93 results on '"Kross E"'
Search Results
2. Self-Distancing
- Author
-
Kross, E., primary and Ayduk, O., additional
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
3. Outsourcing affect regulation to non-deceptive placebos
- Author
-
Guevarra, D., Kross, E., and Moser, J.S.
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
4. Anxiety interferes with cognitive emotion regulation implementation: Evidence from the late positive potential
- Author
-
Moser, J.S., Webster, C., and Kross, E.
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
5. Psychophysiological approaches to emotion regulation and psychopathology: Shedding light on strategies to improve mental health
- Author
-
Webster, C. and Kross, E.
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
6. Self-distancing as an adaptive self-reflection strategy
- Author
-
Verduyn, P., primary, Van Mechelen, I., additional, Kross, E., additional, Chezzi, C., additional, and Van Bever, F., additional
- Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
7. Can imagining negative consequences reduce cravings for cigarette smoking?
- Author
-
Kober, H., primary, Kross, E. F., additional, Mischel, W., additional, Hart, C. L., additional, and Ochsner, K. N., additional
- Published
- 2010
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
8. 'Asking why' from a distance: Its cognitive and emotional consequences for people with major depressive disorder.
- Author
-
Kross E, Gard D, Deldin P, Clifton J, and Ayduk O
- Published
- 2012
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
9. Facilitating adaptive emotional analysis: distinguishing distanced-analysis of depressive experiences from immersed-analysis and distraction.
- Author
-
Kross E and Ayduk O
- Published
- 2008
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
10. Admission Code Status and End-of-life Care for Hospitalized Patients With COVID-19
- Author
-
Whitney A Kiker, Si Cheng, Lauren R Pollack, Claire J Creutzfeldt, Erin K Kross, J Randall Curtis, Katherine A Belden, Roman Melamed, Donna Lee Armaignac, Smith F Heavner, Amy B Christie, Valerie M Banner-Goodspeed, Ashish K Khanna, Uluhan Sili, Harry L Anderson, Vishakha Kumar, Allan Walkey, Rahul Kashyap, Ognjen Gajic, Juan Pablo Domecq, Nita Khandelwal, and Kiker W. A. , Cheng S., Pollack L. R. , Creutzfeldt C. J. , Kross E. K. , Curtis J. R. , Belden K. A. , Melamed R., Armaignac D. L. , Heavner S. F. , et al.
- Subjects
Male ,Anesteziyoloji ve Ağrı Tıbbı ,Nursing ,CLINICAL NEUROLOGY ,Sağlık Bilimleri ,Clinical Medicine (MED) ,Nöroloji (klinik) ,Nöroloji ,Anesthesiology ,Surgery Medicine Sciences ,Health Sciences ,Humans ,Klinik Tıp (MED) ,Pandemics ,General Nursing ,Resuscitation Orders ,Retrospective Studies ,COVID ,Terminal Care ,HEMŞİRELİK ,KLİNİK NÖROLOJİ ,Internal Medicine Sciences ,Klinik Tıp ,Genel Hemşirelik ,ANESTEZİYOLOJİ ,COVID-19 ,Dahili Tıp Bilimleri ,CLINICAL MEDICINE ,Tıp ,Cross-Sectional Studies ,Anesthesiology and Pain Medicine ,Anesteziyoloji ,Neurology ,Cerrahi Tıp Bilimleri ,End of life ,Palliative care ,Medicine ,Neurology (clinical) ,Hemşirelik - Abstract
© 2022 American Academy of Hospice and Palliative MedicineContext: The COVID-19 pandemic has highlighted variability in intensity of care. We aimed to characterize intensity of care among hospitalized patients with COVID-19. Objectives: Examine the prevalence and predictors of admission code status, palliative care consultation, comfort-measures-only orders, and cardiopulmonary resuscitation (CPR) among patients hospitalized with COVID-19. Methods: This cross-sectional study examined data from an international registry of hospitalized patients with COVID-19. A proportional odds model evaluated predictors of more aggressive code status (i.e., Full Code) vs. less (i.e., Do Not Resuscitate, DNR). Among decedents, logistic regression was used to identify predictors of palliative care consultation, comfort measures only, and CPR at time of death. Results: We included 29,923 patients across 179 sites. Among those with admission code status documented, Full Code was selected by 90% (n = 15,273). Adjusting for site, Full Code was more likely for patients who were of Black or Asian race (ORs 1.82, 95% CIs 1.5–2.19; 1.78, 1.15–3.09 respectively, relative to White race), Hispanic ethnicity (OR 1.89, CI 1.35–2.32), and male sex (OR 1.16, CI 1.0–1.33). Of the 4951 decedents, 29% received palliative care consultation, 59% transitioned to comfort measures only, and 29% received CPR, with non-White racial and ethnic groups less likely to receive comfort measures only and more likely to receive CPR. Conclusion: In this international cohort of patients with COVID-19, Full Code was the initial code status in the majority, and more likely among patients who were Black or Asian race, Hispanic ethnicity or male. These results provide direction for future studies to improve these disparities in care.
