79 results on '"Oberfeld D"'
Search Results
2. Reverberation makes interior spaces appear larger
- Author
-
Oberfeld, D., primary, Kalmbach, N., additional, Von Castell, C., additional, Boustila, S., additional, Aspöck, L., additional, and Bechmann, D., additional
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
3. Relation zwischen der Lautheitswachstumsfunktion und den spektralen Gewichten bei der Lautheitswahrnehmung
- Author
-
Verhey, J, Oberfeld, D, Hots, J, Verhey, J, Oberfeld, D, and Hots, J
- Published
- 2022
4. Auditory and audiovisual time-to-collision estimation and road-crossing decisions
- Author
-
Oberfeld, D., primary, Wessels, M., additional, and Huisman, T., additional
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
5. Universal Patterns in Color-Emotion Associations Are Further Shaped by Linguistic and Geographic Proximity
- Author
-
Jonauskaite, D. Abu-Akel, A. Dael, N. Oberfeld, D. Abdel-Khalek, A.M. Al-Rasheed, A.S. Antonietti, J.-P. Bogushevskaya, V. Chamseddine, A. Chkonia, E. Corona, V. Fonseca-Pedrero, E. Griber, Y.A. Grimshaw, G. Hasan, A.A. Havelka, J. Hirnstein, M. Karlsson, B.S.A. Laurent, E. Lindeman, M. Marquardt, L. Mefoh, P. Papadatou-Pastou, M. Pérez-Albéniz, A. Pouyan, N. Roinishvili, M. Romanyuk, L. Salgado Montejo, A. Schrag, Y. Sultanova, A. Uusküla, M. Vainio, S. Wąsowicz, G. Zdravković, S. Zhang, M. Mohr, C.
- Abstract
Many of us “see red,” “feel blue,” or “turn green with envy.” Are such color-emotion associations fundamental to our shared cognitive architecture, or are they cultural creations learned through our languages and traditions? To answer these questions, we tested emotional associations of colors in 4,598 participants from 30 nations speaking 22 native languages. Participants associated 20 emotion concepts with 12 color terms. Pattern-similarity analyses revealed universal color-emotion associations (average similarity coefficient r =.88). However, local differences were also apparent. A machine-learning algorithm revealed that nation predicted color-emotion associations above and beyond those observed universally. Similarity was greater when nations were linguistically or geographically close. This study highlights robust universal color-emotion associations, further modulated by linguistic and geographic factors. These results pose further theoretical and empirical questions about the affective properties of color and may inform practice in applied domains, such as well-being and design. © The Author(s) 2020.
- Published
- 2020
6. Universal Patterns in Color-Emotion Associations Are Further Shaped by Linguistic and Geographic Proximity
- Author
-
Jonauskaite, D., Abu-Akel, A., Dael, N., Oberfeld, D., Abdel-Khalek, A. M., Al-Rasheed, A. S., Antonietti, J. -P., Bogushevskaya, Victoria, Chamseddine, A., Chkonia, E., Corona, V., Fonseca-Pedrero, E., Griber, Y. A., Grimshaw, G., Hasan, A. A., Havelka, J., Hirnstein, M., Karlsson, B. S. A., Laurent, E., Lindeman, M., Marquardt, L., Mefoh, P., Papadatou-Pastou, M., Perez-Albeniz, A., Pouyan, N., Roinishvili, M., Romanyuk, L., Salgado Montejo, A., Schrag, Y., Sultanova, A., Uuskula, M., Vainio, S., Wasowicz, G., Zdravkovic, S., Zhang, M., Mohr, C., Bogushevskaya V. (ORCID:0000-0001-8147-3800), Jonauskaite, D., Abu-Akel, A., Dael, N., Oberfeld, D., Abdel-Khalek, A. M., Al-Rasheed, A. S., Antonietti, J. -P., Bogushevskaya, Victoria, Chamseddine, A., Chkonia, E., Corona, V., Fonseca-Pedrero, E., Griber, Y. A., Grimshaw, G., Hasan, A. A., Havelka, J., Hirnstein, M., Karlsson, B. S. A., Laurent, E., Lindeman, M., Marquardt, L., Mefoh, P., Papadatou-Pastou, M., Perez-Albeniz, A., Pouyan, N., Roinishvili, M., Romanyuk, L., Salgado Montejo, A., Schrag, Y., Sultanova, A., Uuskula, M., Vainio, S., Wasowicz, G., Zdravkovic, S., Zhang, M., Mohr, C., and Bogushevskaya V. (ORCID:0000-0001-8147-3800)
- Abstract
Many of us “see red,” “feel blue,” or “turn green with envy.” Are such color-emotion associations fundamental to our shared cognitive architecture, or are they cultural creations learned through our languages and traditions? To answer these questions, we tested emotional associations of colors in 4,598 participants from 30 nations speaking 22 native languages. Participants associated 20 emotion concepts with 12 color terms. Pattern-similarity analyses revealed universal color-emotion associations (average similarity coefficient r =.88). However, local differences were also apparent. A machine-learning algorithm revealed that nation predicted color-emotion associations above and beyond those observed universally. Similarity was greater when nations were linguistically or geographically close. This study highlights robust universal color-emotion associations, further modulated by linguistic and geographic factors. These results pose further theoretical and empirical questions about the affective properties of color and may inform practice in applied domains, such as well-being and design.
- Published
- 2020
7. A machine learning approach to quantifying the specificity of colour–emotion associations and their cultural differences
- Author
-
Jonauskaite, D, Wicker, J, Mohr, C, Dael, N, Havelka, J, Papadatou-Pastou, M, Zhang, M, and Oberfeld, D
- Abstract
The link between colour and emotion and its possible similarity across cultures are questions that have not been fully resolved. Online, 711 participants from China, Germany, Greece and the UK associated 12 colour terms with 20 discrete emotion terms in their native languages. We propose a machine learning approach to quantify (a) the consistency and specificity of colour–emotion associations and (b) the degree to which they are country-specific, on the basis of the accuracy of a statistical classifier in (a) decoding the colour term evaluated on a given trial from the 20 ratings of colour–emotion associations and (b) predicting the country of origin from the 240 individual colour–emotion associations, respectively. The classifier accuracies were significantly above chance level, demonstrating that emotion associations are to some extent colour-specific and that colour–emotion associations are to some extent country-specific. A second measure of country-specificity, the in-group advantage of the colour-decoding accuracy, was detectable but relatively small (6.1%), indicating that colour–emotion associations are both universal and culture-specific. Our results show that machine learning is a promising tool when analysing complex datasets from emotion research
- Published
- 2019
8. The sun is no fun without rain: Physical environments affect how we feel about yellow across 55 countries
- Author
-
Jonauskaite, D. Abdel-Khalek, A.M. Abu-Akel, A. Al-Rasheed, A.S. Antonietti, J.-P. Ásgeirsson, Á.G. Atitsogbe, K.A. Barma, M. Barratt, D. Bogushevskaya, V. Bouayed Meziane, M.K. Chamseddine, A. Charernboom, T. Chkonia, E. Ciobanu, T. Corona, V. Creed, A. Dael, N. Daouk, H. Dimitrova, N. Doorenbos, C.B. Fomins, S. Fonseca-Pedrero, E. Gaspar, A. Gizdic, A. Griber, Y.A. Grimshaw, G.M. Hasan, A.A. Havelka, J. Hirnstein, M. Karlsson, B.S.A. Katembu, S. Kim, J. Konstantinou, N. Laurent, E. Lindeman, M. Manav, B. Marquardt, L. Mefoh, P. Mroczko-Wąsowicz, A. Mutandwa, P. Ngabolo, G. Oberfeld, D. Papadatou-Pastou, M. Perchtold, C.M. Pérez-Albéniz, A. Pouyan, N. Rashid Soron, T. Roinishvili, M. Romanyuk, L. Salgado Montejo, A. Sultanova, A. Tau, R. Uusküla, M. Vainio, S. Vargas-Soto, V. Volkan, E. Wąsowicz, G. Zdravković, S. Zhang, M. Mohr, C.
- Abstract
Across cultures, people associate colours with emotions. Here, we test the hypothesis that one driver of this cross-modal correspondence is the physical environment we live in. We focus on a prime example – the association of yellow with joy, – which conceivably arises because yellow is reminiscent of life-sustaining sunshine and pleasant weather. If so, this association should be especially strong in countries where sunny weather is a rare occurrence. We analysed yellow-joy associations of 6625 participants from 55 countries to investigate how yellow-joy associations varied geographically, climatologically, and seasonally. We assessed the distance to the equator, sunshine, precipitation, and daytime hours. Consistent with our hypotheses, participants who live further away from the equator and in rainier countries are more likely to associate yellow with joy. We did not find associations with seasonal variations. Our findings support a role for the physical environment in shaping the affective meaning of colour. © 2019
- Published
- 2019
9. A machine learning approach to quantify the specificity of colour–emotion associations and their cultural differences
- Author
-
Jonauskaite, D. Wicker, J. Mohr, C. Dael, N. Havelka, J. Papadatou-Pastou, M. Zhang, M. Oberfeld, D.
