareness Training
- Author
-
Jack McKiernan and Margo Avakian
- Subjects
media_common.quotation_subject ,General Engineering ,Awareness training ,Developmental psychology ,Test (assessment) ,Reading Problems ,Reading (process) ,Learning disability ,medicine ,General Earth and Planetary Sciences ,Discrimination learning ,medicine.symptom ,Psychology ,Remedial education ,Reversal test ,General Environmental Science ,media_common - Abstract
Letter reversal errors, both expressive (written) and receptive (read), are common among children and adults with reading problems. Educators have long wrestled with the problem of eliminating letter reversal errors. This investigation examined whether a program which trains directional awareness can reduce the letter reversal discrimination errors of deficient readers. We chose to examine receptive letter reversal errors for two reasons: first, because they represent a significant reading problem; and second, because a standardized measure of these errors exists in the form of the Jordan Left/Right Reversal Test. Ten deficient readers, ranging from 6 to 19 years of age, were selected to participate in the study. Although the older students exceeded the norms range of the Jordan Test, they were included in order to examine the viability of maturational and "critical period" hypotheses which might have some bearing upon skill gains among learners of different ages. Eight of the ten had achieved scores within the normal range on standardized
- Published
- 1980
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
41. Characterization of fisher’s 'ideal index' by three reversal tests—A note
- Author
-
J. Voeller and H. Funke
- Subjects
Statistics and Probability ,Set (abstract data type) ,Index (economics) ,Ideal (set theory) ,Price index ,Statistics, Probability and Uncertainty ,Characterization (mathematics) ,Mathematical economics ,Reversal test ,Axiom ,Test (assessment) ,Mathematics - Abstract
As Vartia (1976, p. 32) has noted correctly there still does not exist a sufficient number of different axiom systems each characterizing one and only one index. By characterization we mean the deduction of a certain index from a set of given conditions such that the index in question not only meets the required conditions but also represents the only index that can be deduced from these conditions. If for some reason these conditions are considered to be basic or natural requirements they are often called axioms or tests. The so-called test approach in the theory of the price index heavily makes use of certain axioms/tests and special conditions in order to define or characterize price indices as well as to evaluate their distinct merits. A rather comprehensive presentation of this branch of index theory is provided in Eichhorn/Voeller (1976b) where the general solution of the inconsistency problem of Fisher's tests is proven I). Also, characterization s of many well-known price indices are introduced including a characterization of Fisher's "ideal index". This latter index deserves particular attention since it satisfies all of Fisher's tests for price and quantity indices except for the Circular Test. Consequently, Fisher himself (1922, p. 271) proposed to discard the Circular Test 2) The problem, however, as to Fisher's assumed superiority of the "ideal index" over all other indices remained unsolved. Furthermore, the inconsistency of different subsets of Fisher's original test system calls for new and consistent axiom/test systems to define or even characterize indices. In the following, we shall state three tests each imposing a kind of reversal procedure on an index function. Two of the reversal tests, the Time Reversal Test and the Factor Reversal Test, are taken from Fisher's original test system. The third test which we call Price Reversal Test is new but very reasonable. As will be shown below, Fisher's "ideal index" represents the only index satisfying all three reversal tests simultaneously, i.e., can be characterized by them.
