77 results on '"Martire, KA"'
Search Results
2. Judging experts: Australian magistrates’ evaluations of expert opinion quality
- Author
-
Martire, KA, Montgomery-Farrer, B, Martire, KA, and Montgomery-Farrer, B
- Abstract
Expert opinions admitted by courts are not always valid and reliable. However, we know little about how indicators of opinion quality affect the persuasiveness of an expert. In this study 25 Australian magistrates and 22 jury-eligible lay people rated the persuasiveness (via credibility, value and weight) of either a high- or a low-quality expert opinion. Opinion quality was determined using attributes specified in the Expert Persuasion Expectancy (ExPEx) framework: Field, Specialty, Ability and Trustworthiness. Both magistrates and jurors were significantly more persuaded by the high- than the low-quality expert opinion. Magistrates were also significantly more sceptical of the expert opinion than lay people, and when given the opportunity sought information that was logically relevant to their decision. These results suggest that magistrates can differentiate between high- and low-quality expert opinions, but it is unclear whether the information they need for the task is actually available for use during trials.
- Published
- 2020
3. The multi-faceted nature of visual statistical learning: Individual differences in learning conditional and distributional regularities across time and space
- Author
-
Growns, B, Siegelman, N, Martire, KA, Growns, B, Siegelman, N, and Martire, KA
- Abstract
Emerging research has demonstrated that statistical learning is a modality-specific ability governed by domain-general principles. Yet limited research has investigated different forms of statistical learning within modality. This paper explores whether there is one unified statistical learning mechanism within the visual modality, or separate task-specific abilities. To do so, we examined individual differences in spatial and nonspatial conditional and distributional statistical learning. Participants completed four visual statistical learning tasks: conditional spatial, conditional nonspatial, distributional spatial, and distributional nonspatial. Performance on all four tasks significantly correlated with each other, and performance on all tasks accounted for a large portion of the variance across tasks (57%). Interestingly, a portion of the variance of task performance (between 11% and 18%) was also accounted for by performance on each of the individual tasks. Our results suggest that visual statistical learning is the result of the interplay between a unified mechanism for extracting conditional and distributional statistical regularities across time and space, and an individual’s ability to extract specific types of regularities.
- Published
- 2020
4. Forensic feature-comparison expertise: Statistical learning facilitates visual comparison performance
- Author
-
Growns, B, Martire, KA, Growns, B, and Martire, KA
- Abstract
Forensic feature-comparison examiners in select disciplines are more accurate than novices when comparing samples of visual evidence. This article examines a key cognitive mechanism that may contribute to this superior visual comparison performance: the ability to learn how often stimuli occur in the environment (distributional statistical learning). We examined the relationship between distributional learning and visual comparison performance and the impact of training on the diagnosticity of distributional information in visual comparison tasks. We compared performance between novices given no training (uninformed novices; n = 32), accurate training (informed novices; n = 32), or inaccurate training (misinformed novices; n = 32) in Experiment 1 and between forensic examiners (n = 26), informed novices (n = 29), and uninformed novices (n = 27) in Experiment 2. Across both experiments, forensic examiners and novices performed significantly above chance in a visual comparison task in which distributional learning was required for high performance. However, informed novices outperformed all participants, and only their visual comparison performance was significantly associated with their distributional learning. It is likely that forensic examiners' expertise is domain specific and doesn't generalize to novel visual comparison tasks. Nevertheless, diagnosticity training could be critical to the relationship between distributional learning and visual comparison performance.
- Published
- 2020
5. Limited not lazy: a quasi-experimental secondary analysis of evidence quality evaluations by those who hold implausible beliefs
- Author
-
Martire, KA, Growns, B, Bali, AS, Montgomery-Farrer, B, Summersby, S, Younan, M, Martire, KA, Growns, B, Bali, AS, Montgomery-Farrer, B, Summersby, S, and Younan, M
- Abstract
Past research suggests that an uncritical or ‘lazy’ style of evaluating evidence may play a role in the development and maintenance of implausible beliefs. We examine this possibility by using a quasi-experimental design to compare how low- and high-quality evidence is evaluated by those who do and do not endorse implausible claims. Seven studies conducted during 2019–2020 provided the data for this analysis (N = 746). Each of the seven primary studies presented participants with high- and/or low-quality evidence and measured implausible claim endorsement and evaluations of evidence persuasiveness (via credibility, value, and/or weight). A linear mixed-effect model was used to predict persuasiveness from the interaction between implausible claim endorsement and evidence quality. Our results showed that endorsers were significantly more persuaded by the evidence than non-endorsers, but both groups were significantly more persuaded by high-quality than low-quality evidence. The interaction between endorsement and evidence quality was not significant. These results suggest that the formation and maintenance of implausible beliefs by endorsers may result from less critical evidence evaluations rather than a failure to analyse. This is consistent with a limited rather than a lazy approach and suggests that interventions to develop analytical skill may be useful for minimising the effects of implausible claims.
- Published
- 2020
6. Exploring juror evaluations of expert opinions using the Expert Persuasion Expectancy framework
- Author
-
Martire, KA, Edmond, G, Navarro, D, Martire, KA, Edmond, G, and Navarro, D
- Abstract
Purpose: Factfinders in trials struggle to differentiate witnesses who offer genuinely expert opinions from those who do not. The Expert Persuasion Expectancy (ExPEx) framework proposes eight attributes logically relevant to this assessment: foundation, field, specialty, ability, opinion, support, consistency, and trustworthiness. We present two experiments examining the effects of these attributes on the persuasiveness of a forensic gait analysis opinion. Methods: Jury-eligible participants rated the credibility, value, and weight of an expert report that was either generally strong (Exp. 1; N = 437) or generally weak (Exp. 2; N = 435). The quality of ExPEx attributes varied between participants. Allocation to condition (none, foundation, field, specialty, ability, opinion, support, consistency, trustworthiness) determined which attribute in the report would be weak (cf. strong; Exp. 1), or strong (cf. weak; Exp. 2). Results: In Experiment 1, the persuasiveness of a strong report was significantly undermined by weak versions of ability, consistency, and trustworthiness. In Experiment 2, a weak report was significantly improved by strong versions of ability and consistency. Unplanned analyses of subjective ratings also identified effects of foundation, field, specialty, and opinion. Conclusions: We found evidence that ability (i.e., personal proficiency), consistency (i.e., endorsement by other experts), and trustworthiness (i.e., objectivity) attributes influence opinion persuasiveness in logically appropriate ways. Ensuring that factfinders have information about these attributes may improve their assessments of expert opinion evidence.