- Abstract
The link between colour and emotion and its possible similarity across cultures are questions that have not been fully resolved. Online, 711 participants from China, Germany, Greece and the UK associated 12 colour terms with 20 discrete emotion terms in their native languages. We propose a machine learning approach to quantify (a) the consistency and specificity of colour–emotion associations and (b) the degree to which they are country-specific, on the basis of the accuracy of a statistical classifier in (a) decoding the colour term evaluated on a given trial from the 20 ratings of colour–emotion associations and (b) predicting the country of origin from the 240 individual colour–emotion associations, respectively. The classifier accuracies were significantly above chance level, demonstrating that emotion associations are to some extent colour-specific and that colour–emotion associations are to some extent country-specific. A second measure of country-specificity, the in-group advantage of the colour-decoding accuracy, was detectable but relatively small (6.1%), indicating that colour–emotion associations are both universal and culture-specific. Our results show that machine learning is a promising tool when analysing complex datasets from emotion research. © 2019 The Authors. Published by the Royal Society under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/, which permits unrestricted use, provided the original author and source are credited.
- Published
- 2019
10. The sun is no fun without rain: Physical environments affect how we feel about yellow across 55 countries
- Author
-
Jonauskaite, D, Abdel-Khalek, AM, Abu-Akel, A, Al-Rasheed, AS, Antonietti, J-P, Asgeirsson, AG, Atitsogbe, KA, Barma, M, Barratt, D, Bogushevskaya, V, Meziane, MKB, Chamseddine, A, Charernboom, T, Chkonia, E, Ciobanu, T, Corona, V, Creed, A, Dael, N, Daouk, H, Dimitrova, N, Doorenbos, CB, Fomins, S, Fonseca-Pedrero, E, Gaspar, A, Gizdic, A, Griber, YA, Grimshaw, GM, Hasan, AA, Havelka, J, Hirnstein, M, Karlsson, BSA, Katembu, S, Kim, J, Konstantinou, N, Laurent, E, Lindeman, M, Manav, B, Marquardt, L, Mefoh, P, Mroczko-Wasowicz, A, Mutandwa, P, Ngabolo, G, Oberfeld, D, Papadatou-Pastou, M, Perchtold, CM, Perez-Albeniz, A, Pouyan, N, Soron, TR, Roinishvili, M, Romanyuk, L, Montejo, AS, Sultanova, A, Tau, R, Uuskula, M, Vainio, S, Vargas-Soto, V, Volkan, E, Wasowicz, G, Zdravkovic, S, Zhang, M, Mohr, C, Jonauskaite, D, Abdel-Khalek, AM, Abu-Akel, A, Al-Rasheed, AS, Antonietti, J-P, Asgeirsson, AG, Atitsogbe, KA, Barma, M, Barratt, D, Bogushevskaya, V, Meziane, MKB, Chamseddine, A, Charernboom, T, Chkonia, E, Ciobanu, T, Corona, V, Creed, A, Dael, N, Daouk, H, Dimitrova, N, Doorenbos, CB, Fomins, S, Fonseca-Pedrero, E, Gaspar, A, Gizdic, A, Griber, YA, Grimshaw, GM, Hasan, AA, Havelka, J, Hirnstein, M, Karlsson, BSA, Katembu, S, Kim, J, Konstantinou, N, Laurent, E, Lindeman, M, Manav, B, Marquardt, L, Mefoh, P, Mroczko-Wasowicz, A, Mutandwa, P, Ngabolo, G, Oberfeld, D, Papadatou-Pastou, M, Perchtold, CM, Perez-Albeniz, A, Pouyan, N, Soron, TR, Roinishvili, M, Romanyuk, L, Montejo, AS, Sultanova, A, Tau, R, Uuskula, M, Vainio, S, Vargas-Soto, V, Volkan, E, Wasowicz, G, Zdravkovic, S, Zhang, M, and Mohr, C
- Abstract
Across cultures, people associate colours with emotions. Here, we test the hypothesis that one driver of this cross-modal correspondence is the physical environment we live in. We focus on a prime example – the association of yellow with joy, – which conceivably arises because yellow is reminiscent of life-sustaining sunshine and pleasant weather. If so, this association should be especially strong in countries where sunny weather is a rare occurrence. We analysed yellow-joy associations of 6625 participants from 55 countries to investigate how yellow-joy associations varied geographically, climatologically, and seasonally. We assessed the distance to the equator, sunshine, precipitation, and daytime hours. Consistent with our hypotheses, participants who live further away from the equator and in rainier countries are more likely to associate yellow with joy. We did not find associations with seasonal variations. Our findings support a role for the physical environment in shaping the affective meaning of colour.
- Published
- 2019
11. Color and time perception: Evidence for temporal overestimation of blue stimuli
- Author
-
Thönes, S., primary, von Castell, C., additional, Iflinger, J., additional, and Oberfeld, D., additional
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
12. Spatial weights in loudness judgements
- Author
-
Hots Jan, Oberfeld Daniel, and Verhey Jesko L.
- Subjects
psychoacoustics ,loudness ,spatial ,binaural perception ,perceptual weights ,Acoustics in engineering. Acoustical engineering ,TA365-367 ,Acoustics. Sound ,QC221-246 - Abstract
In everyday life, the auditory system often receives input from various sound sources at different positions in space more or less simultaneously. The present study investigates how listeners weight different spatial positions when judging the overall loudness of a spatially distributed sound field. Spatial weights were measured in two conditions. In both conditions, sounds were presented from five spatial positions: front, left, right, top and back, using five loudspeakers distributed spatially around the listener. In the first condition, bandpass noises were presented simultaneously on all five loudspeakers. The sounds emanating from the different loudspeakers did not overlap spectrally, to ensure that they could be differentiated from each other. In the second condition, a broadband noise was presented at each of the five spatial positions. In this condition, sounds from the five different positions were presented consecutively. In both conditions, listeners judged the overall loudness of the sound fields. Before measuring spatial weights, all sounds were equalised in loudness. Both conditions showed virtually identical spatial weights for all five spatial positions. Thus, the weight assigned to a specific sound source in judgments of the overall loudness of spatially distributed sound fields appears to be unaffected by the spatial position of the source.
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
13. Verbesserung der Präsentationskompetenz in der Lehre an deutschen Hochschulen: Entwicklung des Mainzer Verfahrens zur Peer-Evaluation studentischer Präsentationen (MPEP)
- Author
-
Kemper, Christoph, Mitschke, T., Rollett, W., Kemper, V., Oberfeld, D., Kemper, Christoph, Mitschke, T., Rollett, W., Kemper, V., and Oberfeld, D.
- Published
- 2016
14. Entwicklung eines Fragebogens für die Peer-Evaluation von studentischen Referaten
- Author
-
Mitschke, T., Oberfeld, D., Kemper, Christoph, Mitschke, T., Oberfeld, D., and Kemper, Christoph
- Published
- 2009
15. Visual discrimination of arrival times: Troublesome effects of stimuli and experimental regime
- Author
-
Landwehr, K., primary, Baures, R., additional, Oberfeld, D., additional, and Hecht, H., additional
- Published
- 2010
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
16. Tactile perceptual processes and their relationship to medically unexplained symptoms and health anxiety.
- Author
-
Katzer A, Oberfeld D, Hiller W, and Witthöft M
- Published
- 2011
17. Crossing a Multi-Lane Street: Irrelevant Cars Increase Unsafe Behavior
- Author
-
Cavallo Viola, Hecht Heiko, Oberfeld Daniel, and Robin Baurès
- Subjects
Microbiology ,QR1-502 ,Physiology ,QP1-981 ,Zoology ,QL1-991 - Abstract
Before crossing a road or an intersection, road users have to determine among the surrounding traffic whether or not they have enough time to safely complete their maneuver. Temporal judgments have been investigated for single cars approaching an intersection, however, close to nothing is known about how street-crossing decisions are being made when several vehicles are simultaneously approaching in two adjacent lanes. We conducted a simulator experiment in which observers indicated whether or not they had enough time to complete safe street crossing. Traffic gaps were presented either with a single or two oncoming cars on different lanes, in such a way that in all cases, only the shortest gap was taskrelevant. Nevertheless, street-crossing decisions were found to be also influenced by the task-irrelevant longer gap, observers being more willing to cross the street when having to judge two gaps simultaneously compared to only one gap. Consequences of this unsafe behavior are discussed.