- Published
- 2022
11. Culture shapes moral reasoning about close others.
- Author
-
Baldwin CR, Berg MK, Yuan J, Sowden WJ, Kitayama S, and Kross E
- Subjects
- Humans, Female, Male, Adult, United States, Japan, Young Adult, Culture, Morals
- Abstract
Moral norms balance the needs of the group versus individuals, and societies across the globe vary in terms of the norms they prioritize. Extant research indicates that people from Western cultures consistently choose to protect (vs. punish) close others who commit crimes. Might this differ in cultural contexts that prioritize the self less? Prior research presents two compelling alternatives. On the one hand, collectivists may feel more intertwined with and tied to those close to them, thus protecting close others more. On the other hand, they may prioritize society over individuals and thus protect close others less. Four studies ( N = 2,688) performed in the United States and Japan provide self-report, narrative, and experimental evidence supporting the latter hypothesis. These findings highlight how personal relationships and culture dynamically interact to shape how we think about important moral decisions. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
12. Remotely administered non-deceptive placebos reduce COVID-related stress, anxiety, and depression.
- Author
-
Guevarra DA, Webster CT, Moros JN, Kross E, and Moser JS
- Abstract
Research suggests that placebos administered without deception (i.e. non-deceptive placebos) may provide an effective and low-effort intervention to manage stress and improve mental health. However, whether non-deceptive placebos administered remotely online can manage distress for people at risk for developing high levels of affective symptoms remains unclear. Volunteers experiencing prolonged stress from the COVID-19 pandemic were recruited into a randomized controlled trial to examine the efficacy of a non-deceptive placebo intervention administered remotely online on affective outcomes. COVID-related stress, overall stress, anxiety, and depression were assessed at baseline, midpoint, and endpoint. Compared with the control group, participants in the non-deceptive placebo group reported significant reductions from baseline in all primary affective outcomes after 2 weeks. Additionally, participants in the non-deceptive placebo group found the intervention feasible, acceptable, and appropriate for the context. Non-deceptive placebos, even when administered remotely online, offer an alternative and effective way to help people manage prolonged stress. Future large-scale studies are needed to determine if non-deceptive placebos can be effective across different prolonged stress situations and for clinical populations., (© 2024 The Author(s). Applied Psychology: Health and Well‐Being published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd on behalf of International Association of Applied Psychology.)
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
13. Investigating cognitive and motivational proximal outcomes in a randomized clinical trial of writing about the future self to reduce drinking.
- Author
-
Lindgren KP, Baldwin SA, Kross E, Ramirez JJ, Tristao T, Peterson KP, Teachman BA, Wiers RW, and Neighbors C
- Abstract
Background: Drinking identity (the extent to which one links the self with drinking alcohol) is a unique risk factor for college students' hazardous drinking that is not directly targeted by existing interventions. We conducted a study that aimed to decrease drinking identity among college students with hazardous drinking. We adapted a writing task about the future self and tested whether three writing sessions could decrease drinking identity and change drinking. We also investigated whether two additional factors (writing perspective and inclusion of participants' social networks) would enhance task impact. The present study evaluated whether posited proximal cognitive and motivational outcomes (drinking identity, self-efficacy, readiness to change, and drinking intentions) changed immediately after each writing session., Method: The study is a randomized clinical trial in which hypotheses and analyses were pre-registered. Participants were 328 college students who met hazardous drinking criteria. The study had a 2 (narrative writing topic: low-risk drinker vs. reduced smartphone use) × 2 (writing perspective: first-person vs. non-first-person) × 2 (social network instruction: instructed to include vs. not) factorial design. Proximal outcomes were drinking identity, self-efficacy, readiness to change, and drinking intentions. The clinical outcome was alcohol consumption. Participants completed three laboratory sessions at weekly intervals that included the writing task and pre- and post-task assessments., Results: Results were largely null, except that readiness to reduce drinking was higher in the low-risk drinker condition and increased over the lab sessions. Time effects indicated that reductions in drinking identity, drinking intentions, and alcohol consumption, and increases in self-efficacy were observed but did not change above and beyond control conditions., Conclusions: Findings indicate the need to strengthen the writing task and select a more appropriate control task to target proposed proximal outcomes. Future studies might try personalizing the task, evaluating its efficacy with individuals motivated to change their drinking, and using a control task that does not involve imagining a future self., (© 2024 Research Society on Alcohol.)
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
14. Writing about the future self to shift drinking identity: An experimental investigation.
- Author
-
Lindgren KP, Baldwin SA, Kross E, Ramirez JJ, Peterson KP, Tristao T, Teachman BA, Wiers R, and Neighbors C
- Subjects
- Adult, Female, Humans, Male, Young Adult, Alcohol Drinking psychology, Students psychology, Universities, Writing, Alcohol Drinking in College psychology, Alcohol-Related Disorders
- Abstract
College student drinking is prevalent and costly to public and personal health, leading to calls to identify and target novel mechanisms of behavior change. We aimed to manipulate drinking identity (a cognitive risk factor for hazardous drinking) via three sessions of narrative writing about a future self. We tested whether writing could shift drinking identity and would be accompanied by changes in alcohol consumption and problems. Participants were college students meeting hazardous drinking criteria (N = 328; M
age = 20.15; 59% women, 40% men, 1% gender-diverse; 60% white; 23% Asian; 12% multiple races; 2% other racial groups; 8% identified as Hispanic/Latino/a/x). The study had a 2 [narrative writing topic: low-risk drinker vs. reduced smartphone use] × 2 [writing perspective: first person vs. non-first-person] × 2 [social network instruction: instructed to include vs. not] factorial design. Outcomes were drinking identity, drinking refusal self-efficacy, alcohol consumption, alcohol-related problems, and craving. Participants completed three writing sessions and online follow-up assessments at 2, 4, and 12 weeks. The study is a registered clinical trial; hypotheses and analyses were preregistered (https://osf.io/vy2ep/). Contrary to predictions, narrative writing about a future self as a low-risk drinker did not significantly impact outcomes. Null results extended to expected interactions with writing perspective and social network instructions. The narrative writing task did not shift drinking or alcohol-related outcomes. Future experimental work may benefit from greater flexibility in conceptualizing a future self, recruiting individuals interested in behavior change, and more sensitive measures of drinking identity., (Copyright © 2023 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.)- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
