138 results on '"Leimar O"'
Search Results
2. Reciprocity and Communication of Partner Quality
- Author
-
Leimar, O.
- Published
- 1997
3. Variation in two phases of post-winter development of a butterfly
- Author
-
Stålhandske, S., Gotthard, K., Posledovich, D., and Leimar, O.
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
4. Heterospecific courtship, minority effects and niche separation between cryptic butterfly species
- Author
-
Friberg, M., Leimar, O., and Wiklund, C.
- Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
5. Adaptation to fluctuating environments in a selection experiment with Drosophila melanogaster
- Author
-
Kubrak, O.I., Nylin, S., Flatt, T., Nässel, D.R., and Leimar, O.
- Subjects
experimental evolution ,food intake ,generalist phenotype ,reaction norm ,resource storage ,starvation resistance - Abstract
A fundamental question in life‐history evolution is how organisms cope with fluctuating environments, including variation between stressful and benign conditions. For short‐ lived organisms, environments commonly vary between generations. Using a novel experimental design, we exposed wild‐derived Drosophila melanogaster to three different selection regimes: one where generations alternated between starvation and benign conditions, and starvation was always preceded by early exposure to cold; another where starvation and benign conditions alternated in the same way, but cold shock sometimes preceded starvation and sometimes benign conditions; and a third where conditions were always benign. Using six replicate populations per selection regime, we found that selected flies increased their starvation resistance, most strongly for the regime where cold and starvation were reliably combined, and this occurred without decreased fecundity or extended developmental time. The selected flies became stress resistant, displayed a pronounced increase in early life food intake and resource storage. In contrast to previous experiments selecting for increased starvation resistance in D. melanogaster, we did not find increased storage of lipids as the main response, but instead that, in particular for females, storage of carbohydrates was more pronounced. We argue that faster mobilization of carbohydrates is advantageous in fluctuating environments and conclude that the phenotype that evolved in our experiment corresponds to a compromise between the requirements of stressful and benign environments.
- Published
- 2019
6. Facing the facts
- Author
-
LEIMAR, O. and HAMMERSTEIN, P.
- Published
- 2006
7. Cooperating for direct fitness benefits
- Author
-
HAMMERSTEIN, P. and LEIMAR, O.
- Published
- 2006
8. Competition, Coexistence and Irreversibility in Models of Early Molecular Evolution
- Author
-
Blomberg, C., von Heijne, G., Leimar, O., and Wolman, Yecheskel, editor
- Published
- 1981
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
9. Borrowed plant defences: Deterring browsers using a forestry by-product
- Author
-
Stutz, R.S., primary, Croak, B.M., additional, Leimar, O., additional, and Bergvall, U.A., additional
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
10. Limiting similarity, species packing, and the shape of competition kernels
- Author
-
Leimar, O., Sasaki, A., Doebeli, M., and Dieckmann, U.
- Abstract
A traditional question in community ecology is whether species' traits are distributed as more-or-less regularly spaced clusters. Interspecific competition has been suggested to play a role in such structuring of communities. The seminal theoretical work on limiting similarity and species packing, presented four decades ago by Robert MacArthur, Richard Levins and Robert May, has recently been extended. There is now a deeper understanding of how competitive interactions influence community structure, for instance, how the shape of competition kernels can determine the clustering of species' traits. Competition is typically weaker for greater phenotypic difference, and the shape of the dependence defines a competition kernel. The clustering tendencies of kernels interact with other effects, such as variation in resource availability along a niche axis, but the kernel shape can have a decisive influence on community structure. Here we review and further extend the recent developments and evaluate their importance.
- Published
- 2013
11. A Simple Fitness Proxy for Structured Populations with Continuous Traits, with Case Studies on the Evolution of Haplodiploids and Genetic Dimorphisms
- Author
-
Metz, J.A.J. and Leimar, O.
- Abstract
For structured populations in equilibrium with everybody born equal ln(R_0) is a useful fitness proxy for ESS and most adaptive dynamics calculations, with R_0 the average lifetime number of offspring in the clonal and haploid cases, and half the average lifetime number of offspring fathered or mothered for Mendelian diploids. When individuals have variable birth states, as is e.g. the case in spatial models, R_0 is itself an eigenvalue, which usually cannot be expressed explicitly in the trait vectors under consideration. In that case Q(Y|X) := -det(I-L(Y|X)) can often be used as fitness proxy, with L the next-generation matrix for a potential mutant characterised by the trait vector Y in the (constant) environment engendered by a resident characterised by X. If the trait space is connected, global univadability can be determined from it. Moreover it can be used in all the usual local calculations like the determination of evolutionarily singular trait vectors and their local invadability and attractivity. We conclude with three extended case studies demonstrating the usefulness of Q: the calculation of ESSes under haplo-diploid genetics (I), of Evolutionarily Steady genetic Dimorphisms with a priori proportionality of macro- and micro-gametic outputs (an assumption that is generally made but the fulfillment of which is a priori highly exceptional) (II), and of ESDs without such proportionality (III). These case studies should also have some interest in their own right for the spelled out calculation recipes and their underlying modelling methodology.
- Published
- 2011
12. Do animal personalities emerge? Reply
- Author
-
Wolf, M., Van Doorn, G.S., Leimar, O., Weissing, F.J., Weissing group, and Van Doorn group
- Subjects
ADAPTIVE DYNAMICS ,EVOLUTION - Published
- 2008
13. Multimodal Pattern Formation in Phenotype Distributions of Sexual Populations
- Author
-
Doebeli, M., Blok, H.J., Leimar, O., and Dieckmann, U.