- Published
- 2011
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
18. The Sun Is no Fun without Rain
- Author
-
Gina M. Grimshaw, Veronica Vargas-Soto, Thammanard Charernboom, Eric Laurent, Suvi Vainio, Tanjir Rashid Soron, Marodégueba jeannette Barma, Alejandro Salgado Montejo, Eliz Volkan, Domicele Jonauskaite, Mari Uusküla, Victoria Bogushevskaya, Bodil S.A. Karlsson, Sergejs Fomins, Nikos Konstantinou, Ramiro Tau, Jean-Philippe Antonietti, Kokou A. Atitsogbe, Alena Gizdic, Ahmed M. Abdel-Khalek, Eka Chkonia, Abdulrahman S. Al-Rasheed, Sunčica Zdravković, Cornelis B. Doorenbos, Eduardo Fonseca-Pedrero, Philip C. Mefoh, Amer Chamseddine, Ahmad Abu-Akel, Phillip Mutandwa, Nevena Dimitrova, Grażyna Wąsowicz, Teofil Ciobanu, Aygun Sultanova, Violeta Corona, Meng Zhang, Maya Roinishvili, Stephen Katembu, Jelena Havelka, Christine Mohr, Daniel Barratt, Árni Gunnar Ásgeirsson, Yulia A. Griber, Jejoong Kim, Georgette Ngabolo, Niloufar Pouyan, Banu Manav, Augusta Gaspar, Daniel Oberfeld, Corinna M. Perchtold, Nele Dael, Marjaana Lindeman, Alicia Pérez-Albéniz, Hassan Daouk, Maliha Khadidja Bouayed Meziane, Aleksandra Mroczko-Wąsowicz, Marco Hirnstein, Aya Ahmed Hasan, Marietta Papadatou-Pastou, Lyudmyla Romanyuk, Lynn Marquardt, Allison Creed, Department of Psychology and Logopedics, Jonauskaite, D., Abdel-Khalek, A. M., Abu-Akel, A., Al-Rasheed, A. S., Antonietti, J. -P., Asgeirsson, A. G., Atitsogbe, K. A., Barma, M., Barratt, D., Bogushevskaya, V., Bouayed Meziane, M. K., Chamseddine, A., Charernboom, T., Chkonia, E., Ciobanu, T., Corona, V., Creed, A., Dael, N., Daouk, H., Dimitrova, N., Doorenbos, C. B., Fomins, S., Fonseca-Pedrero, E., Gaspar, A., Gizdic, A., Griber, Y. A., Grimshaw, G. M., Hasan, A. A., Havelka, J., Hirnstein, M., Karlsson, B. S. A., Katembu, S., Kim, J., Konstantinou, N., Laurent, E., Lindeman, M., Manav, B., Marquardt, L., Mefoh, P., Mroczko-Wasowicz, A., Mutandwa, P., Ngabolo, G., Oberfeld, D., Papadatou-Pastou, M., Perchtold, C. M., Perez-Albeniz, A., Pouyan, N., Rashid Soron, T., Roinishvili, M., Romanyuk, L., Salgado Montejo, A., Sultanova, A., Tau, R., Uuskula, M., Vainio, S., Vargas-Soto, V., Volkan, E., Wasowicz, G., Zdravkovic, S., Zhang, M., and Mohr, C.
- Subjects
Social Psychology ,515 Psychology ,Climate ,Settore L-LIN/01 - GLOTTOLOGIA E LINGUISTICA ,Equator ,050109 social psychology ,Environment ,Affect (psychology) ,Medical and Health Sciences ,050105 experimental psychology ,Emotion, Cross-cultural ,Colour ,collection ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Applied Psychology ,Emotion ,behavior ,05 social sciences ,Cross-cultural ,color ,Affect ,Other Medical Sciences ,weather ,Settore L-OR/21 - LINGUE E LETTERATURE DELLA CINA E DELL'ASIA SUD-ORIENTALE ,Demography - Abstract
Across cultures, people associate colours with emotions. Here, we test the hypothesis that one driver of this cross-modal correspondence is the physical environment we live in. We focus on a prime example – the association of yellow with joy, – which conceivably arises because yellow is reminiscent of life-sustaining sunshine and pleasant weather. If so, this association should be especially strong in countries where sunny weather is a rare occurrence. We analysed yellow-joy associations of 6625 participants from 55 countries to investigate how yellow-joy associations varied geographically, climatologically, and seasonally. We assessed the distance to the equator, sunshine, precipitation, and daytime hours. Consistent with our hypotheses, participants who live further away from the equator and in rainier countries are more likely to associate yellow with joy. We did not find associations with seasonal variations. Our findings support a role for the physical environment in shaping the affective meaning of colour.
- Published
- 2019
19. A comparative analysis of colour-emotion associations in 16-88-year-old adults from 31 countries.
- Author
-
Jonauskaite D, Epicoco D, Al-Rasheed AS, Aruta JJBR, Bogushevskaya V, Brederoo SG, Corona V, Fomins S, Gizdic A, Griber YA, Havelka J, Hirnstein M, John G, Jopp DS, Karlsson B, Konstantinou N, Laurent É, Marquardt L, Mefoh PC, Oberfeld D, Papadatou-Pastou M, Perchtold-Stefan CM, Spagnulo GFM, Sultanova A, Tanaka T, Tengco-Pacquing MC, Uusküla M, Wąsowicz G, and Mohr C
- Subjects
- Adolescent, Humans, Aged, Young Adult, Adult, Middle Aged, Aged, 80 and over, Color, Color Perception, Arousal, Emotions, Affect
- Abstract
As people age, they tend to spend more time indoors, and the colours in their surroundings may significantly impact their mood and overall well-being. However, there is a lack of empirical evidence to provide informed guidance on colour choices, irrespective of age group. To work towards informed choices, we investigated whether the associations between colours and emotions observed in younger individuals also apply to older adults. We recruited 7393 participants, aged between 16 and 88 years and coming from 31 countries. Each participant associated 12 colour terms with 20 emotion concepts and rated the intensity of each associated emotion. Different age groups exhibited highly similar patterns of colour-emotion associations (average similarity coefficient of .97), with subtle yet meaningful age-related differences. Adolescents associated the greatest number but the least positively biased emotions with colours. Older participants associated a smaller number but more intense and more positive emotions with all colour terms, displaying a positivity effect. Age also predicted arousal and power biases, varying by colour. Findings suggest parallels in colour-emotion associations between younger and older adults, with subtle but significant age-related variations. Future studies should next assess whether colour-emotion associations reflect what people actually feel when exposed to colour., (© 2023 The British Psychological Society.)
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
20. A binary acceleration signal reduces overestimation in pedestrians' visual time-to-collision estimation for accelerating vehicles.
- Author
-
Wessels M and Oberfeld D
- Abstract
When a pedestrian intends to cross the street, it is essential for safe mobility to correctly estimate the arrival time (time-to-collision, TTC) of an approaching vehicle. However, visual perception of acceleration is rather imprecise. Previous studies consistently showed that humans (mostly) disregard acceleration, but judge the TTC for an object as if it were traveling at constant speed (first-order estimation), which is associated with overestimated TTCs for positively accelerating objects. In a traffic context, such TTC overestimation could motivate pedestrians to cross in front of an approaching vehicle, although the time remaining is not sufficiently long. Can a simple acceleration signal help improve visual TTC estimation for accelerating objects? The present study investigated whether a signal that only indicates whether a vehicle is accelerating or not can remove the first-order pattern of overestimated TTCs. In a virtual reality simulation, 26 participants estimated the TTC of vehicles that approached with constant velocity or accelerated, from the perspective of a pedestrian at the curb. In half of the experimental blocks, a light band on the windshield illuminated whenever the vehicle accelerated but remained deactivated when the vehicle travelled at a constant speed. In the other blocks, the light band never illuminated, regardless of whether or not the vehicle accelerated. Participants were informed about the light band function in each block. Without acceleration signal, the estimated TTCs for the accelerating vehicles were consistent with an erroneous first-order approximation. In blocks with acceleration signal, participants substantially changed their estimation strategy, so that TTC overestimations for accelerating vehicles were reduced. Our data suggest that a binary acceleration signal helps pedestrians to effectively reduce the TTC overestimation for accelerating vehicles and could therefore increase pedestrian safety., Competing Interests: The authors declare that they have no known competing financial interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence the work reported in this paper., (© 2024 The Authors.)