15. Sensory emotion regulation.
- Author
-
Rodriguez M and Kross E
- Subjects
- Animals, Emotions physiology, Sensation, Emotional Regulation
- Abstract
Decades of evidence reveal intimate links between sensation and emotion. Yet, discussion of sensory experiences as tools that promote emotion regulation is largely absent from current theorizing on this topic. Here, we address this gap by integrating evidence from social-personality, clinical, cognitive-neuroscience, and animal research to highlight the role of sensation as a tool that can be harnessed to up- or downregulate emotion. Further, we review evidence implicating sensation as a rapid and relatively effortless emotion regulation modality and highlight future research directions. Notably, we emphasize the need to examine the duration of sensory emotion regulation effects, the moderating role of individual and cultural differences, and how sensory strategies interact with other strategies., Competing Interests: Declaration of interests No interests are declared., (Copyright © 2023 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.)
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
16. Maturing Out: Between- and Within-Persons Changes in Social-Network Drinking, Drinking Identity, and Hazardous Drinking Following College Graduation.
- Author
-
Lindgren KP, Baldwin SA, Peterson KP, Ramirez JJ, Teachman BA, Kross E, Wiers RW, and Neighbors C
- Abstract
Many college students reduce hazardous drinking (HD) following graduation without treatment. Identifying cognitive mechanisms facilitating this "natural" reduction in HD during this transition is crucial. We evaluated drinking identity as a potential mechanism and tested whether within-person changes in one's social network's drinking were linked to within-person changes in drinking identity and subsequent within-person changes in HD. A sample of 422 undergraduates reporting HD was followed from six months before until two years after graduation. Their drinking, drinking identity, and social networks were assessed online. Within-person changes in drinking identity did not mediate the relationship between within-person changes in social network drinking and personal HD, though significant positive between-person associations among all constructs were found. Instead, there was some evidence that within-person changes in drinking identity followed changes in HD, suggesting that drinking identity may function as a marker versus mechanism of "natural" HD reduction during transition out of college.
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
17. The Role of the Palliative Medicine Clinician in the Family Conference.
- Author
-
Vendetta L, Vig E, Kross E, and Merel SE
- Subjects
- Humans, Palliative Care, Communication, Palliative Medicine
- Abstract
Facilitating a family conference is a core skill for a palliative medicine clinician, yet the role of the palliative medicine consultant in a family conference has not been clearly defined in the literature. Most educational articles describe a structured approach to a family conference that focuses on the role of the person leading the conference, who may be a palliative medicine specialist or a member of the primary team caring for the patient. For the palliative medicine clinician, balancing the roles of communication facilitator and palliative consultant is nuanced and requires a specific framework and set of skills. In this article, we review the literature on family conferences focusing on facilitation and communication by the palliative care consultant during the conferences, and outline specific ways the palliative medicine clinician can contribute to family conferences. Our hope is that this framework helps guide palliative medicine clinicians and others seeking more specialized training in palliative medicine to be more intentional with their contributions to family conferences in the future. We also hope that this framework will help palliative medicine educators training future specialists.
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
18. Learning the rules of the game: The role of generic "you" and "we" in shaping children's interpretations of norms.
- Author
-
Orvell A, Elli G, Umscheid V, Simmons E, Kross E, and Gelman SA
- Subjects
- Male, Female, Child, Humans, Child, Preschool, Language, Judgment, Social Norms, Learning, Child Development
- Abstract
A critical skill of childhood is learning social norms. We examine whether the generic pronouns "you" and "we," which frame information as applying to people in general rather than to a specific individual, facilitate this process. In one pre-registered experiment conducted online between 2020 and 2021, children 4- to 9-year-old primarily living in the midwestern U.S. (N = 146, 75 girls, 71 boys, M
age = 7.14, SD = 1.69, 82% White) interpreted actions described with generic pronouns (vs. "I") as normatively correct and selected the speaker who used generic pronouns as the rule-follower, particularly when generic pronouns were presented first. There were no significant effects of age. These results illustrate how generic pronouns influence how children discern unfamiliar norms and form interpersonal judgments., (© 2022 The Authors. Child Development © 2022 Society for Research in Child Development.)- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