- Published
- 1979
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
42. The price reversal test and economic indexes
- Author
-
Kazuo Sato
- Subjects
Statistics and Probability ,Econometrics ,Statistics, Probability and Uncertainty ,Reversal test ,Mathematics - Published
- 1980
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
43. The Ideal Log-Change Index Number
- Author
-
Kazuo Sato
- Subjects
Economics and Econometrics ,Index (economics) ,Ideal (set theory) ,Function (mathematics) ,Divisia index ,Homothetic preferences ,Reversal test ,Mathematical economics ,Preference (economics) ,Social Sciences (miscellaneous) ,Homothetic transformation ,Mathematics - Abstract
RICE and quantum indexes (P, Q) are dual to each other if PQ = E where E is the expenditure index. They satisfy the weak factor reversal test.' If they share an identical weighting formula as weighted averages of price and quantity relatives, they satisfy the strong factor reversal test, that is, they are ideal. The most celebrated ideal economic index is the one associated with the name of Irving Fisher though it was discovered before him. No ideal index as simple as Fisher's has been discovered since. Log-change index numbers have become increasingly popular in recent years, particularly as an approximation to the theoretically desirable Divisia index. Theil (1973) proposed a new log-change index number that alhnost satisfies the strong factor reversal test. I derived several alternative formulas that improve in the degree of approximation (Sato, 1974b). But neither Theil nor I was able to obtain the ideal log-change index. In section II, I report its discovery. Our pessimism has proved premature. Indeed, the formula was self-evident from the very beginning -we simply failed to see it.2 There are dual dualities between economic indexes and homothetic preferences (Samuelson and Swamy, 1974). A price or quantum index is associated with a homothetic indirect or direct preference ordering. If P and Q are dual to each other, so are the direct and indirect preference orderings corresponding to them. If P and Q are ideal, the latter are not only dual but also share an identical mathematical form. They are strictly self-dual as I call them elsewhere.3 An obvious example is the association of Cobb-Douglas indexes and preferences. A less obvious example is the association of Fisher's ideal indexes and quadratic preferences. The association itself was discovered by Konuis and Buscheguence a half century ago in 1926.4 Note that homothetic quadratic preferences are self-dual. Then, what is the selfdual preference ordering that corresponds to our ideal log-change index? We shall show in section III that it is the CES function that has become so popular in the economic literature, originally discussed by Bergson (1936), rediscovered by Solow (1956), and popularized by Arrow et al. (1961). The CES function is known to be self-dual (Samuelson, 1965) and yet the economic index associated with it has eluded discovery until now. Economic indexes are useful because they apply even when underlying preferences are not homothetic.5 We shall show in section IV that the ideal log-change index corresponds to the addilog preference ordering introduced by Houthakker (1960).
- Published
- 1976
44. A Note on the Jordan Left-Right Reversal Test
- Author
-
Richard L. Allington
- Subjects
Visual perception ,Learning disability ,General Engineering ,medicine ,Primary education ,Mathematics education ,General Earth and Planetary Sciences ,Test validity ,medicine.symptom ,Psychology ,Reversal test ,General Environmental Science - Published
- 1976
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
45. Jordan left-right reversal test: A study of visual reversals in children
- Author
-
Brian T. Jordan and Susan G. Jordan
- Subjects
Male ,Analysis of Variance ,Psychological Tests ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Brain dysfunction ,Age Factors ,Audiology ,Test (assessment) ,Developmental psychology ,Dyslexia ,Perceptual Disorders ,Psychiatry and Mental health ,Sex Factors ,Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity ,Pediatrics, Perinatology and Child Health ,Normal children ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,medicine ,Humans ,Female ,Child ,Psychology ,Reversal test - Abstract
The Jordan Left-Right Reversal Test was constructed in order to standardize a measure for symbol reversals (letters and numbers) in normal children, ages 6 through 10. A random sample of 2,500 subjects was tested in schools selected in six different states. Results established age-sex norms for symbol reversals. Error scores were inversely related to age of all subjects and generally lower for girls. Younger children (6–8.5) made significantly more number reversals than letter reversals. Boys in this age grouping made the highest percentage of number errors. When the Reversal Test was administered to a sample of minimally brain dysfunction children, results indicated that the test has adequate discrimination ability and suggest that visual reversals are a symptom of this childhood disorder.
- Published
- 1974
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
46. DRIVE STRENGTH AND ADAPTATION TO STRESS
- Author
-
J. Rowell and Harry M. B. Hurwitz
- Subjects
Behavior ,Mental Disorders ,Adaptation (eye) ,Adaptation, Physiological ,Brightness discrimination ,Prolonged exposure ,Stress, Physiological ,Stress (linguistics) ,Humans ,Discrimination learning ,Psychology ,Social psychology ,Reversal test ,General Psychology ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Support for the hypothesis that adaptive responses reduce the drive component in behaviour was found in an experiment involving two groups of rats placed, for different periods, in a water tank. After being detained in a delay chamber, the rats were allowed to escape after making a brightness discrimination. A discrimination reversal test was undertaken in order to establish whether the original learning had been affected by the length of the water-detention period. It was found that a long prediscrimination detention period facilitated reversal learning. This result was taken to support the major contention of the paper, that prolonged exposure to a potential stress situation encouraged the elaboration of drive-minimizing responses and, in consequence, favoured discrimination learning.