- Abstract
During bouts of evolutionary diversification, such as adaptive radiations, the emerging species cluster around different locations in phenotype space, How such multimodal patterns in phenotype space can emerge from a single ancestral species is a fundamental question in biology. Frequency-dependent competition is one potential mechanism for such pattern formation, as has previously been shown in models based on the theory of adaptive dynamics. Here we demonstrate that also in models similar to those used in quantitative genetics, phenotype distributions can split into multiple modes under the force of frequency-dependent competition. In sexual populations, this requires assortative mating, and we show that the multimodal splitting of initially unimodal distributions occurs over a range of assortment parameters. In addition, assortative mating can be favoured evolutionarily even if it incurs costs, because it provides a means of alleviating the effects of frequency dependence. Our results reveal that models at both ends of the spectrum between essentially monomorphic (adaptive dynamics) and fully polymorphic (quantitative genetics) yield similar results. This underscores that frequency-dependent selection is a strong agent of pattern formation in phenotype distributions, potentially resulting in adaptive speciation.
- Published
- 2006
14. Group report : interspecific mutualism
- Author
-
Bergstrom, C. T., Bronstein, J. L., Bshary, R., Connor, R. C., Daly, M., Frank, S. A., Gintis, H., Keller, L., Leimar, O., Noë, R., Queller, C. C., and Hammerstein, P. (ed.)
- Published
- 2003
15. Group Report: Interspecific Mutualism - Puzzles and Predictions
- Author
-
Bergstrom, C. T., Bronstein, J. L., Bshary, R., Connor, R. C., Daly, M., Frank, S. A., Gintis, H., Keller, L., Leimar, O., Nöe, R., Queller, D. C., and Hammerstein, P. (ed.)
- Published
- 2003
16. Heterospecific courtship, minority effects and niche separation between cryptic butterfly species
- Author
-
Friberg, Magne, Leimar, O., Wiklund, C., Friberg, Magne, Leimar, O., and Wiklund, C.
- Abstract
Species interacting in varied ecological conditions often evolve in different directions in different local populations. The butterflies of the cryptic Leptidea complex are sympatrically distributed in different combinations across their Eurasian range. Interestingly, the same species is a habitat generalist in some regions and a habitat specialist in others, where a sibling species has the habitat generalist role. Previous studies suggest that this geographically variable niche divergence is generated by local processes in different contact zones. By varying the absolute and relative densities of Leptidea sinapis and Leptidea juvernica in large outdoor cages, we show that female mating success is unaffected by conspecific density, but strongly negatively affected by the density of the other species. Whereas 80% of the females mated when a conspecific couple was alone in a cage, less than 10% mated when the single couple shared the cage with five pairs of the other species. The heterospecific courtships can thus affect the population fitness, and for the species in the local minority, the suitability of a habitat is likely to depend on the presence or absence of the locally interacting species. If the local relative abundance of the different species depends on the colonization order, priority effects might determine the ecological roles of interacting species in this system.
- Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
17. A simple fitness proxy for structured populations with continuous traits, with case studies on the evolution of haplo-diploids and genetic dimorphisms
- Author
-
Metz, J.A.J., Leimar, O., Metz, J.A.J., and Leimar, O.
- Abstract
For structured populations in equilibrium with everybody born equal, ln(R0) is a useful fitness proxy for evolutionarily steady strategy (ESS) and most adaptive dynamics calculations, with R0 the average lifetime number of offspring in the clonal and haploid cases, and half the average lifetime number of offspring fathered or mothered for Mendelian diploids. When individuals have variable birth states, as is, for example, the case in spatial models, R0 is itself an eigenvalue, which usually cannot be expressed explicitly in the trait vectors under consideration. In that case, Q(Y| X):=-det (I-L(Y| X)) can often be used as fitness proxy, with L the next-generation matrix for a potential mutant characterized by the trait vector Y in the (constant) environment engendered by a resident characterized by X. If the trait space is connected, global uninvadability can be determined from it. Moreover, it can be used in all the usual local calculations like the determination of evolutionarily singular trait vectors and their local invadability and attractivity. We conclude with three extended case studies demonstrating the usefulness of Q: the calculation of ESSs under haplo-diploid genetics (I), of evolutionarily steady genetic dimorphisms (ESDs) with a priori proportionality of macro- and micro-gaetic outputs (an assumption that is generally made but the fulfillment of which is a priori highly exceptional) (II), and of ESDs without such proportionality (III). These case studies should also have some interest in their own right for the spelled out calculation recipes and their underlying modelling methodology.
- Published
- 2011
18. Evolution of phenotypic clusters through competition and local adaptation along an environmental gradient
- Author
-
Leimar, O., Doebeli, M., Dieckmann, U., Leimar, O., Doebeli, M., and Dieckmann, U.