19. Patient Perspectives on Longitudinal Adherence to Lung Cancer Screening.
- Author
-
Holman A, Kross E, Crothers K, Cole A, Wernli K, and Triplette M
- Subjects
- Humans, Insurance Coverage, Mass Screening methods, Surveys and Questionnaires, Early Detection of Cancer methods, Lung Neoplasms diagnosis
- Abstract
Background: Annual lung cancer screening (LCS) has mortality benefits for eligible participants; however, studies demonstrate low adherence to follow-up LCS., Research Question: What are patients' perspectives on barriers and facilitators to adherence to annual LCS?, Study Design and Methods: Forty participants enrolled in the University of Washington/Seattle Cancer Care Alliance LCS program completed a demographic questionnaire and a semistructured interview based on the Tailored Implementation in Chronic Diseases framework to determine attitudes, barriers, and facilitators to longitudinal LCS. Interviews were coded using principles of framework analysis to identify and compare themes between adherent and nonadherent participants., Results: The 40 participants underwent initial LCS in 2017 with negative results. Seventeen were adherent to follow-up annual LCS, whereas 23 were not. Seven overall themes emerged from qualitative analysis, which are summarized as follows: (1) screening experiences are positive and participants have positive attitudes toward screening; (2) provider recommendation is a motivator and key facilitator for most patients; (3) many patients are influenced by personal factors and symptoms and do not understand the importance of asymptomatic screening; (4) common barriers to longitudinal screening include cost, insurance coverage, accessibility, and other medical conditions; (5) patients have variable preferences about how they receive their screening results, and many have residual questions about their results and future screening; (6) reminders are an important facilitator of annual screening; and (7) most patients think a navigator would be beneficial to the screening process, with different aspects of navigation thought to be most helpful. Those who were not adherent more commonly reported individual barriers to screening, competing health concerns, and less provider communication., Interpretation: Key facilitators (eg, patient reminders, provider recommendations) may improve long-term screening behavior, and a number of barriers to the screening process could be addressed through patient navigation., (Copyright © 2022 American College of Chest Physicians. All rights reserved.)
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
20. An event-related potential investigation of distanced self-talk: Replication and comparison to detached reappraisal.
- Author
-
Webster CT, Berg MK, Kross E, and Moser JS
- Subjects
- Arousal physiology, Cognition physiology, Emotions physiology, Evoked Potentials physiology, Humans, Electroencephalography methods, Emotional Regulation
- Abstract
Emotion regulation is critical for managing stress, but many regulatory strategies consume high levels of cognitive resources to implement, which are depleted under stress. This raises a conundrum: the tools we have to feel better may be ineffective when they are most needed. Recent event-related potential (ERP) research indicates that distanced self-talk (i.e., reflecting on one's experiences using non-first-person singular pronouns and one's name) reduces negative emotional reactivity without overtaxing cognitive resources. Here, we report the first direct replication of this work and extend it by examining how distanced self-talk compares to detached reappraisal, one of the most frequently studied regulatory techniques. Sixty-seven participants were randomly assigned to an emotion regulation picture task and instructed to reflect on the feelings they experienced in response to viewing negative emotional images using distanced self-talk or detached reappraisal while ERPs were measured. Directly replicating past findings, distanced self-talk led to a reduction in an affective arousal ERP, the late positive potential (LPP), without increasing stimulus preceding negativity (SPN), an ERP that reflects anticipatory and preparatory processing. These results further bolster support for distanced self-talk as a relatively effortless emotion regulation strategy. On the other hand, detached reappraisal was neither associated with the modulation of the LPP nor the SPN. Due to the failed replication of the reappraisal effect, a direct comparison between emotion regulation strategies was not conducted. Methodological limitations that may have contributed to the reappraisal failure and future directions for comparisons between emotion regulation strategies are discussed., (Copyright © 2022 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.)
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
21. How Spanish speakers express norms using generic person markers.
- Author
-
Salvador CE, Orvell A, Kross E, and Gelman SA
- Subjects
- Humans, Reference Values, Language, Linguistics
- Abstract
Language is one powerful vehicle for transmitting norms-a universal feature of society. In English, people use "you" generically (e.g., "You win some you lose some") to express and interpret norms. Here, we examine how norms are conveyed and interpreted in Spanish, a language that-unlike English-has two forms of you (i.e., formal, informal), distinct generic person markers, and pro-drop, allowing for an examination of underlying conceptual tendencies in how the structure of language facilitates the transmission of norms. In Study 1a-b (N = 838) Spanish speakers used informal generic-you and the generic person marker "se" (but not formal-you) to express norms (vs. preferences). In Study 2 (N = 300), formal you, informal you, and impersonal "se" had persuasive force over personal endorsements (e.g., "I"), informing Spanish speaker's interpretation of unfamiliar norms. Our findings add to a growing literature on how subtle linguistic shifts reflect and influence cognitive processes., (© 2022. The Author(s).)
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
22. Distanced self-talk increases rational self-interest.
- Author
-
Gainsburg I, Sowden WJ, Drake B, Herold W, and Kross E
- Subjects
- Adult, Communication, Decision Making, Emotions, Female, Humans, Male, Morals, Young Adult, Psychological Distance, Rationalization
- Abstract
Does stepping back to evaluate a situation from a distanced perspective lead us to be selfish or fair? This question has been of philosophical interest for centuries, and, more recently, the focus of extensive empirical inquiry. Yet, extant research reveals a puzzle: some studies suggest that adopting a distanced perspective will produce more rationally self-interested behavior, whereas others suggest that it will produce more impartial behavior. Here we adjudicate between these perspectives by testing the effects of adopting a third-person perspective on decision making in a task that pits rational self-interest against impartiality: the dictator game. Aggregating across three experiments (N = 774), participants who used third-person (i.e., distanced) vs. first-person (i.e., immersed) self-talk during the dictator game kept more money for themselves. We discuss these results in light of prior research showing that psychological distance can promote cooperation and fairmindedness and how the effect of psychological distance on moral decision-making may be sensitive to social context., (© 2022. The Author(s).)
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
23. What we would (but shouldn't) do for those we love: Universalism versus partiality in responding to others' moral transgressions.