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
21. Is Recognition of Speech in Noise Related to Memory Disruption Caused by Irrelevant Sound?
- Author
-
Oberfeld D, Staab K, Kattner F, and Ellermeier W
- Subjects
- Humans, Male, Female, Young Adult, Adult, Speech Intelligibility, Attention physiology, Adolescent, Noise adverse effects, Speech Perception physiology, Memory, Short-Term physiology, Auditory Threshold, Perceptual Masking, Acoustic Stimulation, Recognition, Psychology
- Abstract
Listeners with normal audiometric thresholds show substantial variability in their ability to understand speech in noise (SiN). These individual differences have been reported to be associated with a range of auditory and cognitive abilities. The present study addresses the association between SiN processing and the individual susceptibility of short-term memory to auditory distraction (i.e., the irrelevant sound effect [ISE]). In a sample of 67 young adult participants with normal audiometric thresholds, we measured speech recognition performance in a spatial listening task with two interfering talkers (speech-in-speech identification), audiometric thresholds, binaural sensitivity to the temporal fine structure (interaural phase differences [IPD]), serial memory with and without interfering talkers, and self-reported noise sensitivity. Speech-in-speech processing was not significantly associated with the ISE. The most important predictors of high speech-in-speech recognition performance were a large short-term memory span, low IPD thresholds, bilaterally symmetrical audiometric thresholds, and low individual noise sensitivity. Surprisingly, the susceptibility of short-term memory to irrelevant sound accounted for a substantially smaller amount of variance in speech-in-speech processing than the nondisrupted short-term memory capacity. The data confirm the role of binaural sensitivity to the temporal fine structure, although its association to SiN recognition was weaker than in some previous studies. The inverse association between self-reported noise sensitivity and SiN processing deserves further investigation., Competing Interests: Data Availability StatementThe dataset used for the regression analyses is available at OSF.io (https://osf.io/daxvz/). The trial-by-trial data in the different experimental tasks are available from the corresponding author (DO), upon request. Declaration of Conflicting InterestsThe authors declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
22. Trial-by-trial feedback fails to improve the consideration of acceleration in visual time-to-collision estimation.
- Author
-
Wessels M, Hecht H, Huisman T, and Oberfeld D
- Subjects
- Humans, Acceleration, Computer Simulation, Feedback, Safety, Accidents, Traffic prevention & control, Pedestrians
- Abstract
When judging the time-to-collision (TTC) of visually presented accelerating vehicles, untrained observers do not adequately account for acceleration (second-order information). Instead, their estimations only rely on vehicle distance and velocity (first-order information). As a result, they systemically overestimate the TTC for accelerating objects, which represents a potential risk for pedestrians in traffic situations because it might trigger unsafe road-crossing behavior. Can training help reduce these estimation errors? In this study, we tested whether training with trial-by-trial feedback about the signed deviation of the estimated from the actual TTC can improve TTC estimation accuracy for accelerating vehicles. Using a prediction-motion paradigm, we measured the estimated TTCs of twenty participants for constant-velocity and accelerated vehicle approaches, from a pedestrian's perspective in a VR traffic simulation. The experiment included three blocks, of which only the second block provided trial-by-trial feedback about the TTC estimation accuracy. Participants adjusted their estimations during and after the feedback, but they failed to differentiate between accelerated and constant-velocity approaches. Thus, the feedback did not help them account for acceleration. The results suggest that a safety training program based on trial-by-trial feedback is not a promising countermeasure against pedestrians' erroneous TTC estimation for accelerating objects., Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist., (Copyright: © 2023 Wessels et al. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.)
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
23. Overestimated time-to-collision for quiet vehicles: Evidence from a study using a novel audiovisual virtual-reality system for traffic scenarios.
- Author
-
Oberfeld D, Wessels M, and Büttner D
- Subjects
- Accidents, Traffic, Automobiles, Humans, Safety, Sound, Walking, Pedestrians, Virtual Reality
- Abstract
To avoid collision, pedestrians intending to cross a road need to estimate the time-to-collision (TTC) of an approaching vehicle. Here, we present a novel interactive audiovisual virtual-reality system for investigating how the acoustic characteristics (loudness and engine type) of vehicles influence the TTC estimation. Using acoustic recordings of real vehicles as source signals, the dynamic spatial sound fields corresponding to a vehicle approaching in an urban setting are generated based on physical modeling of the sound propagation between vehicle and pedestrian and are presented via sound field synthesis. We studied TTC estimation for vehicles with internal combustion engine and for loudness-matched electric vehicles. The vehicle sound levels were varied by 10 dB, independently of the speed, presented TTC, and vehicle type. In an auditory-only condition, the cars were not visible, and lower loudness of the cars resulted in considerably longer TTC estimates. Importantly, the loudness of the cars also had a significant effect in the same direction on the TTC estimates in an audiovisual condition, where the cars were additionally visually presented via interactive virtual-reality simulations. Thus, pedestrians use auditory information when estimating TTC, even when full visual information is available. At equal loudness, the TTC judgments for electric and conventional vehicles were virtually identical, indicating that loudness has a stronger effect than spectral differences. Because TTC overestimations can result in risky road crossing decisions, the results imply that vehicle loudness should be considered as an important factor in pedestrian safety., (Copyright © 2022 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.)
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
24. Adapting to the pandemic: longitudinal effects of social restrictions on time perception and boredom during the Covid-19 pandemic in Germany.
- Author
-
Wessels M, Utegaliyev N, Bernhard C, Welsch R, Oberfeld D, Thönes S, and von Castell C
- Subjects
- Adaptation, Psychological, Adolescent, Adult, COVID-19 psychology, Female, Germany epidemiology, Humans, Incidence, Male, Time Factors, Young Adult, Boredom, COVID-19 epidemiology, COVID-19 prevention & control, Communicable Disease Control methods, Longitudinal Studies, Social Isolation psychology, Stress, Psychological psychology, Time Perception
- Abstract
With the Covid-19 pandemic, many governments introduced nationwide lockdowns that disrupted people's daily routines and promoted social isolation. We applied a longitudinal online survey to investigate the mid-term effects of the mandated restrictions on the perceived passage of time (PPT) and boredom during and after a strict lockdown in Germany. One week after the beginning of the lockdown in March 2020, respondents reported a slower PPT and increased boredom compared to the pre-pandemic level. However, in the course of the lockdown, PPT accelerated and boredom decreased again until August 2020. Then, in October 2020, when incidence rates sharply rose and new restrictions were introduced, we again observed a slight trend toward a slowing of PPT and an increase of boredom. Our data also show that as the pandemic progressed, respondents adjusted their predictions about the pandemic's duration substantially upward. In sum, our findings suggest that respondents adapted to the pandemic situation and anticipated it as the new "normal". Furthermore, we determined perceived boredom and the general emotional state to be predictive of PPT, while depressive symptoms played a minor role., (© 2022. The Author(s).)
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
25. Temporal loudness weights: Primacy effects, loudness dominance and their interaction.
- Author
-
Fischenich A, Hots J, Verhey J, Guldan J, and Oberfeld D
- Subjects
- Adult, Female, Humans, Judgment, Male, Noise, Sound, Young Adult, Acoustic Stimulation methods, Discrimination, Psychological physiology, Loudness Perception, Psychoacoustics
- Abstract
Loudness judgments of sounds varying in level across time show a non-uniform temporal weighting, with increased weights assigned to the beginning of the sound (primacy effect). In addition, higher weights are observed for temporal components that are higher in level than the remaining components (loudness dominance). In three experiments, sounds consisting of 100- or 475-ms Gaussian wideband noise segments with random level variations were presented and either none, the first, or a central temporal segment was amplified or attenuated. In Experiment 1, the sounds consisted of four 100-ms segments that were separated by 500-ms gaps. Previous experiments did not show a primacy effect in such a condition. In Experiment 2, four- or ten-100-ms-segment sounds without gaps between the segments were presented to examine the interaction between the primacy effect and level dominance. As expected, for the sounds with segments separated by gaps, no primacy effect was observed, but weights on amplified segments were increased and weights on attenuated segments were decreased. For the sounds with contiguous segments, a primacy effect as well as effects of relative level (similar to those in Experiment 1) were found. For attenuation, the data indicated no substantial interaction between the primacy effect and loudness dominance, whereas for amplification an interaction was present. In Experiment 3, sounds consisting of either four contiguous 100-ms or 475-ms segments, or four 100-ms segments separated by 500-ms gaps were presented. Effects of relative level were more pronounced for the contiguous sounds. Across all three experiments, the effects of relative level were more pronounced for attenuation. In addition, the effects of relative level showed a dependence on the position of the change in level, with opposite direction for attenuation compared to amplification. Some of the results are in accordance with explanations based on masking effects on auditory intensity resolution., Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