- Abstract
We have analyzed the evolution of a quantitative trait in populations that are spatially extended along an environmental gradient, with gene flow between nearby locations. In the absence of competition, there is stabilizing selection toward a locally best-adapted trait that changes gradually along the gradient. According to traditional ideas, gradual spatial variation in environmental conditions is expected to lead to gradual variation in the evolved trait. A contrasting possibility is that the trait distribution instead breaks up into discrete clusters. Doebeli and Dieckmann (2003) argued that competition acting locally in trait space and geographical space can promote such clustering. We have investigated this possibility using deterministic population dynamics for asexual populations, analyzing our model numerically and through an analytical approximation. We examined how the evolution of clusters is affected by the shape of competition kernels, by the presence of Allee effects, and by the strength of gene flow along the gradient. For certain parameter ranges clustering was a robust outcome, and for other ranges there was no clustering. Our analysis shows that the shape of competition kernels is important for clustering: the sign structure of the Fourier transform of a competition kernel determines whether the kernel promotes clustering. Also, we found that Allee effects promote clustering, whereas gene flow can have a counteracting influence. In line with earlier findings, we could demonstrate that phenotypic clustering was favored by gradients of intermediate slope.
- Published
- 2008
19. Multimodal pattern formation in phenotype distributions of sexual populations
- Author
-
Doebeli, M., Blok, H.J., Leimar, O., Dieckmann, U., Doebeli, M., Blok, H.J., Leimar, O., and Dieckmann, U.
- Abstract
During bouts of evolutionary diversification, such as adaptive radiations, the emerging species cluster around different locations in phenotype space. How such multimodal patterns in phenotype space can emerge from a single ancestral species is a fundamental question in biology. Frequency-dependent competition is one potential mechanism for such pattern formation, as has previously been shown in models based on the theory of adaptive dynamics. Here, we demonstrate that also in models similar to those used in quantitative genetics, phenotype distributions can split into multiple modes under the force of frequency-dependent competition. In sexual populations, this requires assortative mating, and we show that the multimodal splitting of initially unimodal distributions occurs over a range of assortment parameters. In addition, assortative mating can be favoured evolutionarily even if it incurs costs, because it provides a means of alleviating the effects of frequency dependence. Our results reveal that models at both ends of the spectrum between essentially monomorphic (adaptive dynamics) and fully polymorphic (quantitative genetics) yield similar results. This underscores that frequency-dependent selection is a strong agent of pattern formation in phenotype distributions, potentially resulting in adaptive speciation.
- Published
- 2007
20. A simple fitness proxy for structured populations with continuous traits, with case studies on the evolution of haplo-diploids and genetic dimorphisms
- Author
-
Metz, J. A.J., primary and Leimar, O., additional
- Published
- 2011
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
21. Group Report: Interspecific Mutualism - Puzzles and Predictions
- Author
-
Hammerstein, P. (ed.), Bergstrom, C. T., Bronstein, J. L., Bshary, R., Connor, R. C., Daly, M., Frank, S. A., Gintis, H., Keller, L., Leimar, O., Nöe, R., Queller, D. C., Hammerstein, P. (ed.), Bergstrom, C. T., Bronstein, J. L., Bshary, R., Connor, R. C., Daly, M., Frank, S. A., Gintis, H., Keller, L., Leimar, O., Nöe, R., and Queller, D. C.
- Published
- 2003
22. Group report : interspecific mutualism
- Author
-
Hammerstein, P. (ed.), Bergstrom, C. T., Bronstein, J. L., Bshary, R., Connor, R. C., Daly, M., Frank, S. A., Gintis, H., Keller, L., Leimar, O., Noë, R., Queller, C. C., Hammerstein, P. (ed.), Bergstrom, C. T., Bronstein, J. L., Bshary, R., Connor, R. C., Daly, M., Frank, S. A., Gintis, H., Keller, L., Leimar, O., Noë, R., and Queller, C. C.
- Published
- 2003
23. Sex-biased dispersal in sperm whales: contrasting mitochondrial andnuclear genetic structure of global populations.
- Author
-
Lyrholm, T, Leimar, O, Johanneson, B, Gyllensten, U, Lyrholm, T, Leimar, O, Johanneson, B, and Gyllensten, U
- Published
- 1999
24. Evolutionary Change and Darwinian Demons
- Author
-
Leimar, O., primary
- Published
- 2002
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
25. Low diversity and biased substitution patterns in the mitochondrial DNA control region of sperm whales: implications for estimates of time since common ancestry
- Author
-
Lyrholm, T., primary, Leimar, O., additional, and Gyllensten, U., additional
- Published
- 1996
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
26. Habitat exploration in butterflies--an outdoor cage experiment.
- Author
-
Norberg, U., Enfjall, K., and Leimar, O.
- Subjects
HABITATS ,BUTTERFLIES ,ECOLOGY - Abstract
Cites the key findings of an outdoor cage experiment on habitat exploration by butterflies. Key issues of interest; Analysis of pertinent topics and relevant issues; Implications on evolutionary ecology.
- Published
- 2002
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
27. Metapopulation extinction and genetic variation in dispersal-relatedtraits
- Author
-
Leimar, O. and Norberg, U.
- Subjects
- *
BIOLOGICAL extinction , *BIOLOGICAL variation , *GENETICS , *ECOLOGY , *CONSERVATION biology , *NATURAL selection - Abstract
When the suitable habitat for a population is fragmented into relatively isolated patches, an individual's probability of success in interpatch migration may become low. If, in addition, the local demes inhabiting the patches experience random extinctions, the persistence ofthe entire metapopulation can be threatened. A reduction in migration success results in natural selection, and given genetic variation in dispersal-related traits, fragmentation may be followed by a changein dispersal behaviour and capacity, which in turn can influence thepersistence of the metapopulation. Using computer simulation, we investigate the consequences of genetic variation in dispersal-related traits for the probability of extinction following fragmentation. We find that within-deme selection against dispersal can set off a critical phase, during which the patch occupancy is low and the risk of extinction high. In this manner, genetic variation can pose a threat to persistence. However, if a metapopulation recovers from the critical phase, through recolonization of empty patches by efficient dispersers, the ensuing persistence tends to be higher than would have been the case without genetic variation. The severity of a critical phase depends on factors like the rapidity of the fragmentation process and the magnitude of the drop in survival. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 1997
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
28. Life history plasticity: influence of photoperiod on growth and development in the common blue butterfly
- Author
-
Leimar, O.