- Author
-
Soter LK, Berg MK, Gelman SA, and Kross E
- Subjects
- Humans, Moral Obligations, Love, Morals
- Abstract
Recent work indicates that people are more likely to protect a close (vs. distant) other who commits a crime. But do people think it is morally right to treat close others differently? On the one hand, universalist moral principles dictate that people should be treated equally. On the other hand, close relationships are the source of special moral obligations, which may lead people to believe they ought to preferentially protect close others. Here we attempt to adjudicate between these competing considerations by examining what people think they would and should do when a close (vs. distant) other behaves immorally. Across four experiments (N = 2002), we show that people believe they morally should protect close others more than distant others. However, we also document a striking discrepancy: participants reported that they would protect close others far more than they should protect them. These findings demonstrate that people believe close relationships influence what they morally ought to do-but also that moral decisions about close others may be a context in which people are particularly likely to fail to do what they think is morally right., (Copyright © 2021 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.)
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
24. Examining emotional tool use in daily life.
- Author
-
Weidman AC and Kross E
- Subjects
- Adaptation, Psychological, Anger, Ecological Momentary Assessment, Emotional Regulation, Humans, Jealousy, Mental Recall, Emotions
- Abstract
Emotions such as anger, gratitude, envy, and pride can be thought of as tools: They tend to serve context-specific functions in daily life. Prior work has shown that people can use emotions as tools in laboratory contexts, yet it is unclear whether people do use emotions as tools in daily life by intentionally trying to feel or express emotions that could yield context-specific beneficial outcomes. We examined this issue in 6 studies (total N = 1,409) in which participants (a) identified scenarios where specific emotions typically function as tools, (b) recalled episodes of emotional tool use, and (c) reported on emotional tool use in daily life via experience-sampling under experimental instructions. We found that people regularly used emotions as tools in daily life, but that people used positive emotions as tools much more frequently than negative emotions. Yet, when people used positive emotions as tools, this led to less beneficial outcomes than when participants felt positive emotions reactively-in part because using positive emotions as tools felt inauthentic-whereas using negative emotions as tools led to more beneficial outcomes than feeling negative emotions reactively. These findings point to a fascinating paradox: Although people are more willing to use positive (vs. negative) emotions as tools, these choices may not lead people to garner maximal possible benefits of positive emotions, while preventing people from capitalizing on the benefits of using negative emotions as tools. We discuss implications of this work for incorporating emotional tool use into theories of emotion regulation. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
25. The impact of social network sites on mental health: distinguishing active from passive use.
- Author
-
Verduyn P, Gugushvili N, and Kross E
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
26. Social Media and Well-Being: Pitfalls, Progress, and Next Steps.
- Author
-
Kross E, Verduyn P, Sheppes G, Costello CK, Jonides J, and Ybarra O
- Subjects
- Emotions, Humans, Knowledge, Social Media
- Abstract
Within a relatively short time span, social media have transformed the way humans interact, leading many to wonder what, if any, implications this interactive revolution has had for people's emotional lives. Over the past 15 years, an explosion of research has examined this issue, generating countless studies and heated debate. Although early research generated inconclusive findings, several experiments have revealed small negative effects of social media use on well-being. These results mask, however, a deeper set of complexities. Accumulating evidence indicates that social media can enhance or diminish well-being depending on how people use them. Future research is needed to model these complexities using stronger methods to advance knowledge in this domain., (Copyright © 2020 The Authors. Published by Elsevier Ltd.. All rights reserved.)
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
27. "You" speaks to me: Effects of generic-you in creating resonance between people and ideas.
- Author
-
Orvell A, Kross E, and Gelman SA
- Subjects
- Adult, Female, Humans, Linguistics, Male, Multilevel Analysis, Language
- Abstract
Creating resonance between people and ideas is a central goal of communication. Historically, attempts to understand the factors that promote resonance have focused on altering the content of a message. Here we identify an additional route to evoking resonance that is embedded in the structure of language: the generic use of the word "you" (e.g., "You can't understand someone until you've walked a mile in their shoes"). Using crowd-sourced data from the Amazon Kindle application, we demonstrate that passages that people highlighted-collectively, over a quarter of a million times-were substantially more likely to contain generic-you compared to yoked passages that they did not highlight. We also demonstrate in four experiments ( n = 1,900) that ideas expressed with generic-you increased resonance. These findings illustrate how a subtle shift in language establishes a powerful sense of connection between people and ideas., Competing Interests: The authors declare no competing interest., (Copyright © 2020 the Author(s). Published by PNAS.)
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
28. Social comparison on social networking sites.
- Author
-
Verduyn P, Gugushvili N, Massar K, Täht K, and Kross E
- Subjects
- Humans, Social Comparison, Social Networking
- Abstract
Because of the rise of social networking sites (SNSs), social comparisons take place at an unprecedented rate and scale. There is a growing concern that these online social comparisons negatively impact people's subjective well-being (SWB). In this paper, we review research on (a) the antecedents of social comparisons on SNSs, (b) the consequences of social comparisons on SNSs for SWB and, (c) social comparison as a mechanism explaining (mediator) or affecting (moderator) the relationship between SNSs and SWB. The occurrence of social comparisons on SNSs depends on who uses the SNS and on how the SNS is being used with passive use in particular resulting in increased levels of social comparison. Moreover, social comparison on SNSs may occasionally result in an increase in SWB but typically negative effects are found as people tend to engage in contrasting upward social comparisons. Finally, several studies show that social comparison is a key mechanism explaining the relationship between use of SNSs and SWB and that users with a tendency to engage in social comparison are especially likely to be negatively impacted by SNSs. The dynamic, cyclical processes that result from this pattern of findings are discussed., (Copyright © 2020 The Author(s). Published by Elsevier Ltd.. All rights reserved.)
