6 results on '"Matthew MacLachlan"'
Search Results
2. Hidden patterns of insect establishment risk revealed from two centuries of alien species discoveries
- Author
-
Andrew M. Liebhold, Takehiko Yamanaka, Matthew MacLachlan, and Michael R. Springborn
- Subjects
Policy development ,Multidisciplinary ,Earth, Environmental, Ecological, and Space Sciences ,Ecology ,Economics ,media_common.quotation_subject ,fungi ,SciAdv r-articles ,food and beverages ,Introduced species ,Insect ,Bioeconomics ,Invasive species ,Geography ,ComputingMethodologies_DOCUMENTANDTEXTPROCESSING ,Alien species ,ComputingMilieux_MISCELLANEOUS ,media_common ,Research Article - Abstract
Description, Many non-native insect establishments are undetected—across regions, this estimated “establishment debt” ranges from 10 to 52%., Understanding the socioeconomic drivers of biological invasion informs policy development for curtailing future invasions. While early 20th-century plant trade expansions preceded increased establishments of plant pests in Northern America, increased establishments did not follow accelerating imports later that century. To explore this puzzle, we estimate the historical establishment of plant-feeding Hemiptera in Northern America as a function of historical U.S. imports of live plants from seven world regions. Delays between establishment and discovery are modeled using a previously unused proxy for dynamic discovery effort. By recovering the timing of pest arrivals from their historical discoveries, we disentangle the joint establishment-discovery process. We estimate long delays to discovery, which are partially attributable to the low detectability of less economically important insect species. We estimate that many introduced species remain undiscovered, ranging from around one-fifth for Eurasian regions to two-fifths for Central and South America.
- Published
- 2021
3. Characteristics and Outcomes of Endogenous Endophthalmitis
- Author
-
Dean Eliott, Thanos D. Papakostas, Scott M. Barb, Tavé van Zyl, Avni P. Finn, Matthew MacLachlan, and Bobeck S. Modjtahedi
- Subjects
Pars plana ,0303 health sciences ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Visual acuity ,genetic structures ,business.industry ,medicine.medical_treatment ,Vitrectomy ,Retrospective cohort study ,Odds ratio ,Eye infection ,eye diseases ,03 medical and health sciences ,Ophthalmology ,0302 clinical medicine ,medicine.anatomical_structure ,Statistical significance ,Internal medicine ,030221 ophthalmology & optometry ,Medicine ,Young adult ,medicine.symptom ,business ,030304 developmental biology - Abstract
Purpose To describe the characteristics and outcomes of endogenous endophthalmitis. Design Retrospective case series. Participants Patients with endogenous endophthalmitis. Methods A retrospective chart review of patients diagnosed with endogenous endophthalmitis between September 1, 2006, and November 1, 2014. Main Outcome Measures Clinical findings, treatments, microbial results, visual outcomes, and secondary ocular sequelae. Results Sixty-three patients (68 eyes) were diagnosed with endogenous endophthalmitis. Ocular symptoms were the first manifestation of disease in 76% of patients. Fungal and bacterial endophthalmitis were seen in 37% (n = 25) and 43% (n = 29) of eyes, respectively. In 47% of eyes (n = 32), the disease was associated with intravenous drug use. Eighteen percent of eyes (n = 12) underwent an initial pars plana vitrectomy (PPV) with intravitreal antibiotics, none of which required a secondary intervention for acute infection. Four percent of eyes (n = 3) received only systemic treatment. Seventy-eight percent of eyes (n = 53) underwent initial bedside aspirate with intravitreal injection of antibiotics (tap-and-injection), of which 55% (n = 29) required a secondary PPV. Of eyes that underwent secondary PPV after initially negative culture results from the tap-and-injection, 52% demonstrated positive culture results at the time of secondary PPV (n = 11/21) despite all but 1 having received appropriate antimicrobial coverage initially. Fifty-four percent of eyes (n = 37) experienced secondary ocular sequelae. Eyes that received initial tap-and-injection had statistically nonsignificant better average initial vision, but worse average vision at each follow-up interval, compared to PPV while being less likely to gain 2 lines or more of vision at every follow-up interval except 6 months, with the difference reaching statistical significance at 1 week (odds ratio = 0.014; P = 0.037). Eyes that underwent initial tap-and-injection were statistically significantly more likely to require a secondary PPV (55% vs. 0%; P = 0.0006) and had fewer positive microbial results (19% vs. 67%) than those that underwent initial PPV (P = 0.002). Conclusions Most eyes that received initial tap-and-injection eventually underwent a secondary PPV. Initial PPV may have an important role in management because it was associated with better diagnostic yield and a trend toward better visual outcomes.