- Subjects
- *
INSECTS - Published
- 1996
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
29. The accuracy of Kramers' theory of chemical kinetics
- Author
-
Edholm, O., primary and Leimar, O., additional
- Published
- 1979
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
30. Synergistic selection and graded traits
- Author
-
Leimar, O. and Tuomi, J.
- Subjects
EVOLUTIONARY theories - Abstract
Fitness interactions where benefits are shared only between individuals with similar traits are often referred to as 'synergistic'. Examples include defence characters, like insect warning colouration and plant unpalatability, and joint activities needing the active participation of all group members. Previous analyses, assuming discrete variation in the trait, have shown that synergistic selection can be a sufficient explanation for the evolutionary stability of such traits. Here, we investigate the consequences of graded variation in the traitresponsible for synergistic effects. Classifying the synergism as unbiased when an individual receives maximum associational benefit by having the same trait value as its neighbours, and letting a positive (negative) bias represent the maximum above (below) this value, we show that only positively biased synergistic selection can enhance a graded trait. Thus for graded traits, a synergistic benefit is not in itself sufficient for evolutionary stability. We study possible reasons for synergistic bias in a simple model of plant defences against herbivores, and suggest that the processes of herbivore avoidance learning and diet selection are probable causes of positive bias. We propose that mammalian herbivores exposed to a given level of toxicity will show stronger feeding aversion to higher toxicity, resulting in positively biased synergistic selection of plant defence traits. Positive bias produced by avoidance learning may, in a similar way, also select for defence signals. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 1998
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
31. The evolutionary consequences of learning under competition.
- Author
-
McNamara JM, Dall SRX, Houston AI, and Leimar O
- Subjects
- Animals, Reinforcement, Psychology, Biological Evolution, Learning, Game Theory, Competitive Behavior
- Abstract
Learning is a taxonomically widespread process by which animals change their behavioural responses to stimuli as a result of experience. In this way, it plays a crucial role in the development of individual behaviour and underpins substantial phenotypic variation within populations. Nevertheless, the impact of learning in social contexts on evolutionary change is not well understood. Here, we develop game theoretical models of competition for resources in small groups (e.g. producer-scrounger and hawk-dove games) in which actions are controlled by reinforcement learning and show that biases in the subjective valuation of different actions readily evolve. Moreover, in many cases, the convergence stable levels of bias exist at fitness minima and therefore lead to disruptive selection on learning rules and, potentially, to the evolution of genetic polymorphisms. Thus, we show how reinforcement learning in social contexts can be a driver of evolutionary diversification. In addition, we consider the evolution of ability in our games, showing that learning can also drive disruptive selection on the ability to perform a task.
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
32. Social bond dynamics and the evolution of helping.
- Author
-
Leimar O and Bshary R
- Subjects
- Animals, Biological Evolution, Food, Game Theory, Residence Characteristics, Chiroptera, Cooperative Behavior
- Abstract
Empiricists often struggle to apply game theory models to real-life cases of animal cooperation. One reason is that many examples of cooperation occur in stable groups, where individuals form social bonds that influence exchanges of help in ways that are not well described by previous models, including the extent of reciprocity and how relationships are initiated. We present a game theory model exploring the conditions under which social bonds between group members promote cooperation. In the model, bonds build up from exchanges of help in a similar way as the strength of association increases in learning, as in the Rescorla-Wagner rule. The bonds in turn affect partner choice and influence helping amounts. The model has a mechanism of reciprocity for bonded pairs, which can evolve toward either loose or strict reciprocation. Several aspects of the model are inspired by observations of food sharing in vampire bats. We find that small social neighborhoods are required for the evolutionary stability of helping, either as small group sizes, or if bonded members of larger groups can form temporary (daily) smaller groupings. The costs of helping need to be fairly low, while the benefits can be substantial. The form of reciprocity that evolves is neither immediate nor very strict. Individuals in need request help based on bond strength, but there is also an evolved preference for initiating bonds with new group members. In contrast, if different groups come into temporary contact, the evolved tendency is to avoid forming bonds between groups., Competing Interests: Competing interests statement:The authors declare no competing interest.
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
33. Flexible learning in complex worlds.
- Author
-
Leimar O, Quiñones AE, and Bshary R
- Abstract
Cognitive flexibility can enhance the ability to adjust to changing environments. Here, we use learning simulations to investigate the possible advantages of flexible learning in volatile (changing) environments. We compare two established learning mechanisms, one with constant learning rates and one with rates that adjust to volatility. We study an ecologically relevant case of volatility, based on observations of developing cleaner fish Labroides dimidiatus that experience a transition from a simpler to a more complex foraging environment. There are other similar transitions in nature, such as migrating to a new and different habitat. We also examine two traditional approaches to volatile environments in experimental psychology and behavioral ecology: reversal learning, and learning set formation (consisting of a sequence of different discrimination tasks). These provide experimental measures of cognitive flexibility. Concerning transitions to a complex world, we show that both constant and flexible learning rates perform well, losing only a small proportion of available rewards in the period after a transition, but flexible rates perform better than constant rates. For reversal learning, flexible rates improve the performance with each successive reversal because of increasing learning rates, but this does not happen for constant rates. For learning set formation, we find no improvement in performance with successive shifts to new stimuli to discriminate for either flexible or constant learning rates. Flexible learning rates might thus explain increasing performance in reversal learning but not in learning set formation, and this can shed light on the nature of cognitive flexibility in a given system., Competing Interests: The authors declare no conflict of interest., (© The Author(s) 2023. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the International Society for Behavioral Ecology.)