- Published
- 2020
7. Communicating forensic science opinion: An examination of expert reporting practices
- Author
-
Bali, AS, Edmond, G, Ballantyne, KN, Kemp, RI, Martire, KA, Bali, AS, Edmond, G, Ballantyne, KN, Kemp, RI, and Martire, KA
- Abstract
Forensic scientists endeavour to explain complex scientific principles to legal decision-makers with limited scientific training (e.g., police, lawyers, judges, and jurors). Much of the time this communication is limited to written opinions in expert reports. Notwithstanding considerable scientific research and debate about the best way to communicate forensic science opinions, it is unclear how much of the advice has translated into forensic science practice. In conducting this descriptive study, we examined the reporting practices adopted by forensic scientists across a range of forensic science disciplines. Specifically, we used a quantitative content analysis approach to identify the conclusion types and additional information submitted by forensic scientists in proficiency tests during 2016 (“What would be the wording of the Conclusions in your report?”). Our analysis of 500 randomly selected responses in eight disciplines indicated that the conclusion type which has received the most criticism in recent years (categorical statements) remains the preferred means of expression in a clear majority of responses. We also found that the provision of additional information often considered necessary for rational evaluation of the evidence (e.g., information about reliability and validity) was rarely reported. These results suggest limited engagement with recent recommendations and are concerning given the gravity of the legal decisions that hinge on accurate and transparent forensic science communication.
- Published
- 2020
8. Forensic science evidence: Naive estimates of false positive error rates and reliability
- Author
-
Martire, KA, Ballantyne, KN, Bali, A, Edmond, G, Kemp, RI, Found, B, Martire, KA, Ballantyne, KN, Bali, A, Edmond, G, Kemp, RI, and Found, B
- Abstract
We do not know how often false positive reports are made in a range of forensic science disciplines. In the absence of this information it is important to understand the naive beliefs held by potential jurors about forensic science evidence reliability. It is these beliefs that will shape evaluations at trial. This descriptive study adds to our knowledge about naive beliefs by: (1) measuring jury-eligible (lay) perceptions of reliability for the largest range of forensic science disciplines to date, over three waves of data collection between 2011 and 2016 (n = 674); (2) calibrating reliability ratings with false positive report estimates; and (3) comparing lay reliability estimates with those of an opportunity sample of forensic practitioners (n = 53). Overall the data suggest that both jury-eligible participants and practitioners consider forensic evidence highly reliable. When compared to best or plausible estimates of reliability and error in the forensic sciences these views appear to overestimate reliability and underestimate the frequency of false positive errors. This result highlights the importance of collecting and disseminating empirically derived estimates of false positive error rates to ensure that practitioners and potential jurors have a realistic impression of the value of forensic science evidence.
- Published
- 2019
9. The effect of ambiguous question wording on jurors’ presumption of innocence
- Author
-
Martire, KA, Dahlman, C, Martire, KA, and Dahlman, C
- Published
- 2019
10. Are Forensic Scientists Experts?
- Author
-
Towler, A, White, D, Ballantyne, K, Searston, RA, Martire, KA, Kemp, RI, Towler, A, White, D, Ballantyne, K, Searston, RA, Martire, KA, and Kemp, RI
- Abstract
Despite playing a critical role in our criminal justice system, very little is known about the expertise of forensic scientists. Here, we review three disciplines where research has begun to investigate such expertise: handwriting analysis, fingerprint examination, and facial image comparison. We assess expertise against the scientific standard, but conclude that meeting this standard does not provide a sufficiently high benchmark for the forensic sciences. Forensic scientists must demonstrate a minimum standard of performance, the ability to defer judgement in cases at high risk of error, and the ability to effectively communicate the strength of their evidence to factfinders. We discuss the limitations of current forensic science expertise research to adequately capture factors affecting operational accuracy and outline crucial differences between studies assessing perceptual skill and operational accuracy. Finally, we identify key areas for future research and encourage cognitive scientists to engage in forensic science research.
- Published
- 2018
11. Clear communication through clear purpose: understanding statistical statements made by forensic scientists
- Author
-
Martire, KA and Martire, KA
- Abstract
Based on a plenary lecture presented at the 50th Anniversary of the Australian Academy of Forensic Sciences Symposium (November 2017) this article reviews the literature relating to the communication of forensic science evidence using statistical statements. Five dimensions of communication are identified: consistency, sensitivity, coherence, ability and orthodoxy. The review also considers the strengths and weaknesses of lay comprehension as well as opportunities for improving understanding through reflection and collaboration.
- Published
- 2018
12. Considerations when designing human performance tests in the forensic sciences
- Author
-
Martire, KA, Kemp, RI, Martire, KA, and Kemp, RI
- Published
- 2018
13. Expert Reports and the Forensic Sciences
- Author
-
Edmond, G., Martire, Ka, and Mehera San Roque
- Subjects
General Medicine - Published
- 2017
14. Knowing experts? Section 79, forensic science evidence and the limits of 'training, study or experience
- Author
-
Roberts, A, Gans, J, Edmond, G, Martire, KA, Roberts, A, Gans, J, Edmond, G, and Martire, KA