26. Visual and postural eye-height information is flexibly coupled in the perception of virtual environments.
- Author
-
von Castell C, Oberfeld D, and Hecht H
- Subjects
- Humans, Perception, Postural Balance, Posture, Visual Perception, Cues, Virtual Reality
- Abstract
We conducted two experiments to investigate how observers integrate postural and visual eye-height information when estimating the layout of interior space. In Experiment 1, we varied postural and visual eye-height information independently of each other in a virtual-reality setup. Observers estimated the width, depth, and height of simulated rooms. All dimensions were perceived as larger when the virtual visual eye-height corresponded to sitting on the floor as compared with standing upright. In contrast, the estimates remained widely unaffected by the observer's physical posture (likewise sitting vs. standing). In Experiment 2, we studied effects of the viewing condition (real vs. virtual rooms) and (in case of the virtual rooms) adaptation to congruence versus incongruence of visual and postural information. Both media yielded comparable results, which indicates that eye-height information is processed similarly in virtual and genuine reality. In addition, observers adapted to the (in)congruence of visual and postural cues. When we presented trials with congruent information first, both visual and postural cues had an effect on the estimates. However, when information was initially incongruent, observers mostly relied on visual cues, presumably relative to an internalized standard, and disregarded postural cues. Taken together, our results show that the integration of visual and postural eye-height information is situation-dependent. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
27. Temporal Loudness Weights Are Frequency Specific.
- Author
-
Fischenich A, Hots J, Verhey J, and Oberfeld D
- Abstract
Previous work showed that the beginning of a sound is more important for the perception of loudness than later parts. When a short silent gap of sufficient duration is inserted into a sound, this primacy effect reoccurs in the second sound part after the gap. The present study investigates whether this temporal weighting occurs independently for different frequency bands. Sounds consisting of two bandpass noises were presented in four different conditions: (1) a simultaneous gap in both bands, (2) a gap in only the lower frequency band, (3) a gap in only the higher frequency band, or (4) no gap. In all conditions, the temporal loudness weights showed a primacy effect at sound onset. For the frequency bands without a gap, the temporal weights decreased gradually across time, regardless of whether the other frequency band did or did not contain a gap. When a frequency band contained a gap, the weight at the onset of this band after the gap was increased. This reoccurrence of the primacy effect following the gap was again largely independent of whether or not the other band contained a gap. Thus, the results indicate that the temporal loudness weights are frequency specific., Competing Interests: The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest., (Copyright © 2021 Fischenich, Hots, Verhey and Oberfeld.)
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
28. Universal Patterns in Color-Emotion Associations Are Further Shaped by Linguistic and Geographic Proximity.
- Author
-
Jonauskaite D, Abu-Akel A, Dael N, Oberfeld D, Abdel-Khalek AM, Al-Rasheed AS, Antonietti JP, Bogushevskaya V, Chamseddine A, Chkonia E, Corona V, Fonseca-Pedrero E, Griber YA, Grimshaw G, Hasan AA, Havelka J, Hirnstein M, Karlsson BSA, Laurent E, Lindeman M, Marquardt L, Mefoh P, Papadatou-Pastou M, Pérez-Albéniz A, Pouyan N, Roinishvili M, Romanyuk L, Salgado Montejo A, Schrag Y, Sultanova A, Uusküla M, Vainio S, Wąsowicz G, Zdravković S, Zhang M, and Mohr C
- Subjects
- Color, Color Perception, Humans, Jealousy, Linguistics, Machine Learning, Emotions, Language
- Abstract
Many of us "see red," "feel blue," or "turn green with envy." Are such color-emotion associations fundamental to our shared cognitive architecture, or are they cultural creations learned through our languages and traditions? To answer these questions, we tested emotional associations of colors in 4,598 participants from 30 nations speaking 22 native languages. Participants associated 20 emotion concepts with 12 color terms. Pattern-similarity analyses revealed universal color-emotion associations (average similarity coefficient r = .88). However, local differences were also apparent. A machine-learning algorithm revealed that nation predicted color-emotion associations above and beyond those observed universally. Similarity was greater when nations were linguistically or geographically close. This study highlights robust universal color-emotion associations, further modulated by linguistic and geographic factors. These results pose further theoretical and empirical questions about the affective properties of color and may inform practice in applied domains, such as well-being and design.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
29. The effect of silent gaps on temporal weights in loudness judgments.
- Author
-
Fischenich A, Hots J, Verhey JL, and Oberfeld D
- Subjects
- Acoustic Stimulation, Humans, Loudness Perception, Judgment, Sound
- Abstract
Human loudness judgments of time-varying sounds show a non-uniform temporal weighting pattern with increased weights at the beginning of a sound. Four experiments were conducted to investigate whether this primacy effect reoccurs after a silent gap of an appropriate duration that is inserted into a level-fluctuating sound. In three of the experiments, contiguous sounds as well as sounds containing silent gaps of different durations were presented. The temporal loudness weights were compared between the sounds that contained a gap and the sounds without a gap. The data showed that with increasing gap duration an increasingly pronounced primacy effect reoccurred on the second sound part in the sense that a) the weights assigned to the first segments after the gap were increased compared to the conditions without a gap, and that b) the following weights again showed a decrease over time. This effect was statistically significant for gap durations of 350 ms and above. To investigate whether an attenuation in level can lead to the same results as a silent gap, segments in the middle part of a sound were attenuated in the fourth experiment, and the resulting weights were compared to conditions in which the middle segments were unattenuated or where a 700 ms silent gap was presented instead of the middle segments. An attenuation of 15 dB resulted in a significant reoccurrence of the primacy effect, although the effect was more pronounced for an attenuation of 30 dB and the silent gap. The results are discussed in the light of auditory nerve responses, masking effects on intensity resolution, and assumptions based on evidence integration processes., (Copyright © 2020 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.)
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
30. Wall patterns influence the perception of interior space.
- Author
-
von Castell C, Hecht H, and Oberfeld D
- Subjects
- Adolescent, Adult, Female, Humans, Male, Middle Aged, Orientation, Spatial, Photic Stimulation, Young Adult, Interior Design and Furnishings, Space Perception
- Abstract
The texture of an object's surface influences its perceived spatial extent. For example, Hermann von Helmholtz reported that a square patch with black and white stripes appears elongated perpendicular to the stripes' orientation. This time-honoured finding stands in contrast with more recent recommendations by interior-design experts who suggest that stripe wall patterns make rooms appear elongated in the direction parallel to the stripes' orientation. In a series of four experiments, we presented stripe wall patterns and varied the orientation of the stripes (horizontal vs. vertical) and their density (number of stripes per degree of visual angle). Subjects estimated the width and height of stereoscopically presented interior spaces. Stripe patterns with higher densities made rooms appear both wider and higher than did stripe patterns with lower densities or plain walls. In contrast to both the predictions from the Helmholtz-square and the design guidelines, this effect was only weakly modulated by pattern orientation, in the sense that rooms appeared elongated in the direction parallel to the stripes' orientation. We conclude that object-based texture effects cannot be generalised to interior space perception. For a room's perceived spatial extent, pattern density is more important than pattern orientation.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
31. Temporal weights in loudness: Investigation of the effects of background noise and sound level.
- Author
-
Fischenich A, Hots J, Verhey J, and Oberfeld D
- Subjects
- Acoustic Stimulation, Adolescent, Adult, Auditory Threshold physiology, Female, Humans, Male, Models, Psychological, Noise, Perceptual Masking physiology, Psychoacoustics, Sound, Time Factors, Young Adult, Loudness Perception physiology
- Abstract
Previous research has consistently shown that for sounds varying in intensity over time, the beginning of the sound is of higher importance for the perception of loudness than later parts (primacy effect). However, in all previous studies, the target sounds were presented in quiet, and at a fixed average sound level. In the present study, temporal loudness weights for a time-varying narrowband noise were investigated in the presence of a continuous bandpass-filtered background noise and the average sound levels of the target stimuli were varied across a range of 60 dB. Pronounced primacy effects were observed in all conditions and there were no significant differences between the temporal weights observed in the conditions in quiet and in background noise. Within the conditions in background noise, there was a significant effect of the sound level on the pattern of weights, which was mainly caused by a slight trend for increased weights at the end of the sounds ("recency effect") in the condition with lower average level. No such effect was observed for the in-quiet conditions. Taken together, the observed primacy effect is largely independent of masking as well as of sound level. Compatible with this conclusion, the observed primacy effects in quiet and in background noise can be well described by an exponential decay function using parameters based on previous studies. Simulations using a model for the partial loudness of time-varying sounds in background noise showed that the model does not predict the observed temporal loudness weights., Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
32. A machine learning approach to quantify the specificity of colour-emotion associations and their cultural differences.