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
34. Game theory in biology: 50 years and onwards.
- Author
-
Leimar O and McNamara JM
- Subjects
- Animals, Aggression, Models, Theoretical, Biology, Cooperative Behavior, Game Theory, Biological Evolution
- Abstract
Game theory in biology gained prominence 50 years ago, when Maynard Smith & Price formulated the concept of an evolutionarily stable strategy (ESS). Their aim was to explain why conflicts between animals of the same species usually are of a 'limited war' type, not causing serious injury. They emphasized that game theory is an alternative to previous ideas about group selection, which were used by ethologists to explain limited aggression. Subsequently, the ESS concept was applied to many phenomena with frequency dependence in the evolutionary success of strategies, including sex allocation, alternative mating types, contest behaviour and signalling, cooperation, and parental care. Both the analyses of signalling and cooperation were inspired by similar problems in economics and attracted much attention in biology. Here we give a perspective on which of the ambitions in the field have been achieved, with a focus on contest behaviour and cooperation. We evaluate whether the game-theoretical study of the evolution of cooperation has measured up to expectations in explaining the behaviour of non-human animals. We also point to potentially fruitful directions for the field, and emphasize the importance of incorporating realistic behavioural mechanisms into models. This article is part of the theme issue 'Half a century of evolutionary games: a synthesis of theory, application and future directions'.
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
35. Effects of local versus global competition on reproductive skew and sex differences in social dominance behaviour.
- Author
-
Leimar O and Bshary R
- Subjects
- Female, Male, Animals, Social Behavior, Reproduction, Hierarchy, Social, Sex Characteristics, Social Dominance
- Abstract
Social hierarchies are often found in group-living animals. The hierarchy position can influence reproductive success (RS), with a skew towards high-ranking individuals. The amount of aggression in social dominance varies greatly, both between species and between males and females within species. Using game theory we study this variation by taking into account the degree to which reproductive competition in a social group is mainly local to the group, emphasizing within-group relative RS, or global to a larger population, emphasizing an individual's absolute RS. Our model is similar to recent approaches in that reinforcement learning is used as a behavioural mechanism allowing social-hierarchy formation. We test two hypotheses. The first is that local competition should favour the evolution of mating or foraging interference, and thus of reproductive skew. Second, decreases in reproductive output caused by an individual's accumulated fighting damage, such as reduced parenting ability, will favour less intense aggression but should have little influence on reproductive skew. From individual-based simulations of the evolution of social dominance and interference, we find support for both hypotheses. We discuss to what extent our results can explain observed sex differences in reproductive skew and social dominance behaviour.
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
36. Behavioural specialization and learning in social networks.
- Author
-
Leimar O, Dall SRX, Houston AI, and McNamara JM
- Subjects
- Humans, Reinforcement, Psychology, Game Theory, Social Networking
- Abstract
Interactions in social groups can promote behavioural specialization. One way this can happen is when individuals engage in activities with two behavioural options and learn which option to choose. We analyse interactions in groups where individuals learn from playing games with two actions and negatively frequency-dependent payoffs, such as producer-scrounger, caller-satellite, or hawk-dove games. Group members are placed in social networks, characterized by the group size and the number of neighbours to interact with, ranging from just a few neighbours to interactions between all group members. The networks we analyse include ring lattices and the much-studied small-world networks. By implementing two basic reinforcement-learning approaches, action-value learning and actor-critic learning, in different games, we find that individuals often show behavioural specialization. Specialization develops more rapidly when there are few neighbours in a network and when learning rates are high. There can be learned specialization also with many neighbours, but we show that, for action-value learning, behavioural consistency over time is higher with a smaller number of neighbours. We conclude that frequency-dependent competition for resources is a main driver of specialization. We discuss our theoretical results in relation to experimental and field observations of behavioural specialization in social situations.
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
37. An evolutionary perspective on stress responses, damage and repair.
- Author
-
Taborsky B, Kuijper B, Fawcett TW, English S, Leimar O, McNamara JM, and Ruuskanen S
- Subjects
- Animals, Stress, Physiological physiology, Adaptation, Physiological physiology, Hormones
- Abstract
Variation in stress responses has been investigated in relation to environmental factors, species ecology, life history and fitness. Moreover, mechanistic studies have unravelled molecular mechanisms of how acute and chronic stress responses cause physiological impacts ('damage'), and how this damage can be repaired. However, it is not yet understood how the fitness effects of damage and repair influence stress response evolution. Here we study the evolution of hormone levels as a function of stressor occurrence, damage and the efficiency of repair. We hypothesise that the evolution of stress responses depends on the fitness consequences of damage and the ability to repair that damage. To obtain some general insights, we model a simplified scenario in which an organism repeatedly encounters a stressor with a certain frequency and predictability (temporal autocorrelation). The organism can defend itself by mounting a stress response (elevated hormone level), but this causes damage that takes time to repair. We identify optimal strategies in this scenario and then investigate how those strategies respond to acute and chronic exposures to the stressor. We find that for higher repair rates, baseline and peak hormone levels are higher. This typically means that the organism experiences higher levels of damage, which it can afford because that damage is repaired more quickly, but for very high repair rates the damage does not build up. With increasing predictability of the stressor, stress responses are sustained for longer, because the animal expects the stressor to persist, and thus damage builds up. This can result in very high (and potentially fatal) levels of damage when organisms are exposed to chronic stressors to which they are not evolutionarily adapted. Overall, our results highlight that at least three factors need to be considered jointly to advance our understanding of how stress physiology has evolved: (i) temporal dynamics of stressor occurrence; (ii) relative mortality risk imposed by the stressor itself versus damage caused by the stress response; and (iii) the efficiency of repair mechanisms., (Copyright © 2022 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.)