- Published
- 2017
15. Rethinking expert opinion evidence
- Author
-
Martire, KA, Edmond, G, Martire, KA, and Edmond, G
- Published
- 2017
16. Consistent with: What doctors say and jurors hear.
- Author
-
Ross, R, Martire, KA, Kramer, K, Ross, R, Martire, KA, and Kramer, K
- Published
- 2017
17. Expert reports and the forensic sciences
- Author
-
Edmond, G, Martire, KA, San Roque, M, Edmond, G, Martire, KA, and San Roque, M
- Published
- 2017
18. Antipodean forensics: a comment on ANZFSS's response to PCAST
- Author
-
Edmond, G, Martire, KA, Edmond, G, and Martire, KA
- Published
- 2017
19. Smoking and Finances: Baseline Characteristics of Low Income Daily Smokers in the FISCALS Cohort
- Author
-
Martire, KA, clare, P, courtney, R, bonevski, B, boland, V, borland, R, Doran, C, Farrell, M, hall, W, Iredale, J, siahpush, M, mattick, R, Martire, KA, clare, P, courtney, R, bonevski, B, boland, V, borland, R, Doran, C, Farrell, M, hall, W, Iredale, J, siahpush, M, and mattick, R
- Published
- 2017
20. On the likelihood of “encapsulating all uncertainty”
- Author
-
Martire, KA, Edmond, G, Navarro, D, Newell, B, Martire, KA, Edmond, G, Navarro, D, and Newell, B
- Published
- 2017
21. A comment on the PCAST report: Skip the “match”/“non-match” stage
- Author
-
Morrison, GS, Kaye, DH, Balding, DJ, Taylor, D, Dawid, P, Aitken, CGG, Gittelson, S, Zadora, G, Robertson, B, Willis, S, Pope, S, Neil, M, Martire, KA, Helper, A, Gil, RD, Jamieson, A, De Zoete, J, Ostrum, RB, Caliebe, A, Morrison, GS, Kaye, DH, Balding, DJ, Taylor, D, Dawid, P, Aitken, CGG, Gittelson, S, Zadora, G, Robertson, B, Willis, S, Pope, S, Neil, M, Martire, KA, Helper, A, Gil, RD, Jamieson, A, De Zoete, J, Ostrum, RB, and Caliebe, A
- Published
- 2017
22. Predictors of retention in a randomised trial of smoking cessation in low-socioeconomic status Australian smokers
- Author
-
Courtney, RJ, Clare, P, Boland, V, Martire, KA, Bonevski, B, Hall, W, Siahpush, M, Borland, R, Doran, CM, West, R, Farrell, M, Mattick, RP, Courtney, RJ, Clare, P, Boland, V, Martire, KA, Bonevski, B, Hall, W, Siahpush, M, Borland, R, Doran, CM, West, R, Farrell, M, and Mattick, RP
- Abstract
Background and aims Little is known about the factors associated with retention in smoking cessation trials, especially for low-socioeconomic status (low-SES) smokers. This study examined the factors associated with retention of low-SES smokers in the Australian Financial Interventions for Smoking Cessation Among Low-Income Smokers (FISCALS) trial. Design A two-group parallel block randomised open-label trial with allocation concealment. Setting Australia. The study was conducted primarily by telephone-based interviews with nicotine replacement therapy delivered via mail. Participants 1047 low-SES smokers interested in quitting smoking were randomised. Measurements Participants completed computer assisted telephone interviews (CATIs) at baseline, 2-month and 8-month follow-up. Smoking-related, substance use, mental or physical health, general psychological constructs, sociodemographic and recruitment sources association with retention at 8-month follow-up were examined using binary logistic regression. Findings 946 participants (90%) completed the 2-month follow-up interview and 880 participants (84%) completed the 8-month follow-up interview. Retention at 8-months was associated with higher motivation to quit (OR: 1.15; 95% CI: 1.04, 1.27 p < 0.01), more recent quit attempts (OR: 1.20; 95% CI: 1.04, 1.40 p < 0.05), increasing age (OR: 1.05; 95% CI: 1.03, 1.07 p < 0.01), and higher level of education (OR: 2.24; 95% CI: 1.45, 3.46 p < 0.01). Lower retention at 8-months occurred for those participants recruited from posters placed in Department of Human Service Centrelink Offices (OR: 0.56; 95% CI: 0.35, 0.89, p < 0.05) compared to participants recruited from Quitline services. No significant differences in retention were found for participants recruited via newspaper advertisements or word of mouth compared to Quitline services. No significant associations were found between health-related or behavioural factors and retention. Conclusions In the context of high overa
- Published
- 2017
23. Associations between behavioural risk factors and smoking, heavy smoking and future smoking among an Australian population-based sample
- Author
-
Iredale, JM, Clare, PJ, Courtney, RJ, Martire, KA, Bonevski, B, Borland, R, Siahpush, M, Mattick, RP, Iredale, JM, Clare, PJ, Courtney, RJ, Martire, KA, Bonevski, B, Borland, R, Siahpush, M, and Mattick, RP
- Abstract
Introduction: Tobacco smoking co-occurs with behavioural risk factors including diet, alcohol use and obesity. However, the association between behavioural risk factors and heavy smoking (> 20 cig/day) compared to light-moderate smoking is unknown. The link between behavioural risk factors and future smoking for both ex and current smokers is also unknown. This study sought to examine these relationships. It is hypothesised that behavioural risk factors will be more strongly associated with heavy smoking. Method: Data from Wave 7 (2007) of the Household and Labour Dynamics in Australia (HILDA) survey was analysed using logistic regression to determine relationships between diet (fruit and vegetable consumption, and unhealthy diet choices), alcohol consumption, obesity and physical activity with light-moderate smoking and heavy smoking. The association between these risk factors and future smoking (2008) was assessed for current and ex-smokers (2007). Results: Obese respondents were less likely to be light/moderate smokers (RRR: 0.53; 95% CI: 0.43, 0.66) but not heavy smokers. Those who consume confectionary weekly were less likely to be light/moderate smokers (RRR: 0.73; 95% CI: 0.61, 0.87), but not heavy smokers. Smokers in 2007 were more likely to continue smoking in 2008 if they consumed 1-4 drinks per occasion (OR: 2.52; 95% CI: 1.13, 5.62). Ex-smokers in 2007 were less likely to relapse in 2008 if they consumed recommended levels of both fruit and vegetables (OR: 0.31; CI: 0.10, 0.91). Conclusion: The relationships between heavy smoking and behavioural risk factors differ from moderate-light smoking. Future primary care interventions would benefit from targeting multiple risk factors, particularly for heavy smokers.