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
29. Placebos without deception reduce self-report and neural measures of emotional distress.
- Author
-
Guevarra DA, Moser JS, Wager TD, and Kross E
- Subjects
- Adolescent, Electroencephalography, Female, Humans, Male, Self Report statistics & numerical data, Stress, Psychological diagnosis, Stress, Psychological psychology, Treatment Outcome, Young Adult, Placebos administration & dosage, Psychological Distress, Stress, Psychological prevention & control
- Abstract
Several recent studies suggest that placebos administered without deception (i.e., non-deceptive placebos) can help people manage a variety of highly distressing clinical disorders and nonclinical impairments. However, whether non-deceptive placebos represent genuine psychobiological effects is unknown. Here we address this issue by demonstrating across two experiments that during a highly arousing negative picture viewing task, non-deceptive placebos reduce both a self-report and neural measure of emotional distress, the late positive potential. These results show that non-deceptive placebo effects are not merely a product of response bias. Additionally, they provide insight into the neural time course of non-deceptive placebo effects on emotional distress and the psychological mechanisms that explain how they function.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
30. Punish or Protect? How Close Relationships Shape Responses to Moral Violations.
- Author
-
Weidman AC, Sowden WJ, Berg MK, and Kross E
- Subjects
- Adult, Altruism, Emotions, Female, Humans, Judgment, Male, Middle Aged, Young Adult, Interpersonal Relations, Morals, Punishment psychology
- Abstract
People have fundamental tendencies to punish immoral actors and treat close others altruistically. What happens when these tendencies collide-do people punish or protect close others who behave immorally? Across 10 studies ( N = 2,847), we show that people consistently anticipate protecting close others who commit moral infractions, particularly highly severe acts of theft and sexual harassment. This tendency emerged regardless of gender, political orientation, moral foundations, and disgust sensitivity and was driven by concerns about self-interest, loyalty, and harm. We further find that people justify this tendency by planning to discipline close others on their own. We also identify a psychological mechanism that mitigates the tendency to protect close others who have committed severe (but not mild) moral infractions: self-distancing. These findings highlight the role that relational closeness plays in shaping people's responses to moral violations, underscoring the need to consider relational closeness in future moral psychology work.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
31. When chatting about negative experiences helps-and when it hurts: Distinguishing adaptive versus maladaptive social support in computer-mediated communication.
- Author
-
Lee DS, Orvell A, Briskin J, Shrapnell T, Gelman SA, Ayduk O, Ybarra O, and Kross E
- Subjects
- Aged, Female, Humans, Male, Communication, Computers standards, Emotions physiology, Social Support
- Abstract
Does talking to others about negative experiences improve the way people feel? Although some work suggests that the answer to this question is "yes," other work reveals the opposite. Here we attempt to shed light on this puzzle by examining how people can talk to others about their negative experiences constructively via computer-mediated communication, a platform that people increasingly use to provide and receive social support. Drawing from prior research on meaning-making and self-reflection, we predicted that cueing participants to reconstrue their experience in ways that lead them to focus on it from a broader perspective during a conversation would buffer them against negative affect and enhance their sense of closure compared with cueing them to recount the emotionally arousing details concerning what happened. Results supported this prediction. Content analyses additionally revealed that participants in the reconstrue condition used the word "you" generically (e.g., you cannot always get what you want) more than participants in the recount condition, identifying a linguistic mechanism that supports reconstrual. These findings highlight the psychological processes that distinguish adaptive versus maladaptive ways of talking about negative experiences, particularly in the context of computer-mediated support interactions. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights reserved).
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
32. Effect of the Promoting Resilience in Stress Management Intervention for Parents of Children With Cancer (PRISM-P): A Randomized Clinical Trial.