- Published
- 1958
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
47. Cross-modal reversal in the rabbit
- Author
-
Arthur L. Yehle and Jeannette P. Ward
- Subjects
medicine.medical_specialty ,genetic structures ,Stimulation ,General Chemistry ,Visual modality ,Audiology ,Stimulus (physiology) ,Catalysis ,Developmental psychology ,Stimulus modality ,otorhinolaryngologic diseases ,medicine ,Psychology ,Reversal test ,psychological phenomena and processes - Abstract
A classically conditioned discrimination was established between two rates of intermittent stimulation in the auditory modality for four rabbits and in the visual modality for four additional rabbits. Following acquisition in the original modality, both stimulus modality and stimulus contingencies were changed in a cross-modal reversal test. All rabbits were subsequently tested in reacquisition of the original discrimination. Results were compared to a previous test of direct cross-modal transfer. It was concluded that results of cross-modal reversal support the earlier finding of cross-modal transfer in the rabbit.
- Published
- 1970
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
48. Production-Theoretic input Price Indices and the Measurement of Real Aggregate input Use
- Author
-
Franklin M. Fisher
- Subjects
Index (economics) ,Number theory ,Price index ,Factor price ,Aggregate (data warehouse) ,Economics ,Mathematical economics ,Measure (mathematics) ,Reversal test ,Term (time) - Abstract
Index number theory, and the theory of price indices in particular, tends to run in terms of arithmetic properties. Such questions as whether a particular index satisfies a chain property or a reversal test are well discussed and relatively well understood. Important as such properties and such discussions are, however, they are only one side of the index number story. The other side lies in what I shall term the economic theory of index numbers: What are we trying to measure? What would we do if we had all the information possible? What, if anything, are we attempting to approximate with the index numbers computed in practice?
- Published
- 1988
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
49. Relationship of left-right reversals to academic achievement
- Author
-
Helon C. Boone
- Subjects
Male ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Writing ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Academic achievement ,Functional Laterality ,Developmental psychology ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,Achievement test ,Humans ,Child ,Reversal test ,media_common ,Language ,05 social sciences ,050301 education ,Space perception ,030229 sport sciences ,Achievement ,Sensory Systems ,El Niño ,Reading ,Test score ,Laterality ,Visual Perception ,Aptitude ,Female ,Psychology ,0503 education - Abstract
The study was designed to investigate the relationship of reversal of letters and numbers to academic achievement to decide if the tendency to reversal might be used to predict achievement. Two groups of 45 predominantly white third-grade students whose chronological ages were 9.0 yr. or above were administered the California Achievement Tests and the Jordan Left-Right Reversal Test. A statistical comparison was made using scaled scores on the California Achievement Tests and raw errors on the Jordan Left-Right Reversal Test. Reversal was significantly correlated with academic achievement. Children who exhibit reversals at 9 yr. of age or older may be expected to perform less well academically than children who do not reverse letters.
- Published
- 1986
50. Use of the Jordan Left-Right Reversal Test with learning disabled children
- Author
-
Stephen S. Strichart
- Subjects
Male ,Psychometrics ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Developmental psychology ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,Discrimination, Psychological ,Sex Factors ,Form perception ,Orientation ,medicine ,Humans ,Psychological testing ,Child ,Reversal test ,Psychological Tests ,Learning Disabilities ,05 social sciences ,Age Factors ,050301 education ,030229 sport sciences ,Sensory Systems ,Test (assessment) ,Form Perception ,Pattern Recognition, Visual ,Child, Preschool ,Normal children ,Learning disability ,Female ,medicine.symptom ,Psychology ,0503 education ,Learning disabled - Abstract
This investigation established the reliability of the Jordan Left-Right Reversal Test for learning disabled children. Test-retest reliability coefficients ranged from .89 to .92 for a sample of 91 children, 5 through 12 yr., attending private schools for children with learning disabilities. Reversal errors decreased with age for boys and girls, although girls 9 through 12 made significantly fewer errors than did boys in the same age range. Learning disabled children made more errors at all ages than normal children. This test instrument was determined to be a measure of the global tendency to make visual reversal errors and was viewed as an appropriate part of the learning disabilities diagnostic procedure.
- Published
- 1978
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.