- Published
- 2016
24. Judicial attitudes toward expert evidence
- Author
-
Martire, KA, Bali, A, Martire, KA, and Bali, A
- Published
- 2016
25. Forensic science in criminal courts: The latest scientific insights
- Author
-
Edmond, G, Martire, KA, Edmond, G, and Martire, KA
- Published
- 2016
26. Smoking Cessation among Low-Socioeconomic Status and Disadvantaged Population Groups: A Systematic Review of Research Output
- Author
-
courtney, R, naicker, S, Shakeshaft, A, clare, P, Martire, KA, mattick, R, courtney, R, naicker, S, Shakeshaft, A, clare, P, Martire, KA, and mattick, R
- Abstract
Background: Smoking cessation research output should move beyond descriptive research of the health problem to testing interventions that can provide causal data and effective evidence-based solutions. This review examined the number and type of published smoking cessation studies conducted in low-socioeconomic status (low-SES) and disadvantaged population groups. Methods: A systematic database search was conducted for two time periods: 2000–2004 (TP1) and 2008–2012 (TP2). Publications that examined smoking cessation in a low-SES or disadvantaged population were coded by: population of interest; study type (reviews, non-data based publications, data-based publications (descriptive, measurement and intervention research)); and country. Intervention studies were coded in accordance with the Cochrane Effective Practice and Organisation of Care data collection checklist and use of biochemical verification of self-reported abstinence was assessed. Results: 278 citations were included. Research output (i.e., all study types) had increased from TP1 27% to TP2 73% (χ² = 73.13, p < 0.001), however, the proportion of data-based research had not significantly increased from TP1 and TP2: descriptive (TP1 = 23% vs. TP2 = 33%) or intervention (TP1 = 77% vs. TP2 = 67%). The proportion of intervention studies adopting biochemical verification of self-reported abstinence had significantly decreased from TP1 to TP2 with an increased reliance on self-reported abstinence (TP1 = 12% vs. TP2 = 36%). Conclusions: The current research output is not ideal or optimal to decrease smoking rates. Research institutions, scholars and funding organisations should take heed to review findings when developing future research and policy.
- Published
- 2015
27. Perception problems of the verbal scale: A reanalysis and application of a membership function approach
- Author
-
Martire, KA, Watkins, I, Martire, KA, and Watkins, I
- Published
- 2015
28. Generalized linear mixed models for deception research: avoiding problematic data aggregation
- Author
-
Watkins, IJ, Martire, KA, Watkins, IJ, and Martire, KA
- Abstract
While the concept of sampling variation is well understood by most researchers in the field of deception detection, previous studies have failed to account for the multiple sources of sampling variation present in typical experimental designs and use participant-level data as the dependant measure in analyses. These aggregated data, however, contain inherent biases that can mislead researchers. We argue that to appropriately test hypotheses and make inferences beyond a particular sample of participants, the decision-level data must be modelled directly. To illustrate how this can be achieved we provide an introduction to generalized linear mixed models (GLMMs) for the analysis of deception data and present Monte Carlo simulations demonstrating both the seriousness of the inherent biases present in participant-level data and the benefits of the GLMM approach. These simulations suggest that the empirical Type 1 and Type 2 error rates associated with main effects testing in deception research may be as high as 35% when data are aggregated ‘by-judge’ and as high as 60% when data are aggregated ‘by-sender’, respectively. When decision-level data are modelled directly, however, these rates are likely to be close to nominal levels (6% and 28%, respectively). Implications for past and future research are discussed.
- Published
- 2015
29. How to cross-examine forensic scientists: A guide for lawyers
- Author
-
Edmond, G, Martire, KA, Kemp, RI, Hamer, D, Hibbert, B, Ligertwood, A, Porter, G, San Roque, M, Searston, R, Tangen, J, Thompson, M, White, D, Edmond, G, Martire, KA, Kemp, RI, Hamer, D, Hibbert, B, Ligertwood, A, Porter, G, San Roque, M, Searston, R, Tangen, J, Thompson, M, and White, D
- Abstract
This article is a resource for lawyers approaching the cross-examination offorensic scientists (and other expert witnesses). Through a series ofexamples, it provides information that will assist lawyers to explore theprobative value of forensic science evidence, in particular forensiccomparison evidence, on the voir dire and at trial. Questions covering abroad range of potential topics and issues, including relevance, theexpression of results, codes of conduct, limitations and errors, aresupplemented with detailed commentary and references to authoritativereports and research on the validity and reliability of forensic sciencetechniques.
- Published
- 2014
30. A randomized clinical trial of a financial education intervention with nicotine replacement therapy (NRT) for low socio-economic status Australian smokers: a study protocol
- Author
-
Courtney, RJ, Bradford, D, Martire, KA, Bonevski, B, Borland, R, Doran, C, Hall, W, Farrell, M, Siahpush, M, Sanson-Fisher, R, West, R, Mattick, RP, Courtney, RJ, Bradford, D, Martire, KA, Bonevski, B, Borland, R, Doran, C, Hall, W, Farrell, M, Siahpush, M, Sanson-Fisher, R, West, R, and Mattick, RP
- Abstract
BACKGROUND AND AIMS: Reducing smoking prevalence among smokers from low socio-economic status (SES) is a preventative health priority. Financial stress (e.g. shortage of money or inability to pay bills) may be a major barrier to quitting smoking. This study evaluates the efficacy of a financial education and support programme coupled with pharmacotherapy at improving cessation rates at 8-month follow-up among Australian low SES smokers (people receiving a government pension or allowance).
- Published
- 2014
31. The relationship between socioeconomic status and ‘hardcore’ smoking over time – greater accumulation of hardened smokers in low-SES than high-SES smokers
- Author
-
Clare, bradford, Courtney, RJ, Martire, KA, Mattick, RP, Clare, bradford, Courtney, RJ, Martire, KA, and Mattick, RP
- Abstract
Objectives This paper used national survey data to investigate ‘hardcore’ smoking as predicted by the ‘hardening hypothesis’, and to examine the relationship between ‘hardcore’ smoking and socioeconomic status (SES).Methods Analyses were performed using data from four waves of the Australian National Drug Strategy Household Survey between 2001 and 2010, a large national survey with a sample size of approximately 24 000 participants per wave. The primary outcome variable was ‘hardcore’ smoking, comprised of the variables: ‘no quit attempt in past 12 months’; ‘no plan to quit’; and smoking more than 15 cigarettes per day. The main predictor variables used were SES assessed by the Socio-Economic Indexes for Areas (SEIFA), and survey wave. Other sociodemographic variables were also examined.Results Overall, ‘hardcore’ smoking remained stable from 2001 to 2010. However, ‘hardcore’ smoking declined among high-SES smokers (from 1.8% to 1.0%), but not among low-SES smokers (around 3.4%). ‘Hardcore’ smoking was strongly associated with SEIFA quintile (p<0.001). There was a significant interaction effect between top and bottom SEIFA quintiles and wave (p=0.025), with a decline in ‘hardcore’ smoking measures over the four waves among those in the top two SEIFA quintiles, with odds in 2010 of 0.39 (95% CI 0.17 to 0.87; p=0.012), down from 0.64 (95% CI 0.50 to 0.82; p<0.001) in 2001, while ‘hardcore’ smoking remained stable among those in the bottom two SEIFA quintiles.Conclusions The results from high SES smokers suggest ‘hardcore’ smokers are able to quit, but outcomes among low-SES smokers are less encouraging.