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
38. Reproductive skew, fighting costs and winner-loser effects in social dominance evolution.
- Author
-
Leimar O and Bshary R
- Subjects
- Animals, Game Theory, Reproduction, Behavior, Animal, Social Dominance
- Abstract
Social hierarchies are often found in group-living animals and can be formed through pairwise aggressive interactions. The dominance rank can influence reproductive success (RS) with a skew towards high-ranking individuals. Using game theory, we investigate how the opportunity for differently ranked individuals to achieve RS influences the costs of hierarchy formation and the strength of winner and loser effects. In our model, individuals adjust their aggressive and submissive behaviour towards others through reinforcement learning. The learning is based on rewards and penalties, which depend on relative fighting ability. From individual-based simulations, we determine evolutionary equilibria of traits such as learning rates. We examine situations that differ in the extent of monopolisation of contested RS by dominants and in the proportion of total RS that is contested. The model implements two kinds of fighting costs: a decrease in effective fighting ability from damage (loss of condition) and a risk of mortality that increases with the total accumulated damage. Either of these costs can limit the amount of fighting. We find that individuals form stable dominance hierarchies, with a positive correlation between dominance position and fighting ability. The accumulated costs differ between dominance positions, with the highest costs paid by low or intermediately ranked individuals. Costs tend to be higher in high-skew situations. We identify a 'stay-in, opt-out' syndrome, comprising a range from weaker (stay-in) to stronger (opt-out) winner-loser effects. We interpret the opt-out phenotype to be favoured by selection on lower ranked individuals to opt out of contests over social dominance, because it is more pronounced when more of the total RS is uncontested. We discuss our results in relation to field and experimental observations and argue that there is a need for empirical investigation of the behaviour and reproductive success of lower ranked individuals., (© 2022 The Authors. Journal of Animal Ecology published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd on behalf of British Ecological Society.)
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
39. Cooperation, with friends or with relatives?
- Author
-
Leimar O and Hammerstein P
- Subjects
- Humans, Family, Friends
- Abstract
Competing Interests: The authors declare no competing interest.
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
40. The evolution of social learning as phenotypic cue integration.
- Author
-
Kuijper B, Leimar O, Hammerstein P, McNamara JM, and Dall SRX
- Subjects
- Humans, Models, Psychological, Cues, Cultural Evolution, Social Behavior, Social Learning
- Abstract
Most analyses of the origins of cultural evolution focus on when and where social learning prevails over individual learning, overlooking the fact that there are other developmental inputs that influence phenotypic fit to the selective environment. This raises the question of how the presence of other cue 'channels' affects the scope for social learning. Here, we present a model that considers the simultaneous evolution of (i) multiple forms of social learning (involving vertical or horizontal learning based on either prestige or conformity biases) within the broader context of other evolving inputs on phenotype determination, including (ii) heritable epigenetic factors, (iii) individual learning, (iv) environmental and cascading maternal effects, (v) conservative bet-hedging, and (vi) genetic cues. In fluctuating environments that are autocorrelated (and hence predictable), we find that social learning from members of the same generation (horizontal social learning) explains the large majority of phenotypic variation, whereas other cues are much less important. Moreover, social learning based on prestige biases typically prevails in positively autocorrelated environments, whereas conformity biases prevail in negatively autocorrelated environments. Only when environments are unpredictable or horizontal social learning is characterized by an intrinsically low information content, other cues such as conservative bet-hedging or vertical prestige biases prevail. This article is part of the theme issue 'Foundations of cultural evolution'.
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
41. The Evolution of Social Dominance through Reinforcement Learning.
- Author
-
Leimar O
- Subjects
- Aggression, Animals, Reinforcement, Psychology, Behavior, Animal, Game Theory, Learning, Social Dominance
- Abstract
AbstractGroups of social animals are often organized into dominance hierarchies that are formed through pairwise interactions. There is much experimental data on hierarchies, examining such things as winner, loser, and bystander effects, as well as the linearity and replicability of hierarchies, but there is a lack evolutionary analyses of these basic observations. Here I present a game theory model of hierarchy formation in which individuals adjust their aggressive behavior toward other group members through reinforcement learning. Individual traits such as the tendency to generalize learning between interactions with different individuals, the rate of learning, and the initial tendency to be aggressive are genetically determined and can be tuned by evolution. I find that evolution favors individuals with high social competence, making use of individual recognition, bystander observational learning, and, to a limited extent, generalizing learned behavior between opponents when adjusting their behavior toward other group members. The results are in qualitative agreement with experimental data, for instance, in finding weaker winner effects compared to loser effects.