- Author
-
Rosenberg AR, Bradford MC, Junkins CC, Taylor M, Zhou C, Sherr N, Kross E, Curtis JR, and Yi-Frazier JP
- Subjects
- Adaptation, Psychological, Adolescent, Adult, Child, Child, Preschool, Female, Follow-Up Studies, Health Care Surveys, Humans, Male, Middle Aged, Program Evaluation, Social Support, United States, Young Adult, Child, Hospitalized psychology, Neoplasms psychology, Parents psychology, Resilience, Psychological, Stress, Psychological therapy
- Abstract
Importance: Parents of children with serious illness, such as cancer, experience high stress and distress. Few parent-specific psychosocial interventions have been evaluated in randomized trials., Objective: To determine if individual- or group-based delivery of a novel intervention called Promoting Resilience in Stress Management for Parents (PRISM-P) improves parent-reported resilience compared with usual care., Design, Setting, and Participants: This parallel, phase 2 randomized clinical trial with enrollment from December 2016 through December 2018 and 3-month follow-up was conducted at Seattle Children's Hospital. English-speaking parents or guardians of children who were 2 to 24 years old, who had received a diagnosis of a new malignant neoplasm 1 to 10 weeks prior to enrollment, and who were receiving cancer-directed therapy at Seattle Children's Hospital were included. Parents were randomized 1:1:1 to the one-on-one or group PRISM-P intervention or to usual care. Data were analyzed in 2019 (primary analyses from January to March 2019; final analyses in July 2019)., Interventions: The PRISM-P is a manualized, brief intervention targeting 4 skills: stress management, goal setting, cognitive reframing, and meaning making. For one-on-one delivery, skills were taught privately and in person for 30 to 60 minutes approximately every other week. For group delivery, the same skills were taught in a single session with at least 2 parents present., Main Outcomes and Measures: Participants completed patient-reported outcome surveys at enrollment and at 3 months. Linear regression modeling evaluated associations in the intention-to-treat population between each delivery format and the primary outcome (Connor-Davidson Resilience Scale scores, ranging from 0 to 40, with higher scores reflecting greater resilience) and secondary outcomes (benefit finding, social support, health-related quality of life, stress, and distress) at 3 months., Results: In total, 94 parents enrolled, were randomized to 1 of the 3 groups, and completed baseline surveys (32 parents in one-on-one sessions, 32 in group sessions, and 30 in usual care). Their median (interquartile range) ages were 35 to 38 (31-44) years across the 3 groups, and they were predominantly white, college-educated mothers. Their children had median (interquartile range) ages of 5 to 8 (3-14) years; slightly more than half of the children were boys, and the most common cancer type was leukemia or lymphoma. One-on-one PRISM-P delivery was significantly associated with improvement compared with usual care in parent-reported outcomes for resilience (β, 2.3; 95% CI, 0.1-4.6; P = .04) and for benefit finding (β, 0.5; 95% CI, 0.2-0.8; P = .001). No significant associations were detected between either platform and other parent-reported outcomes., Conclusions and Relevance: When delivered individually, PRISM-P was associated with improved parent-reported resilience and benefit finding. This scalable psychosocial intervention may help parents cope and find meaning after their child receives a diagnosis of a serious illness., Trial Registration: ClinicalTrials.gov identifier: NCT02998086.
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
33. Focusing on the future from afar: Self-distancing from future stressors facilitates adaptive coping.
- Author
-
White RE, Kuehn MM, Duckworth AL, Kross E, and Ayduk Ö
- Subjects
- Adult, Female, Humans, Male, Young Adult, Adaptation, Psychological physiology, Anxiety psychology, Emotions physiology
- Abstract
Prior research indicates that visual self-distancing enhances adaptive self-reflection about negative past events (Kross & Ayduk, 2011). However, whether this process is similarly useful when people reflect on anxiety-provoking future negative experiences, and if so, whether a similar set of mechanisms underlie its benefits in this context, is unknown. Here we addressed these questions using a combination of experimental and individual difference methods with adults and adolescents (total N = 2,344). In Studies 1 and 2, spontaneous self-distancing predicted less anxious emotional reactivity among adults and adolescents. This effect was mediated by differences in how vividly participants imagined a future anxiety-provoking event. Study 3 provided causal evidence in an adult sample: Adopting a self-distanced (vs. self-immersed) perspective when reflecting on a future stressor led to lower levels of anxiety as well as lower imagery vividness. Consistent with Studies 1 and 2, reductions in imagery vividness mediated the emotion regulatory benefits of self-distancing. A meta-analysis of all three studies further confirmed these findings across samples. Thus, the current studies extend previous research on the benefits of self-distancing to future stressors. In addition, they highlight a novel mechanism for this relation: imagery vividness. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all rights reserved).
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
34. Experimental Paradigm for Measuring the Effects of Self-distancing in Young Children.
- Author
-
Grenell A, White RE, Prager EO, Schaefer C, Kross E, Duckworth AL, and Carlson SM
- Subjects
- Adaptation, Psychological, Child, Child, Preschool, Executive Function physiology, Female, Humans, Male, Task Performance and Analysis, Emotions physiology
- Abstract
Self-distancing (i.e., creating mental distance between the self and a stimulus by adopting a less egocentric perspective) has been studied as a way to improve adolescents' and adults' emotion regulation. These studies instruct adolescents and adults to use visual imagery or language to create distance from the self before engaging in self-regulation tasks and when thinking about past and future events. For example, adults are asked to recall past, negative emotional experiences from either a first-person perspective (no distance) or a third-person perspective (self-distanced). These studies show that a self-distanced perspective allows adults to cope more adaptively when recalling negative feelings. However, the self-distancing paradigm used with adults was not developmentally appropriate for young children. This modified self-distancing paradigm involves instructing children to think about their thoughts, feelings, and actions from different perspectives that vary in their distance from the self while completing a self-regulation task. The paradigm involves randomly assigning children to use one of three perspectives: self-immersed, third-person, or exemplar. In the self-immersed condition, children are asked to think about themselves using the first-person perspective (e.g., "How am I feeling?") and no distance is created from the self. In the third-person condition, children are asked to create distance from the self by using the third-person perspective (e.g., "How is [child's name] feeling?"). In the exemplar condition, the greatest distance from the self is created by asking children to pretend to be a media character and to think about that character's thoughts and feelings (e.g., "How is Batman feeling?"). Studies using the self-distancing paradigm with 4-6-year-olds have found that as the amount of distance from the self increases (self-immersed < third-person < exemplar), children perform better on self-regulation tasks. These findings suggest that the strategies implemented in the self-distancing protocol may be useful to include in self-regulation interventions for young children.