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
4. Learning about a Moving Target in Resource Management: Optimal Bayesian Disease Control
- Author
-
Michael R. Springborn, Paul L. Fackler, and Matthew MacLachlan
- Subjects
Economics and Econometrics ,Mathematical optimization ,040301 veterinary sciences ,Computer science ,Adaptive management, bovine tuberculosis, disease control, Partially observable Markov decision process, uncertainty, Livestock Production/Industries, Resource /Energy Economics and Policy, Risk and Uncertainty ,Bioeconomics ,0403 veterinary science ,Resource (project management) ,disease testing ,0502 economics and business ,Statistics ,Resource management ,uncertainty ,dynamic programming ,density projection ,05 social sciences ,Partially observable Markov decision process ,04 agricultural and veterinary sciences ,Q18 ,POMDP ,Optimal control ,Agricultural and Biological Sciences (miscellaneous) ,Q57 ,System dynamics ,Dynamic programming ,Adaptive management ,C61 ,Agricultural Economics & Policy ,Applied Economics ,H41 ,Stochastic optimization ,050202 agricultural economics & policy - Abstract
Resource managers must often make difficult choices in the face of imperfectly observed and dynamically changing systems (e.g., livestock, fisheries, water, and invasive species). A rich set of techniques exists for identifying optimal choices when that uncertainty is assumed to be understood and irreducible. Standard optimization approaches, however, cannot address situations in which reducible uncertainty applies to either system behavior or environmental states. The adaptive management literature overcomes this limitation with tools for optimal learning, but has been limited to highly simplified models with state and action spaces that are discrete and small. We overcome this problem by using a recently developed extension of the Partially Observable Markov Decision Process (POMDP) framework to allow for learning about a continuous state. We illustrate this methodology by exploring optimal control of bovine tuberculosis in New Zealand cattle. Disease testing—the control variable—serves to identify herds for treatment and provides information on prevalence, which is both imperfectly observed and subject to change due to controllable and uncontrollable factors. We find substantial efficiency losses from both ignoring learning (standard stochastic optimization) and from simplifying system dynamics (to facilitate a typical, simple learning model), though the latter effect dominates in our setting. We also find that under an adaptive management approach, simplifying dynamics can lead to a belief trap in which information gathering ceases, beliefs become increasingly inaccurate, and losses abound.
- Published
- 2015
5. Accounting for behavioral responses during a flu epidemic using home television viewing
- Author
-
Michael R. Springborn, Matthew MacLachlan, Eli P. Fenichel, and Gerardo Chowell
- Subjects
Male ,Veterinary medicine ,Time Factors ,Health Behavior ,Proxy (climate) ,Developmental psychology ,Disease Outbreaks ,0302 clinical medicine ,2.5 Research design and methodologies (aetiology) ,Epidemic model ,Influenza A Virus ,A/H1N1 ,030212 general & internal medicine ,Social isolation ,Aetiology ,Child ,0303 health sciences ,Social distance ,Age Factors ,Contrast (statistics) ,3. Good health ,Behavioral modeling ,Infectious Diseases ,Social Isolation ,Medical Microbiology ,SIR ,Television ,Female ,Public Health ,medicine.symptom ,Research Article ,Human ,Adult ,Adolescent ,Social distancing ,Clinical Sciences ,Biology ,Basic Behavioral and Social Science ,Microbiology ,Vaccine Related ,03 medical and health sciences ,Age Distribution ,Behavioral and Social Science ,medicine ,Humans ,H1N1 Subtype ,Socioeconomic status ,Mexico ,030304 developmental biology ,Fixed effects model ,Influenza ,Emerging Infectious Diseases ,Socioeconomic Factors - Abstract