- Author
-
Jonauskaite D, Wicker J, Mohr C, Dael N, Havelka J, Papadatou-Pastou M, Zhang M, and Oberfeld D
- Abstract
The link between colour and emotion and its possible similarity across cultures are questions that have not been fully resolved. Online, 711 participants from China, Germany, Greece and the UK associated 12 colour terms with 20 discrete emotion terms in their native languages. We propose a machine learning approach to quantify (a) the consistency and specificity of colour-emotion associations and (b) the degree to which they are country-specific, on the basis of the accuracy of a statistical classifier in (a) decoding the colour term evaluated on a given trial from the 20 ratings of colour-emotion associations and (b) predicting the country of origin from the 240 individual colour-emotion associations, respectively. The classifier accuracies were significantly above chance level, demonstrating that emotion associations are to some extent colour-specific and that colour-emotion associations are to some extent country-specific. A second measure of country-specificity, the in-group advantage of the colour-decoding accuracy, was detectable but relatively small (6.1%), indicating that colour-emotion associations are both universal and culture-specific. Our results show that machine learning is a promising tool when analysing complex datasets from emotion research., Competing Interests: The authors declare that they have no competing interests., (© 2019 The Authors.)
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
33. Which Attribute of Ceiling Color Influences Perceived Room Height?
- Author
-
von Castell C, Hecht H, and Oberfeld D
- Subjects
- Adult, Humans, Color Perception physiology, Contrast Sensitivity physiology, Size Perception physiology
- Abstract
Objective: We investigate effects of the hue, saturation, and luminance of ceiling color on the perceived height of interior spaces., Background: Previous studies have reported that the perceived height of an interior space is influenced by the luminance of the ceiling, but not by the luminance contrast between ceiling and walls: brighter ceilings appeared higher than darker ceilings, irrespective of wall and floor luminance. However, these studies used solely achromatic colors. We report an experiment in which we extend these findings to effects of chromatic ceiling colors., Methods: We presented stereoscopic room simulations on a head-mounted display (Oculus Rift DK2) and varied hue (red, green, blue), saturation (low, high), and luminance (bright, dark) of the ceiling independently of each other., Results: We found the previously reported ceiling luminance effect to apply also to chromatic colors: subjects judged brighter ceilings to be higher than darker ceilings. The remaining color dimensions merely had a very small (hue) or virtually no effect (saturation) on perceived height., Conclusion: In order to maximize the perceived height of an interior space, we suggest painting the ceiling in the brightest possible color. The hue and saturation of the paint are only of minor importance., Application: The present study improves the empirical basis for interior design guidelines regarding effects of surface color on the perceived layout of interior spaces.
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
34. Bright paint makes interior-space surfaces appear farther away.
- Author
-
von Castell C, Hecht H, and Oberfeld D
- Subjects
- Adult, Analysis of Variance, Female, Humans, Lighting, Male, Young Adult, Color, Color Perception physiology, Interior Design and Furnishings methods, Paint, Space Perception physiology
- Abstract
Previous studies have reported that bright ceilings appear higher than dark ceilings, irrespective of the other colorimetric properties of the ceiling color (hue, saturation) and irrespective of the luminance of the remaining room surfaces (walls, floor). In the present study, we expand these findings to width and depth estimates. We presented stereoscopic full-scale room simulations on a head-mounted display and varied the luminance of the side walls, rear wall, and ceiling independently of each other. Participants judged the width and depth of the simulated rooms. Our results show that the perceived spatial layout of a given room is significantly influenced by the luminance of the direct bounding surfaces (e.g., the side walls when judging perceived width) but less affected by the luminance of the other surfaces. In the discussion, we provide an overall picture of effects of surface luminance on the perceived layout of interior spaces and discuss the conclusions in the context of existing interior-design guidelines., Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
35. Color and emotion: effects of hue, saturation, and brightness.
- Author
-
Wilms L and Oberfeld D
- Subjects
- Adult, Arousal physiology, Female, Galvanic Skin Response physiology, Heart Rate physiology, Humans, Male, Middle Aged, Photic Stimulation, Young Adult, Color Perception physiology, Emotions physiology
- Abstract
Previous studies on emotional effects of color often failed to control all the three perceptual dimensions of color: hue, saturation, and brightness. Here, we presented a three-dimensional space of chromatic colors by independently varying hue (blue, green, red), saturation (low, medium, high), and brightness (dark, medium, bright) in a factorial design. The 27 chromatic colors, plus 3 brightness-matched achromatic colors, were presented via an LED display. Participants (N = 62) viewed each color for 30 s and then rated their current emotional state (valence and arousal). Skin conductance and heart rate were measured continuously. The emotion ratings showed that saturated and bright colors were associated with higher arousal. The hue also had a significant effect on arousal, which increased from blue and green to red. The ratings of valence were the highest for saturated and bright colors, and also depended on the hue. Several interaction effects of the three color dimensions were observed for both arousal and valence. For instance, the valence ratings were higher for blue than for the remaining hues, but only for highly saturated colors. Saturated and bright colors caused significantly stronger skin conductance responses. Achromatic colors resulted in a short-term deceleration in the heart rate, while chromatic colors caused an acceleration. The results confirm that color stimuli have effects on the emotional state of the observer. These effects are not only determined by the hue of a color, as is often assumed, but by all the three color dimensions as well as their interactions.
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
36. Evaluation of a model of temporal weights in loudness judgments.
- Author
-
Oberfeld D, Jung L, Verhey JL, and Hots J
- Abstract
The onset of a sound receives a higher weight than later portions in time when its loudness is assessed, an effect commonly referred to as primacy effect. It is investigated if this effect can be predicted on the basis of an exponentially decaying function where the weight assigned to a temporal portion of a sound is the integral of this function over the segment duration. To test this model, temporal loudness weights were measured for sounds with different segment durations and total durations. The model successfully predicted essential aspects of the data.
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
37. Temporal weights in the perception of sound intensity: Effects of sound duration and number of temporal segments.
- Author
-
Oberfeld D, Hots J, and Verhey JL
- Abstract
Loudness is a fundamental aspect of auditory perception that is closely related to the physical level of the sound. However, it has been demonstrated that, in contrast to a sound level meter, human listeners do not weight all temporal segments of a sound equally. Instead, the beginning of a sound is more important for loudness estimation than later temporal portions. The present study investigates the mechanism underlying this primacy effect by varying the number of equal-duration temporal segments (5 and 20) and the total duration of the sound (1.0 to 10.0 s) in a factorial design. Pronounced primacy effects were observed for all 20-segment sounds. The temporal weights for the five-segment sounds are similar to those for the 20-segment sounds when the weights of the segments covering the same temporal range as a segment of the five-segment sounds are averaged. The primacy effect can be described by an exponential decay function with a time constant of about 200 ms. Thus, the temporal weight assigned to a specific temporal portion of a sound is determined by the time delay between sound onset and segment onset rather than by the number of segments or the total duration of the sound.
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
38. Meta-analysis of time perception and temporal processing in schizophrenia: Differential effects on precision and accuracy.
- Author
-
Thoenes S and Oberfeld D
- Subjects
- Humans, Judgment physiology, Schizophrenic Psychology, Time Perception physiology
- Abstract
Numerous studies have reported that time perception and temporal processing are impaired in schizophrenia. In a meta-analytical review, we differentiate between time perception (judgments of time intervals) and basic temporal processing (e.g., judgments of temporal order) as well as between effects on accuracy (deviation of estimates from the veridical value) and precision (variability of judgments). In a meta-regression approach, we also included the specific tasks and the different time interval ranges as covariates. We considered 68 publications of the past 65years, and meta-analyzed data from 957 patients with schizophrenia and 1060 healthy control participants. Independent of tasks and interval durations, our results demonstrate that time perception and basic temporal processing are less precise (more variable) in patients (Hedges' g>1.00), whereas effects of schizophrenia on accuracy of time perception are rather small and task-dependent. Our review also shows that several aspects, e.g., potential influences of medication, have not yet been investigated in sufficient detail. In conclusion, the results are in accordance with theoretical assumptions and the notion of a more variable internal clock in patients with schizophrenia, but not with a strong effect of schizophrenia on clock speed. The impairment of temporal precision, however, may also be clock-unspecific as part of a general cognitive deficiency in schizophrenia., (Copyright © 2017 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.)
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
39. Vection is the main contributor to motion sickness induced by visual yaw rotation: Implications for conflict and eye movement theories.