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
42. Learning, exploitation and bias in games.
- Author
-
McNamara JM, Houston AI, and Leimar O
- Subjects
- Bias, Humans, Learning
- Abstract
We focus on learning during development in a group of individuals that play a competitive game with each other. The game has two actions and there is negative frequency dependence. We define the distribution of actions by group members to be an equilibrium configuration if no individual can improve its payoff by unilaterally changing its action. We show that at this equilibrium, one action is preferred in the sense that those taking the preferred action have a higher payoff than those taking the other, more prosocial, action. We explore the consequences of a simple 'unbiased' reinforcement learning rule during development, showing that groups reach an approximate equilibrium distribution, so that some achieve a higher payoff than others. Because there is learning, an individual's behaviour can influence the future behaviour of others. We show that, as a consequence, there is the potential for an individual to exploit others by influencing them to be the ones to take the non-preferred action. Using an evolutionary simulation, we show that population members can avoid being exploited by over-valuing rewards obtained from the preferred option during learning, an example of a bias that is 'rational'., Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
43. Effects of social experience, aggressiveness and comb size on contest success in male domestic fowl.
- Author
-
Favati A, Løvlie H, and Leimar O
- Abstract
The ability to dominate conspecifics and thereby gain access to resources depends on a number of traits and skills. Experience of dominance relationships during development is a potential source of learning such skills. We here study the importance of social experience, aggressiveness and morphological traits for competitiveness in social interactions (contest success) in male domestic fowl ( Gallus gallus domesticus ). We let males grow up either as a single (dominant) male or as an intermediately ranked male in a group of males, and measured their success in duels against different opponents. We found that single-raised males had lower contest success than group-raised males, and that aggression and comb size correlated positively with contest success. This indicates that experience of dominance interactions with other males increases future success in duels. We similarly studied the consequences of growing up as a dominant or subordinate in a pair of males, finding no statistically significant effect of the dominance position on contest success. Finally, we found that males were consistent over time in contest success. We conclude that social experience increases contest success in male domestic fowl, but that certain behavioural and morphological characteristics have an equal or even stronger covariation with contest success., (© 2021 The Authors.)
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
44. Towards an Evolutionary Theory of Stress Responses.
- Author
-
Taborsky B, English S, Fawcett TW, Kuijper B, Leimar O, McNamara JM, Ruuskanen S, and Sandi C
- Subjects
- Humans, Biological Evolution
- Abstract
All organisms have a stress response system to cope with environmental threats, yet its precise form varies hugely within and across individuals, populations, and species. While the physiological mechanisms are increasingly understood, how stress responses have evolved remains elusive. Here, we show that important insights can be gained from models that incorporate physiological mechanisms within an evolutionary optimality analysis (the 'evo-mecho' approach). Our approach reveals environmental predictability and physiological constraints as key factors shaping stress response evolution, generating testable predictions about variation across species and contexts. We call for an integrated research programme combining theory, experimental evolution, and comparative analysis to advance scientific understanding of how this core physiological system has evolved., (Copyright © 2020 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.)
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
45. Reinforcement Learning Theory Reveals the Cognitive Requirements for Solving the Cleaner Fish Market Task.
- Author
-
Quiñones AE, Leimar O, Lotem A, and Bshary R
- Subjects
- Animals, Computer Simulation, Feeding Behavior, Memory, Reward, Choice Behavior, Fishes physiology, Learning, Reinforcement, Psychology
- Abstract
Learning is an adaptation that allows individuals to respond to environmental stimuli in ways that improve their reproductive outcomes. The degree of sophistication in learning mechanisms potentially explains variation in behavioral responses. Here, we present a model of learning that is inspired by documented intra- and interspecific variation in the performance of a simultaneous two-choice task, the biological market task. The task presents a problem that cleaner fish often face in nature: choosing between two client types, one that is willing to wait for inspection and one that may leave if ignored. The cleaner's choice hence influences the future availability of clients (i.e., it influences food availability). We show that learning the preference that maximizes food intake requires subjects to represent in their memory different combinations of pairs of client types rather than just individual client types. In addition, subjects need to account for future consequences of actions, either by estimating expected long-term reward or by experiencing a client leaving as a penalty (negative reward). Finally, learning is influenced by the absolute and relative abundance of client types. Thus, cognitive mechanisms and ecological conditions jointly explain intra- and interspecific variation in the ability to learn the adaptive response.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
46. Learning leads to bounded rationality and the evolution of cognitive bias in public goods games.
- Author
-
Leimar O and McNamara JM
- Subjects
- Humans, Reward, Cognition, Game Theory, Learning
- Abstract
In social interactions, including cooperation and conflict, individuals can adjust their behaviour over the shorter term through learning within a generation, and natural selection can change behaviour over the longer term of many generations. Here we investigate the evolution of cognitive bias by individuals investing into a project that delivers joint benefits. For members of a group that learn how much to invest using the costs and benefits they experience in repeated interactions, we show that overestimation of the cost of investing can evolve. The bias causes individuals to invest less into the project. Our explanation is that learning responds to immediate rather than longer-term rewards. There are thus cognitive limitations in learning, which can be seen as bounded rationality. Over a time horizon of several rounds of interaction, individuals respond to each other's investments, for instance by partially compensating for another's shortfall. However, learning individuals fail to strategically take into account that social partners respond in this way. Learning instead converges to a one-shot Nash equilibrium of a game with perceived rewards as payoffs. Evolution of bias can then compensate for the cognitive limitations of learning.