- Published
- 2014
32. On the interpretation of likelihood ratios in forensic science evidence: Presentation formats and the Weak evidence effect.
- Author
-
Martire, KA, kemp, sayle, Newell, Martire, KA, kemp, sayle, and Newell
- Abstract
Likelihood ratios are increasingly being adopted to convey expert evaluative opinions to courts. In the absence of appropriate databases, many of these likelihood ratios will include verbal rather than numerical estimates of the support offered by the analysis. However evidence suggests that verbal formulations of uncertainty are a less effective form of communication than equivalent numerical formulations. Moreover, when evidence strength is low a misinterpretation of the valence of the evidence – a “weak evidence effect” – has been found. We report the results of an experiment involving N = 404 (student and online) participants who read a brief summary of a burglary trial containing expert testimony. The expert evidence was varied across conditions in terms of evidence strength (low or high) and presentation method (numerical, verbal, table or visual scale). Results suggest that of these presentation methods, numerical expressions produce belief-change and implicit likelihood ratios which were most commensurate with those intended by the expert and most resistant to the weak evidence effect. These findings raise questions about the extent to which low strength verbal evaluative opinions can be effectively communicated to decision makers at trial.
- Published
- 2014
33. Use of smoking cessation and quit support services by socioeconomic status over 10 years of the national drug strategy household survey
- Author
-
Clare, P, Slade, T, Courtney, RJ, Martire, KA, Mattick, RP, Clare, P, Slade, T, Courtney, RJ, Martire, KA, and Mattick, RP
- Abstract
OBJECTIVES: The aim of this research was to examine the use of quit smoking services and support options in Australia with national survey data to determine whether use of quit smoking services and pharmacotherapy contributes to socioeconomic status (SES) differences in smoking.
- Published
- 2014
34. Response to Recommendation 2 of the 2009 NAS Report—Standards for Formatting and Reporting Expert Evaluative Opinions: Where Do We Stand?
- Author
-
Howes, LM, Martire, KA, Kelty, SF, Howes, LM, Martire, KA, and Kelty, SF
- Abstract
Over four years ago, the 2009 US National Academy of Sciences report on forensic science was published, revealing that few formal standards existed in the forensic sciences. The second recommendation of the NAS report related to the language of reporting. This two-fold recommendation urged that templates for expert reports be developed and that the language and terminology of the reports and related expert testimony be standardized. This paper offers a response to Recommendation 2 of the NAS report and a research update. Since the release of the NAS report, Standards Australia has developed a set of forensic standards, including one for reporting. In light of Recommendation 2 of the NAS report and the Australian Standard for reporting, we outline current reporting practices of forensic science in the Australian context, and review research about the communication of forensic science, highlighting recent Australian research undertaken at two universities. We discuss the progress made to date in the development of best practice in expert reports and language use, introduce new directions for developing communicative excellence amongst forensic scientists, and suggest future research directions.
- Published
- 2014
35. The expression and interpretation of uncertain forensic science evidence: Verbal equivalence, evidence strength, and the weak evidence effect.
- Author
-
Martire, KA, Kemp, RI, Watkins, I, Sayle, MA, Newell, BR, Martire, KA, Kemp, RI, Watkins, I, Sayle, MA, and Newell, BR
- Abstract
Standards published by the Association of Forensic Science Providers (2009, Standards for the formulation of evaluative forensic science expert opinion, Science & Justice, Vol. 49, pp. 161–164) encourage forensic scientists to express their conclusions in the form of a likelihood ratio (LR), in which the value of the evidence is conveyed verbally or numerically. In this article, we report two experiments (using undergraduates and Mechanical Turk recruits) designed to investigate how much decision makers change their beliefs when presented with evidence in the form of verbal or numeric LRs. In Experiment 1 (N = 494), participants read a summary of a larceny trial containing inculpatory expert testimony in which evidence strength (low, moderate, high) and presentation method (verbal, numerical) varied. In Experiment 2 (N = 411), participants read the same larceny trial, this time including either exculpatory or inculpatory expert evidence that varied in strength (low, high) and presentation method (verbal, numerical). Both studies found a reasonable degree of correspondence in observed belief change resulting from verbal and numeric formats. However, belief change was considerably smaller than Bayesian calculations would predict. In addition, participants presented with evidence weakly supporting guilt tended to “invert” the evidence, thereby counterintuitively reducing their belief in the guilt of the accused. This “weak evidence effect” was most apparent in the verbal presentation conditions of both experiments, but only when the evidence was inculpatory. These findings raise questions about the interpretability of LRs by jurors and appear to support an expectancy-based account of the weak evidence effect
- Published
- 2013
36. The psychology of interpreting expert evaluative opinions
- Author
-
Martire, KA, Kemp, RI, Newell, BR, Martire, KA, Kemp, RI, and Newell, BR
- Abstract
The standards for expressions of evaluative opinions in the forensic sciences are increasingly being challenged and refined. Where once categorical statements regarding the origin of a trace were standard practice, criminalists are now being encouraged to represent the uncertainty associated with their inferential process by using numerical or verbal likelihood ratios. Although there are valid reasons to support this shift, the approach is not without limitations. Decades of psychological research investigating the interpretation and integration of probabilistic expressions, and the equivalence of verbal and numerical formulations for uncertainty, reveals a disconnect between what is intended by experts and what is understood by decision-makers. In this paper we present an indicative review of the psychological evidence to foster communication and collaboration between forensic scientists and psychologists and reduce instances of miscommunication in our criminal justice system. © 2013 Australian Academy of Forensic Sciences.
- Published
- 2013
37. Inadequate data collection prevents health planing for released prisoners
- Author
-
Martire, KA, Larney, S, Martire, KA, and Larney, S
- Published
- 2009
38. Individual differences in distributional statistical learning: Better frequency "discriminators" are better "estimators".