- Author
-
Nooij SA, Pretto P, Oberfeld D, Hecht H, and Bülthoff HH
- Subjects
- Adult, Female, Head Movements physiology, Humans, Male, Middle Aged, Models, Biological, Motion, Motion Sickness physiopathology, Nystagmus, Optokinetic physiology, Rotation adverse effects, Visual Perception physiology, Young Adult, Eye Movements physiology, Motion Sickness etiology
- Abstract
This study investigated the role of vection (i.e., a visually induced sense of self-motion), optokinetic nystagmus (OKN), and inadvertent head movements in visually induced motion sickness (VIMS), evoked by yaw rotation of the visual surround. These three elements have all been proposed as contributing factors in VIMS, as they can be linked to different motion sickness theories. However, a full understanding of the role of each factor is still lacking because independent manipulation has proven difficult in the past. We adopted an integrative approach to the problem by obtaining measures of potentially relevant parameters in four experimental conditions and subsequently combining them in a linear mixed regression model. To that end, participants were exposed to visual yaw rotation in four separate sessions. Using a full factorial design, the OKN was manipulated by a fixation target (present/absent), and vection strength by introducing a conflict in the motion direction of the central and peripheral field of view (present/absent). In all conditions, head movements were minimized as much as possible. Measured parameters included vection strength, vection variability, OKN slow phase velocity, OKN frequency, the number of inadvertent head movements, and inadvertent head tilt. Results show that VIMS increases with vection strength, but that this relation varies among participants (R2 = 0.48). Regression parameters for vection variability, head and eye movement parameters were not significant. These results may seem to be in line with the Sensory Conflict theory on motion sickness, but we argue that a more detailed definition of the exact nature of the conflict is required to fully appreciate the relationship between vection and VIMS.
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
40. Estimating the relative weights of visual and auditory tau versus heuristic-based cues for time-to-contact judgments in realistic, familiar scenes by older and younger adults.
- Author
-
Keshavarz B, Campos JL, DeLucia PR, and Oberfeld D
- Subjects
- Adolescent, Adult, Age Factors, Aged, Female, Humans, Judgment, Male, Young Adult, Auditory Perception physiology, Cues, Heuristics, Time Perception physiology, Visual Perception physiology
- Abstract
Estimating time to contact (TTC) involves multiple sensory systems, including vision and audition. Previous findings suggested that the ratio of an object's instantaneous optical size/sound intensity to its instantaneous rate of change in optical size/sound intensity (τ) drives TTC judgments. Other evidence has shown that heuristic-based cues are used, including final optical size or final sound pressure level. Most previous studies have used decontextualized and unfamiliar stimuli (e.g., geometric shapes on a blank background). Here we evaluated TTC estimates by using a traffic scene with an approaching vehicle to evaluate the weights of visual and auditory TTC cues under more realistic conditions. Younger (18-39 years) and older (65+ years) participants made TTC estimates in three sensory conditions: visual-only, auditory-only, and audio-visual. Stimuli were presented within an immersive virtual-reality environment, and cue weights were calculated for both visual cues (e.g., visual τ, final optical size) and auditory cues (e.g., auditory τ, final sound pressure level). The results demonstrated the use of visual τ as well as heuristic cues in the visual-only condition. TTC estimates in the auditory-only condition, however, were primarily based on an auditory heuristic cue (final sound pressure level), rather than on auditory τ. In the audio-visual condition, the visual cues dominated overall, with the highest weight being assigned to visual τ by younger adults, and a more equal weighting of visual τ and heuristic cues in older adults. Overall, better characterizing the effects of combined sensory inputs, stimulus characteristics, and age on the cues used to estimate TTC will provide important insights into how these factors may affect everyday behavior.
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
41. Measuring perceived ceiling height in a visual comparison task.
- Author
-
von Castell C, Hecht H, and Oberfeld D
- Subjects
- Adolescent, Adult, Analysis of Variance, Female, Humans, Male, Middle Aged, Photic Stimulation, Psychometrics, Psychophysics, Young Adult, Distance Perception physiology, Judgment physiology, Space Perception physiology
- Abstract
When judging interior space, a dark ceiling is judged to be lower than a light ceiling. The method of metric judgments (e.g., on a centimetre scale) that has typically been used in such tasks may reflect a genuine perceptual effect or it may reflect a cognitively mediated impression. We employed a height-matching method in which perceived ceiling height had to be matched with an adjustable pillar, thus obtaining psychometric functions that allowed for an estimation of the point of subjective equality (PSE) and the difference limen (DL). The height-matching method developed in this paper allows for a direct visual match and does not require metric judgment. It has the added advantage of providing superior precision. Experiment 1 used ceiling heights between 2.90 m and 3.00 m. The PSE proved sensitive to slight changes in perceived ceiling height. The DL was about 3% of the physical ceiling height. Experiment 2 found similar results for lower (2.30 m to 2.50 m) and higher (3.30 m to 3.50 m) ceilings. In Experiment 3, we additionally varied ceiling lightness (light grey vs. dark grey). The height matches showed that the light ceiling appeared significantly higher than the darker ceiling. We therefore attribute the influence of ceiling lightness on perceived ceiling height to a direct perceptual rather than a cognitive effect.
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
42. Asymmetric interference in concurrent time-to-contact estimation: Cousin or twin of the psychological refractory period effect?
- Author
-
Baurès R, DeLucia PR, Olson M, and Oberfeld D
- Subjects
- Adult, Female, Humans, Male, Young Adult, Photic Stimulation methods, Psychomotor Performance physiology, Reaction Time physiology, Refractory Period, Psychological physiology, Time Perception physiology
- Abstract
In a reaction time (RT) task requiring fast responses to two stimuli presented close in time, human observers show a delayed RT to the second stimulus. This phenomenon has been attributed to a psychological refractory period (PRP). A similar asymmetric interference is found when performing multiple concurrent visual time-to-contact (TTC) estimations for moving objects, despite important differences between the tasks. In the present study, we studied the properties of the asymmetric interference found in the TTC task and compared them to the classical PRP effect. In Experiment 1, we varied the time interval between the two objects' arrival times to determine the dependence of the PRP-like effect on the asynchrony between the two TTCs. In Experiment 2, we investigated whether the physical or the perceived arrival order determined the asymmetric interference. Our results confirmed the existence of asymmetric interference in the multiple TTC estimation task, but also indicated important differences from the traditional PRP effect observed in the RT paradigm. The origins of these differences are discussed, as well as the practical implications.
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
43. Effects of Adjacent Vehicles on Judgments of a Lead Car During Car Following.
- Author
-
Levulis SJ, DeLucia PR, and Oberfeld D
- Subjects
- Adult, Deceleration, Humans, Judgment, Automobile Driving, Motion Perception physiology, Psychomotor Performance physiology
- Abstract
Objective: Two experiments were conducted to determine whether detection of the onset of a lead car's deceleration and judgments of its time to contact (TTC) were affected by the presence of vehicles in lanes adjacent to the lead car., Background: In a previous study, TTC judgments of an approaching object by a stationary observer were influenced by an adjacent task-irrelevant approaching object. The implication is that vehicles in lanes adjacent to a lead car could influence a driver's ability to detect the lead car's deceleration and to make judgments of its TTC., Method: Displays simulated car-following scenes in which two vehicles in adjacent lanes were either present or absent. Participants were instructed to respond as soon as the lead car decelerated (Experiment 1) or when they thought their car would hit the decelerating lead car (Experiment 2)., Results: The presence of adjacent vehicles did not affect response time to detect deceleration of a lead car but did affect the signal detection theory measure of sensitivity d' and the number of missed deceleration events. Judgments of the lead car's TTC were shorter when adjacent vehicles were present and decelerated early than when adjacent vehicles were absent., Conclusion: The presence of vehicles in nearby lanes can affect a driver's ability to detect a lead car's deceleration and to make subsequent judgments of its TTC., Application: Results suggest that nearby traffic can affect a driver's ability to accurately judge a lead car's motion in situations that pose risk for rear-end collisions., (© 2016, Human Factors and Ergonomics Society.)
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
44. Individual differences in selective attention predict speech identification at a cocktail party.
- Author
-
Oberfeld D and Klöckner-Nowotny F
- Subjects
- Adult, Female, Humans, Male, Young Adult, Attention, Individuality, Noise, Speech
- Abstract
Listeners with normal hearing show considerable individual differences in speech understanding when competing speakers are present, as in a crowded restaurant. Here, we show that one source of this variance are individual differences in the ability to focus selective attention on a target stimulus in the presence of distractors. In 50 young normal-hearing listeners, the performance in tasks measuring auditory and visual selective attention was associated with sentence identification in the presence of spatially separated competing speakers. Together, the measures of selective attention explained a similar proportion of variance as the binaural sensitivity for the acoustic temporal fine structure. Working memory span, age, and audiometric thresholds showed no significant association with speech understanding. These results suggest that a reduced ability to focus attention on a target is one reason why some listeners with normal hearing sensitivity have difficulty communicating in situations with background noise.