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
47. Generalization of learned preferences covaries with behavioral flexibility in red junglefowl chicks.
- Author
-
Zidar J, Balogh ACV, Leimar O, and Løvlie H
- Abstract
The relationship between animal cognition and consistent among-individual behavioral differences (i.e., behavioral types, animal personality, or coping styles), has recently received increased research attention. Focus has mainly been on linking different behavioral types to performance in learning tasks. It has been suggested that behavioral differences could influence also how individuals use previously learnt information to generalize about new stimuli with similar properties. Nonetheless, this has rarely been empirically tested. Here, we therefore explore the possibility that individual variation in generalization is related to variation in behavioral types in red junglefowl chicks ( Gallus gallus ). We show that more behaviorally flexible chicks have a stronger preference for a novel stimulus that is intermediate between 2 learnt positive stimuli compared to more inflexible chicks. Thus, more flexible and inflexible chicks differ in how they generalize. Further, behavioral flexibility correlates with fearfulness, suggesting a coping style, supporting that variation in generalization is related to variation in behavioral types. How individuals generalize affects decision making and responses to novel situations or objects, and can thus have a broad influence on the life of an individual. Our results add to the growing body of evidence linking cognition to consistent behavioral differences., (© The Author(s) 2019. Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the International Society for Behavioral Ecology.)
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
48. Ecological Genetic Conflict: Genetic Architecture Can Shift the Balance between Local Adaptation and Plasticity.
- Author
-
Leimar O, Dall SRX, McNamara JM, Kuijper B, and Hammerstein P
- Subjects
- Ecosystem, Adaptation, Biological genetics, Models, Genetic
- Abstract
Genetic polymorphism can contribute to local adaptation in heterogeneous habitats, for instance, as a single locus with alleles adapted to different habitats. Phenotypic plasticity can also contribute to trait variation across habitats, through developmental responses to habitat-specific cues. We show that the genetic architecture of genetically polymorphic and plasticity loci may influence the balance between local adaptation and phenotypic plasticity. These effects of genetic architecture are instances of ecological genetic conflict. A reduced effective migration rate for genes tightly linked to a genetic polymorphism provides an explanation for the effects, and they can occur both for a single trait and for a syndrome of coadapted traits. Using individual-based simulations and numerical analysis, we investigate how among-habitat genetic polymorphism and phenotypic plasticity depend on genetic architecture. We also study the evolution of genetic architecture itself, in the form of rates of recombination between genetically polymorphic loci and plasticity loci. Our main result is that for plasticity genes that are unlinked to loci with between-habitat genetic polymorphism, the slope of a reaction norm is steeper in comparison with the slope favored by plasticity genes that are tightly linked to genes for local adaptation.
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
49. Biased generalization of salient traits drives the evolution of warning signals.
- Author
-
Gamberale-Stille G, Kazemi B, Balogh A, and Leimar O
- Subjects
- Animals, Female, Food Chain, Models, Biological, Biological Evolution, Chickens physiology, Color, Learning, Predatory Behavior
- Abstract
The importance of receiver biases in shaping the evolution of many signalling systems is widely acknowledged. Here, we show that receiver bias can explain which traits evolve to become warning signals. For warning coloration, a generalization bias for a signalling trait can result from predators learning to discriminate unprofitable from profitable prey. However, because the colour patterns of prey are complex traits with multiple components, it is crucial to understand which of the many aspects of prey appearance evolve into signals. We provide experimental evidence that the more salient differences in prey traits give rise to greater generalization bias, corresponding to stronger selection towards trait exaggeration. Our results are based on experiments with domestic chickens as predators in a Skinner-box-like setting, and imply that the difference in appearance between profitable and unprofitable prey that is most rapidly learnt produces the greatest generalization bias. As a consequence, certain salient traits of unprofitable prey are selected towards exaggeration to even higher salience, driving the evolution of warning coloration. This general idea may also help to explain the evolution of many other striking signalling traits found in nature., (© 2018 The Author(s).)
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
50. Learning of salient prey traits explains Batesian mimicry evolution.
- Author
-
Kazemi B, Gamberale-Stille G, Wåtz T, Wiklund C, and Leimar O
- Subjects
- Animals, Color, Female, Food Chain, Male, Wings, Animal physiology, Biological Mimicry, Butterflies physiology, Learning, Predatory Behavior, Songbirds physiology
- Abstract
Batesian mimicry evolution involves an initial major mutation that produces a rough resemblance to the model, followed by smaller improving changes. To examine the learning psychology of this process, we applied established ideas about mimicry in Papilio polyxenes asterius of the model Battus philenor. We performed experiments with wild birds as predators and butterfly wings as semiartificial prey. Wings of hybrids of P. p. asterius and Papilio machaon were used to approximate the first mutant, with melanism as the hypothesized first mimetic trait. Based on previous results about learning psychology and imperfect mimicry, we predicted that: melanism should have high salience (i.e., being noticeable and prominent), meaning that predators readily discriminate a melanistic mutant from appearances similar to P. machaon; the difference between the first mutant and the model should have intermediate salience to allow further improvement of mimicry; and the final difference in appearance between P. p. asterius and B. philenor should have very low salience, causing improvement to level off. Our results supported both the traditional hypothesis and all our predictions about relative salience. We conclude that there is good agreement between long-held ideas about how Batesian mimicry evolves and recent insights from learning psychology about the role of salience in mimicry evolution., (© 2018 The Author(s). Evolution © 2018 The Society for the Study of Evolution.)
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.