- Author
-
Growns B, Martire KA, and Mattijssen EJAT
- Abstract
People can easily extract and encode statistical information from their environment. However, research has primarily focused on conditional statistical learning (i.e., the ability to learn joint and conditional relationships between stimuli) and has largely neglected distributional statistical learning (i.e., the ability to learn the frequency and variability of distributions). For example, learning that "E" is more common in the English alphabet than "Z." In this article, we investigate how distributional learning can be measured by exploring the relationship between, and psychometric properties of, four different measures of distributional learning-from the ability to discriminate relative frequencies to the ability to estimate frequencies. We identified moderate relationships between four distributional learning measures and these tasks accounted for a substantial portion of the variance in performance across tasks (44.3%). A measure of divergent validity (intrinsic motivation) did not significantly correlate with any statistical learning measure and accounted for a separate portion of the variance across tasks. Our results suggest that distributional statistical learning encompasses the ability to discriminate between relative frequencies and estimating them., Competing Interests: Declaration of conflicting interestsThe author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
39. Jack of all trades, master of one: domain-specific and domain-general contributions to perceptual expertise in visual comparison.
- Author
-
Growns B, Dunn JD, Helm RK, Towler A, Mattijssen EJAT, and Martire KA
- Subjects
- Humans, Male, Female, Adult, Young Adult, Forensic Sciences, Dermatoglyphics, Firearms, Pattern Recognition, Visual physiology, Middle Aged, Visual Perception physiology, Facial Recognition physiology, Individuality, Professional Competence
- Abstract
Perceptual expertise is typically domain-specific and rarely generalises beyond an expert's domain of experience. Forensic feature-comparison examiners outperform the norm in domain-specific visual comparison, but emerging research suggests that they show advantages on other similar tasks outside their domain of expertise. For example, fingerprint examiners not only outperform novices in fingerprint comparison, but also in face comparison. Yet, the extent to which their skills generalise is poorly understood. In this study, we investigated the generalisability of perceptual expertise amongst forensic examiners by comparing their performance to novices and other examiners within and outside their area of expertise. We recruited 85 experts from three forensic disciplines (face, fingerprint, and firearms) and asked them to complete four different visual comparison tasks: faces, fingerprints, firearms, and novel-objects. Examiners displayed domain-specific expertise: they outperformed novices and other examiners within their domain of visual comparison expertise. Yet, some of their skill also generalised: examiners also outperformed novices outside their area of expertise. However, while individual differences in examiners' performance within their domain of experience were associated with their performance in a novel comparison task, they were not related to their performance on tasks outside their expert domain. These results provide key insight into the domain-specific and domain-general contributions of forensic examiners' perceptual expertise. Forensic expertise lends some generalisable skill to other visual comparison tasks, but best performance is still seen within examiners' domain of expertise., (© 2024. The Author(s).)
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
40. Speaker identification in courtroom contexts - Part III: Groups of collaborating listeners compared to forensic voice comparison based on automatic-speaker-recognition technology.
- Author
-
Bali AS, Basu N, Weber P, Rosas-Aguilar C, Edmond G, Martire KA, and Morrison GS
- Subjects
- Humans, Expert Testimony, Male, Female, Adult, Speech Recognition Software, Cooperative Behavior, Biometric Identification methods, Voice, Forensic Sciences methods
- Abstract
Expert testimony is only admissible in common-law systems if it will potentially assist the trier of fact. In order for a forensic-voice-comparison expert's testimony to assist a trier of fact, the expert's forensic voice comparison should be more accurate than the trier of fact's speaker identification. "Speaker identification in courtroom contexts - Part I" addressed the question of whether speaker identification by an individual lay listener (such as a judge) would be more or less accurate than the output of a forensic-voice-comparison system that is based on state-of-the-art automatic-speaker-recognition technology. The present paper addresses the question of whether speaker identification by a group of collaborating lay listeners (such as a jury) would be more or less accurate than the output of such a forensic-voice-comparison system. As members of collaborating groups, participants listen to pairs of recordings reflecting the conditions of the questioned- and known-speaker recordings in an actual case, confer, and make a probabilistic consensus judgement on each pair of recordings. The present paper also compares group-consensus responses with "wisdom of the crowd" which uses the average of the responses from multiple independent individual listeners., Competing Interests: Declaration of Competing Interest The authors declare the following financial interests/personal relationships which may be considered as potential competing interests: Dr Morrison is Director and Forensic Consultant for Forensic Evaluation Ltd. Dr Weber has worked as a contractor for Forensic Evaluation Ltd. Forensic Evaluation Ltd charges clients fees to perform forensic-voice-comparison evaluations, and to submit reports and testify in court regarding forensic voice comparison, and regarding speaker recognition and speaker identification by laypersons., (Copyright © 2024 The Authors. Published by Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.)
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
41. The effect of following best practice reporting recommendations on legal and community evaluations of forensic examiners reports.
- Author
-
Summersby S, Edmond G, Kemp RI, Ballantyne KN, and Martire KA
- Subjects
- Humans, Male, Guideline Adherence, Female, Disclosure legislation & jurisprudence, Adult, Decision Making, Practice Guidelines as Topic, Dermatoglyphics, Reproducibility of Results, Middle Aged, Forensic Sciences legislation & jurisprudence
- Abstract
Commentators have recommended that forensic scientists' reports contain various disclosures to facilitate comprehension. However, little research has explored whether following best practice recommendations for disclosure impacts on receivers' impressions of the evidence. We examined whether forensic science reports that are more compliant with these best practice recommendations reduced overvaluing of the evidence and sensitized legal and community decision-makers to evidence quality. Across three experiments, 240 legal practitioners/trainees and 566 community decision-makers were presented with a fingerprint or footwear report that was either compliant or non-compliant with best practice recommendations. Participants were then asked to make evaluations and decisions based on the report. We found mixed effects of report compliance. Report compliance affected community participant's evaluations of the persuasiveness of the evidence but had limited impact on the judgments of legal practitioners/trainees. When presented with compliant reports, we found that community participants regarded unknown reliability evidence as less reliable and less persuasive than high reliability evidence, suggesting disclosures helped reduce overvaluing of the evidence and create sensitivity to differences in evidence quality. These results suggest compliance with reporting recommendations does affect community impressions, while only minimally influencing legal impressions of forensic science evidence. The costs and/or benefits of this outcome require further examination., Competing Interests: Declaration of Competing Interest Summersby, S and Ballantyne, K.N. are employees of Victoria Police Forensic Services Department. Edmond G, Kemp, R .I, and Martire, K.A declare no conflicts of interest., (Copyright © 2024 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.)