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
45. Audiovisual Integration of Time-to-Contact Information for Approaching Objects.
- Author
-
DeLucia PR, Preddy D, and Oberfeld D
- Subjects
- Adolescent, Cues, Female, Humans, Male, Psychophysics, Time Factors, Young Adult, Auditory Perception physiology, Psychomotor Performance physiology, Visual Perception physiology
- Abstract
Previous studies of time-to-collision (TTC) judgments of approaching objects focused on effectiveness of visual TTC information in the optical expansion pattern (e.g., visual tau, disparity). Fewer studies examined effectiveness of auditory TTC information in the pattern of increasing intensity (auditory tau), or measured integration of auditory and visual TTC information. Here, participants judged TTC of an approaching object presented in the visual or auditory modality, or both concurrently. TTC information provided by the modalities was jittered slightly against each other, so that auditory and visual TTC were not perfectly correlated. A psychophysical reverse correlation approach was used to estimate the influence of auditory and visual cues on TTC estimates. TTC estimates were shorter in the auditory than the visual condition. On average, TTC judgments in the audiovisual condition were not significantly different from judgments in the visual condition. However, multiple regression analyses showed that TTC estimates were based on both auditory and visual information. Although heuristic cues (final sound pressure level, final optical size) and more reliable information (relative rate of change in acoustic intensity, optical expansion) contributed to auditory and visual judgments, the effect of heuristics was greater in the auditory condition. Although auditory and visual information influenced judgments, concurrent presentation of both did not result in lower response variability compared to presentation of either one alone; there was no multimodal advantage. The relative weightings of heuristics and more reliable information differed between auditory and visual TTC judgments, and when both were available, visual information was weighted more heavily.
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
46. Temporal weighting of loudness: Comparison between two different psychophysical tasks.
- Author
-
Ponsot E, Susini P, and Oberfeld D
- Subjects
- Acoustic Stimulation, Adult, Analysis of Variance, Decision Making physiology, Female, Humans, Judgment physiology, Male, Noise, Perceptual Masking physiology, Psychological Tests, Young Adult, Loudness Perception physiology
- Abstract
Psychophysical studies on loudness have so far examined the temporal weighting of loudness solely in level-discrimination tasks. Typically, listeners were asked to discriminate hundreds of level-fluctuating sounds regarding their global loudness. Temporal weights, i.e., the importance of each temporal portion of the stimuli for the loudness judgment, were then estimated from listeners' responses. Consistent non-uniform "u-shaped" temporal weighting patterns were observed, with greater weights assigned to the first and the last temporal portions of the stimuli, revealing significant primacy and recency effects, respectively. In this study, the question was addressed whether the same weighting pattern could be found in a traditional loudness estimation task. Temporal loudness weights were compared between a level-discrimination (LD) task and an absolute magnitude estimation (AME) task. Stimuli were 3-s broadband noises consisting of 250-ms segments randomly varying in level. Listeners were asked to evaluate the global loudness of the stimuli by classifying them as "loud" or "soft" (LD), or by assigning a number representing their loudness (AME). Results showed non-uniform temporal weighting in both tasks, but also significant differences between the two tasks. An explanation based on the difference in complexity between the evaluation processes underlying each task is proposed.
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
47. Time perception in depression: a meta-analysis.
- Author
-
Thönes S and Oberfeld D
- Subjects
- Humans, Time Factors, Depression psychology, Depressive Disorder psychology, Time Perception
- Abstract
Background: Depressive patients frequently report to perceive time as going by very slowly. Potential effects of depression on duration judgments have been investigated mostly by means of four different time perception tasks: verbal time estimation, time production, time reproduction, and duration discrimination. Ratings of the subjective flow of time have also been obtained., Methods: By means of a classical random-effects meta-regression model and a robust variance estimation model, this meta-analysis aims at evaluating the inconsistent results from 16 previous studies on time perception in depression, representing data of 433 depressive patients and 485 healthy control subjects., Results: Depressive patients perceive time as going by less quickly relative to control subjects (g=0.66, p=0.033). However, the analyses showed no significant effects of depression in the four time perception tasks. There was a trend towards inferior time discrimination performance in depression (g=0.38, p=0.079). The meta-regression also showed no significant effects of interval duration. Thus, the lack of effects of depression on timing does not depend on interval duration. However, for time production, there was a tendency towards overproduction of short and underproduction of long durations in depressive patients compared to healthy controls., Limitations: Several aspects, such as influences of medication and the dopaminergic neurotransmitter system on time perception in depression, have not been investigated in sufficient detail yet and were therefore not addressed by this meta-analysis., Conclusions: Depression has medium effects on the subjective flow of time whereas duration judgments basically remain unaffected., (Copyright © 2015 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.)
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
48. The effect of furnishing on perceived spatial dimensions and spaciousness of interior space.
- Author
-
von Castell C, Oberfeld D, and Hecht H
- Subjects
- Adolescent, Adult, Female, Humans, Interior Design and Furnishings, Judgment, Male, Middle Aged, Young Adult, Distance Perception, Size Perception
- Abstract
Despite the ubiquity of interior space design, there is virtually no scientific research on the influence of furnishing on the perception of interior space. We conducted two experiments in which observers were asked to estimate the spatial dimensions (size of the room dimensions in meters and centimeters) and to judge subjective spaciousness of various rooms. Experiment 1 used true-to-scale model rooms with a square surface area. Furnishing affected both the perceived height and the spaciousness judgments. The furnished room was perceived as higher but less spacious. In Experiment 2, rooms with different square surface areas and constant physical height were presented in virtual reality. Furnishing affected neither the perceived spatial dimensions nor the perceived spaciousness. Possible reasons for this discrepancy, such as the influence of the presentation medium, are discussed. Moreover, our results suggest a compression of perceived height and depth with decreasing surface area of the room.
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
49. Depression does not affect time perception and time-to-contact estimation.
- Author
-
Oberfeld D, Thönes S, Palayoor BJ, and Hecht H
- Abstract
Depressed patients frequently report a subjective slowing of the passage of time. However, experimental demonstrations of altered time perception in depressed patients are not conclusive. We added a timed action task (time-to-contact estimation, TTC) and compared this indirect time perception task to the more direct classical methods of verbal time estimation, time production, and time reproduction. In the TTC estimation task, the deviations of the estimates from the veridical values (relative errors) revealed no differences between depressed patients (N= 22) and healthy controls (N= 22). Neither did the relative errors of the TTC estimates differ between groups. There was a weak trend toward higher variability of the estimates in depressed patients but only at the shortest TTC and at the fastest velocities. Time experience (subjective flow of time) as well as time perception in terms of interval timing (verbal estimation, time production, time reproduction) performed on the same subjects likewise failed to produce effects of depression. We conclude that the notion that depression has a sizeable effect on time perception cannot be maintained.
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
50. Why do forward maskers affect auditory intensity discrimination? Evidence from "molecular psychophysics".
- Author
-
Oberfeld D, Stahn P, and Kuta M
- Subjects
- Acoustic Stimulation, Adult, Auditory Threshold, Female, Humans, Male, Perceptual Masking, Young Adult, Loudness Perception, Psychoacoustics
- Abstract
Nonsimultaneous maskers can strongly impair performance in an auditory intensity discrimination task. Using methods of molecular psychophysics, we quantified the extent to which (1) a masker-induced impairment of the representation of target intensity (i.e., increase in internal noise) and (2) a systematic influence of the masker intensities on the decision variable contribute to these effects. In a two-interval intensity discrimination procedure, targets were presented in quiet, and combined with forward maskers. The lateralization of the maskers relative to the targets was varied via the interaural time difference. Intensity difference limens (DLs) were strongly elevated under forward masking but less with contralateral than with ipsilateral maskers. For most listeners and conditions, perceptual weights measuring the relation between the target and masker levels and the response in the intensity discrimination task were positive and significant. Higher perceptual weights assigned to the maskers corresponded to stronger elevations of the intensity DL. The maskers caused only a weak increase in internal noise, unrelated to target level and masker lateralization. The results indicate that the effects of forward masking on intensity discrimination are determined by an inclusion of the masker intensities in the decision variable, compatible with the hypothesis that the impairment in performance is to a large part caused by difficulties in directing selective attention to the targets. The effects of masker lateralization are evidence for top-down influences, and the observed positive signs of the masker weights suggest that the relevant mechanisms are located at higher processing stages rather than in the auditory periphery.
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.