Background Theory suggests that individual behavioral responses impact the spread of flu-like illnesses, but this has been difficult to empirically characterize. Social distancing is an important component of behavioral response, though analyses have been limited by a lack of behavioral data. Our objective is to use media data to characterize social distancing behavior in order to empirically inform explanatory and predictive epidemiological models. Methods We use data on variation in home television viewing as a proxy for variation in time spent in the home and, by extension, contact. This behavioral proxy is imperfect but appealing since information on a rich and representative sample is collected using consistent techniques across time and most major cities. We study the April-May 2009 outbreak of A/H1N1 in Central Mexico and examine the dynamic behavioral response in aggregate and contrast the observed patterns of various demographic subgroups. We develop and calibrate a dynamic behavioral model of disease transmission informed by the proxy data on daily variation in contact rates and compare it to a standard (non-adaptive) model and a fixed effects model that crudely captures behavior. Results We find that after a demonstrable initial behavioral response (consistent with social distancing) at the onset of the outbreak, there was attenuation in the response before the conclusion of the public health intervention. We find substantial differences in the behavioral response across age subgroups and socioeconomic levels. We also find that the dynamic behavioral and fixed effects transmission models better account for variation in new confirmed cases, generate more stable estimates of the baseline rate of transmission over time and predict the number of new cases over a short horizon with substantially less error. Conclusions Results suggest that A/H1N1 had an innate transmission potential greater than previously thought but this was masked by behavioral responses. Observed differences in behavioral response across demographic groups indicate a potential benefit from targeting social distancing outreach efforts.
- Published
- 2015
6. Erratum To: Accounting for Behavioral Responses during a Flu Epidemic Using Home Television Viewing
- Author
-
Gerardo Chowell, Michael R. Springborn, Eli P. Fenichel, and Matthew MacLachlan
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,0301 basic medicine ,Veterinary medicine ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Television viewing ,Time Factors ,Adolescent ,Health Behavior ,030106 microbiology ,Alternative medicine ,Disease Outbreaks ,03 medical and health sciences ,Age Distribution ,Influenza A Virus, H1N1 Subtype ,0302 clinical medicine ,Influenza, Human ,Humans ,Medicine ,030212 general & internal medicine ,Child ,Mexico ,Medical education ,business.industry ,Age Factors ,Infectious Diseases ,Social Isolation ,Socioeconomic Factors ,Tropical medicine ,Female ,Television ,Public Health ,Erratum ,business - Abstract
Theory suggests that individual behavioral responses impact the spread of flu-like illnesses, but this has been difficult to empirically characterize. Social distancing is an important component of behavioral response, though analyses have been limited by a lack of behavioral data. Our objective is to use media data to characterize social distancing behavior in order to empirically inform explanatory and predictive epidemiological models.We use data on variation in home television viewing as a proxy for variation in time spent in the home and, by extension, contact. This behavioral proxy is imperfect but appealing since information on a rich and representative sample is collected using consistent techniques across time and most major cities. We study the April-May 2009 outbreak of A/H1N1 in Central Mexico and examine the dynamic behavioral response in aggregate and contrast the observed patterns of various demographic subgroups. We develop and calibrate a dynamic behavioral model of disease transmission informed by the proxy data on daily variation in contact rates and compare it to a standard (non-adaptive) model and a fixed effects model that crudely captures behavior.We find that after a demonstrable initial behavioral response (consistent with social distancing) at the onset of the outbreak, there was attenuation in the response before the conclusion of the public health intervention. We find substantial differences in the behavioral response across age subgroups and socioeconomic levels. We also find that the dynamic behavioral and fixed effects transmission models better account for variation in new confirmed cases, generate more stable estimates of the baseline rate of transmission over time and predict the number of new cases over a short horizon with substantially less error.Results suggest that A/H1N1 had an innate transmission potential greater than previously thought but this was masked by behavioral responses. Observed differences in behavioral response across demographic groups indicate a potential benefit from targeting social distancing outreach efforts.
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.