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
42. Understanding 'error' in the forensic sciences: A primer.
- Author
-
Martire KA, Chin JM, Davis C, Edmond G, Growns B, Gorski S, Kemp RI, Lee Z, Verdon CM, Jansen G, Lang T, Neal TMS, Searston RA, Slocum J, Summersby S, Tangen JM, Thompson MB, Towler A, Watson D, Werrett MV, Younan M, and Ballantyne KN
- Abstract
This paper distils seven key lessons about 'error' from a collaborative webinar series between practitioners at Victoria Police Forensic Services Department and academics. It aims to provide the common understanding of error necessary to foster interdisciplinary dialogue, collaboration and research. The lessons underscore the inevitability, complexity and subjectivity of error, as well as opportunities for learning and growth. Ultimately, we argue that error can be a potent tool for continuous improvement and accountability, enhancing the reliability of forensic sciences and public trust. It is hoped the shared understanding provided by this paper will support future initiatives and funding for collaborative developments in this vital domain., Competing Interests: The authors declare the following financial interests/personal relationships which may be considered as potential competing interests: Kristy A. Martire reports financial support was provided by Victoria Police Forensic Services Department. Richard I. Kemp, Rachel A. Searston, Alice Towler, Jason M. Tangen, Gary Edmond, Matthew B. Thompson and Kristy Martire report financial support was provided by 10.13039/501100000923Australian Research Council. Tess M. S. Neal reports financial support was provided by PLuS Alliance Fellowship and Australian-American Fulbright Commission. Carolyn Davis, Stacey Gorski, Zara Lee, Christopher M. Verdon, Gabrielle Jansen, Tanya Lang, Joshua Slocum, Stephanie Summersby, Darren Watson, Melissa V. Werrett and Kaye N. Ballantyne report a relationship with Victoria Police Forensic Services Department that includes: employment. Jason M. Chin is an Editor at Forensic Science International: Synergy. If there are other authors, they declare that they have no known competing financial interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence the work reported in this paper., (© 2024 The Authors.)
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
43. Thinking false and slow: Implausible beliefs and the Cognitive Reflection Test.
- Author
-
Martire KA, Robson SG, Drew M, Nicholls K, and Faasse K
- Subjects
- Humans, Thinking physiology, Personality, Cognitive Reflection, Intuition physiology
- Abstract
Why do people believe implausible claims like conspiracy theories, pseudoscience, and fake news? Past studies using the Cognitive Reflection Test (CRT) suggest that implausible beliefs may result from an unwillingness to effortfully process information (i.e., cognitive miserliness). Our analysis (N = 664) tests this account by comparing CRT performance (total score, number and proportion of incorrect intuitive responses, and completion time) for endorsers and non-endorsers of implausible claims. Our results show that endorsers performed worse than non-endorsers on the CRT, but they took significantly longer to answer the questions and did not make proportionally more intuitive mistakes. Endorsers therefore appear to process information effortfully but nonetheless score lower on the CRT. Poorer overall CRT performance may not necessarily indicate that those who endorse implausible beliefs have a more reflexive, intuitive, or non-analytical cognitive style than non-endorsers., (© 2023. The Author(s).)
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
44. Speaker identification in courtroom contexts - Part II: Investigation of bias in individual listeners' responses.
- Author
-
Basu N, Weber P, Bali AS, Rosas-Aguilar C, Edmond G, Martire KA, and Morrison GS
- Abstract
In "Speaker identification in courtroom contexts - Part I" individual listeners made speaker-identification judgements on pairs of recordings which reflected the conditions of the questioned-speaker and known-speaker recordings in a real case. The recording conditions were poor, and there was a mismatch between the questioned-speaker condition and the known-speaker condition. No contextual information that could potentially bias listeners' responses was included in the experiment condition - it was decontextualized with respect to case circumstances and with respect to other evidence that could be presented in the context of a case. Listeners' responses exhibited a bias in favour of the different-speaker hypothesis. It was hypothesized that the bias was due to the poor and mismatched recording conditions. The present research compares speaker-identification performance between: (1) listeners under the original Part I experiment condition, (2) listeners who were informed ahead of time that the recording conditions would make the recordings sound more different from one another than had they both been high-quality recordings, and (3) listeners who were presented with high-quality versions of the recordings. Under all experiment conditions, there was a substantial bias in favour of the different-speaker hypothesis. The bias in favour of the different-speaker hypothesis therefore appears not to be due to the poor and mismatched recording conditions., Competing Interests: Declaration of Competing Interest The authors declare the following financial interests/personal relationships which may be considered as potential competing interests: Dr Morrison is Director and Forensic Consultant for Forensic Evaluation Ltd. Dr Weber has worked as a contractor for Forensic Evaluation Ltd. Forensic Evaluation Ltd charges clients fees to perform forensic-voice-comparison evaluations, and to submit reports and testify in court regarding forensic voice comparison, and regarding speaker recognition and speaker identification by laypersons., (Copyright © 2023 The Author(s). Published by Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.)
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
45. Finding the perfect match: Fingerprint expertise facilitates statistical learning and visual comparison decision-making.
- Author
-
Growns B, Mattijssen EJAT, Salerno JM, Schweitzer NJ, Cole SA, and Martire KA
- Subjects
- Humans, Learning, Forensic Sciences, Dermatoglyphics, Judgment
- Abstract
Forensic feature-comparison examiners compare-or "match"-evidence samples (e.g., fingerprints) to provide judgments about the source of the evidence. Research demonstrates that examiners in select disciplines possess expertise in this task by outperforming novices-yet the psychological mechanisms underpinning this expertise are unclear. This article investigates one implicated mechanism: statistical learning, the ability to learn how often things occur in the environment. This ability is likely important in forensic decision-making as samples sharing rarer statistical information are more likely to come from the same source than those sharing more common information. We investigated 46 fingerprint examiners' and 52 novices' statistical learning of fingerprint categories and application of this knowledge in a source-likelihood judgment task. Participants completed four measures of their statistical learning (frequency discrimination judgments, bounded and unbounded frequency estimates, and source-likelihood judgments) before and after familiarization to the "ground-truth" category frequencies. Compared to novices, fingerprint examiners had superior domain-specific statistical learning across all measures-both before and after familiarization. This suggests that fingerprint expertise facilitates domain-specific statistical learning-something that has important theoretical and applied implications for the development of training programs and statistical databases in forensic science. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2023 APA, all rights reserved).
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
46. Speaker identification in courtroom contexts - Part I: Individual listeners compared to forensic voice comparison based on automatic-speaker-recognition technology.
- Author
-
Basu N, Bali AS, Weber P, Rosas-Aguilar C, Edmond G, Martire KA, and Morrison GS
- Subjects
- Recognition, Psychology, Forensic Medicine, Expert Testimony, Technology, Voice
- Abstract
Expert testimony is only admissible in common law if it will potentially assist the trier of fact to make a decision that they would not be able to make unaided. The present paper addresses the question of whether speaker identification by an individual lay listener (such as a judge) would be more or less accurate than the output of a forensic-voice-comparison system that is based on state-of-the-art automatic-speaker-recognition technology. Listeners listen to and make probabilistic judgements on pairs of recordings reflecting the conditions of the questioned- and known-speaker recordings in an actual case. Reflecting different courtroom contexts, listeners with different language backgrounds are tested: Some are familiar with the language and accent spoken, some are familiar with the language but less familiar with the accent, and others are less familiar with the language. Also reflecting different courtroom contexts: In one condition listeners make judgements based only on listening, and in another condition listeners make judgements based on both listening to the recordings and considering the likelihood-ratio values output by the forensic-voice-comparison system., Competing Interests: Declaration of Competing Interest The authors declare the following financial interests/personal relationships which may be considered as potential competing interests: Dr Morrison is Director and Forensic Consultant for Forensic Evaluation Ltd. Dr Weber has worked as a contractor for Forensic Evaluation Ltd. Forensic Evaluation Ltd charges clients fees to perform forensic-voice-comparison evaluations, and to submit reports and testify in court regarding forensic voice comparison, and regarding speaker recognition and speaker identification by laypersons., (Copyright © 2022 The Authors. Published by Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.)
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
47. Likeability and Expert Persuasion: Dislikeability Reduces the Perceived Persuasiveness of Expert Evidence.
- Author
-
Younan M and Martire KA
- Abstract
With the use of expert evidence increasing in civil and criminal trials, there is concern jurors' decisions are affected by factors that are irrelevant to the quality of the expert opinion. Past research suggests that the likeability of an expert significantly affects juror attributions of credibility and merit. However, we know little about the effects of expert likeability when detailed information about expertise is provided. Two studies examined the effect of an expert's likeability on the persuasiveness judgments and sentencing decisions of 456 jury-eligible respondents. Participants viewed and/or read an expert's testimony (lower vs. higher quality) before rating expert persuasiveness (via credibility, value, and weight), and making a sentencing decision in a Capitol murder case (death penalty vs. life in prison). Lower quality evidence was significantly less persuasive than higher quality evidence. Less likeable experts were also significantly less persuasive than either neutral or more likeable experts. This "penalty" for less likeable experts was observed irrespective of evidence quality. However, only perceptions of the foundational validity of the expert's discipline, the expert's trustworthiness and the clarity and conservativeness of the expert opinion significantly predicted sentencing decisions. Thus, the present study demonstrates that while likeability does influence persuasiveness, it does not necessarily affect sentencing outcomes., Competing Interests: The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest., (Copyright © 2021 Younan and Martire.)
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
48. Public attitudes towards the use of automatic facial recognition technology in criminal justice systems around the world.
- Author
-
Ritchie KL, Cartledge C, Growns B, Yan A, Wang Y, Guo K, Kramer RSS, Edmond G, Martire KA, San Roque M, and White D
- Subjects
- Adolescent, Adult, Aged, Aged, 80 and over, Female, Humans, Male, Middle Aged, Surveys and Questionnaires, Trust, Young Adult, Attitude, Automated Facial Recognition, Criminal Law, Public Opinion
- Abstract
Automatic facial recognition technology (AFR) is increasingly used in criminal justice systems around the world, yet to date there has not been an international survey of public attitudes toward its use. In Study 1, we ran focus groups in the UK, Australia and China (countries at different stages of adopting AFR) and in Study 2 we collected data from over 3,000 participants in the UK, Australia and the USA using a questionnaire investigating attitudes towards AFR use in criminal justice systems. Our results showed that although overall participants were aligned in their attitudes and reasoning behind them, there were some key differences across countries. People in the USA were more accepting of tracking citizens, more accepting of private companies' use of AFR, and less trusting of the police using AFR than people in the UK and Australia. Our results showed that support for the use of AFR depends greatly on what the technology is used for and who it is used by. We recommend vendors and users do more to explain AFR use, including details around accuracy and data protection. We also recommend that governments should set legal boundaries around the use of AFR in investigative and criminal justice settings., Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
49. Corrigendum to "Communicating forensic science opinion: An examination of expert reporting practices" [Sci. Justice 60 (3) (2020) 216-224].
- Author
-
Bali AS, Edmond G, Ballantyne KN, Kemp RI, and Martire KA
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
50. Limited not lazy: a quasi-experimental secondary analysis of evidence quality evaluations by those who hold implausible beliefs.
- Author
-
Martire KA, Growns B, Bali AS, Montgomery-Farrer B, Summersby S, and Younan M
- Subjects
- Humans, Persuasive Communication
- Abstract
Past research suggests that an uncritical or 'lazy' style of evaluating evidence may play a role in the development and maintenance of implausible beliefs. We examine this possibility by using a quasi-experimental design to compare how low- and high-quality evidence is evaluated by those who do and do not endorse implausible claims. Seven studies conducted during 2019-2020 provided the data for this analysis (N = 746). Each of the seven primary studies presented participants with high- and/or low-quality evidence and measured implausible claim endorsement and evaluations of evidence persuasiveness (via credibility, value, and/or weight). A linear mixed-effect model was used to predict persuasiveness from the interaction between implausible claim endorsement and evidence quality. Our results showed that endorsers were significantly more persuaded by the evidence than non-endorsers, but both groups were significantly more persuaded by high-quality than low-quality evidence. The interaction between endorsement and evidence quality was not significant. These results suggest that the formation and maintenance of implausible beliefs by endorsers may result from less critical evidence evaluations rather than a failure to analyse. This is consistent with a limited rather than a lazy approach and suggests that interventions to develop analytical skill may be useful for minimising the effects of implausible claims.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.