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
35. Individual differences in the effectiveness of self-distancing for young children's emotion regulation.
- Author
-
Grenell A, Prager EO, Schaefer C, Kross E, Duckworth AL, and Carlson SM
- Subjects
- Age Factors, Child, Child, Preschool, Female, Humans, Male, Child Behavior physiology, Child Development physiology, Executive Function physiology, Individuality, Self-Control, Theory of Mind physiology
- Abstract
Self-distancing has been shown to improve children's self-regulation in a variety of tasks. However, it is unknown whether this strategy is more effective for some children than others. This study investigated self-distancing in relation to individual differences in executive function (EF) and effortful control (EC). Typically developing 4- (n = 72) and 6-year-olds (n = 67) were randomly assigned to think about the self from one of four perspectives: self-immersed, control, third-person, or competent media character. Children participated in a frustrating task for up to 10 min and overt expressions of frustration were coded. Conceptually replicating prior research with adults, younger children, and children with lower EF and lower EC (independent of age) benefitted the most from self-distancing. This suggests self-distancing is especially effective during a frustrating task for children with less developed self-control, adding to a growing body of research showing self-distancing is especially effective for vulnerable individuals. Statement of contribution What is already known on this subject? Using third-person speech and pretending to be a media character improve children's self-regulation. Age and theory of mind skills are related to the effectiveness of self-distancing. What does this study add? Self-distancing can help children regulate their emotions during an emotionally charged task. Individual differences in executive function and effortful control are related to the efficacy of self-distancing., (© 2018 The British Psychological Society.)
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
36. Does counting emotion words on online social networks provide a window into people's subjective experience of emotion? A case study on Facebook.
- Author
-
Kross E, Verduyn P, Boyer M, Drake B, Gainsburg I, Vickers B, Ybarra O, and Jonides J
- Subjects
- Adult, Ecological Momentary Assessment, Female, Humans, Male, Young Adult, Emotions physiology, Social Networking
- Abstract
Psychologists have long debated whether it is possible to assess how people subjectively feel without asking them. The recent proliferation of online social networks has recently added a fresh chapter to this discussion, with research now suggesting that it is possible to index people's subjective experience of emotion by simply counting the number of emotion words contained in their online social network posts. Whether the conclusions that emerge from this work are valid, however, rests on a critical assumption: that people's usage of emotion words in their posts accurately reflects how they feel. Although this assumption is widespread in psychological research, here we suggest that there are reasons to challenge it. We corroborate these assertions in 2 ways. First, using data from 4 experience-sampling studies of emotion in young adults, we show that people's reports of how they feel throughout the day neither predict, nor are predicted by, their use of emotion words on Facebook. Second, using simulations we show that although significant relationships emerge between the use of emotion words on Facebook and self-reported affect with increasingly large numbers of observations, the relationship between these variables was in the opposite of the theoretically expected direction 50% of the time (i.e., 3 of 6 models that we performed simulations on). In contrast to counting emotion words, we show that judges' ratings of the emotionality of participants' Facebook posts consistently predicts how people feel across all analyses. These findings shed light on how to draw inferences about emotion using online social network data. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all rights reserved).
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
37. Lessons learned: Young children's use of generic-you to make meaning from negative experiences.
- Author
-
Orvell A, Kross E, and Gelman SA
- Subjects
- Child, Child, Preschool, Female, Humans, Male, Child Behavior physiology, Child Development physiology, Comprehension physiology, Emotions physiology, Verbal Behavior physiology
- Abstract
Learning from negative experiences is an essential challenge of childhood. How do children derive meaning from such events? For adults, one way is to move beyond the specifics of a situation by framing it as exemplifying a more general phenomenon. Here we examine whether children are able to make meaning in this way through their use of generic-you, a linguistic device in which people shift from the here and now to refer to people in general. Participants ( N = 89, aged 4-10 years) listened to 2 stories depicting common conflicts and were asked to discuss what lessons the character could learn (Lessons Learned condition) and how the character felt (Relive condition). In the Lessons Learned condition, children were more likely to produce generic-you than in the Relive condition. These findings suggest that young children can make meaning from negative experiences by transcending the immediate context of an event to cast it as normative and general. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2018 APA, all rights reserved).
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
38. Out of the Mouths of Babes: Links Between Linguistic Structure of Loss Narratives and Psychosocial Functioning in Parentally Bereaved Children.
- Author
-
Kaplow JB, Wardecker BM, Layne CM, Kross E, Burnside A, Edelstein RS, and Prossin AR
- Subjects
- Child, Death, Female, Humans, Interviews as Topic, Linguistics, Male, Parents, Self Report, Adaptation, Psychological, Bereavement, Narration, Stress Disorders, Post-Traumatic psychology, Stress, Psychological psychology
- Abstract
This study examined links between the language bereaved children use to describe the death of their caregiver and children's psychological/behavioral functioning and coping strategies. Participants included 44 children (54.5% male) aged 7 to 12 (M = 9.05) years who were bereaved by the death of a caregiver. Children were assessed via self- and caregiver-report measures and an in-person interview regarding the loss of their caregiver. Children's loss narratives gathered through in-person interviews were transcribed and subjected to textual analysis. Linguistic categories included pronouns and verb tense. Drawing from linguistic and self-distancing theories, we hypothesized that children's use of language reflecting self-distancing (third-person pronouns and past tense) or social connectedness (first-person plural pronouns) would be negatively associated with psychological/behavioral distress and avoidant coping. Similarly, we expected that children's use of self-focused language (first-person singular pronouns and present tense) would be positively associated with psychological/behavioral distress and avoidant coping. As hypothesized, preliminary findings suggest that children who employed more self-distancing language and used more social connectedness words reported less avoidant coping, rs = .40-.42. Also as hypothesized, children who employed more self-focused language had higher levels of self-reported posttraumatic stress symptoms, r = .54, and avoidant coping, r = .54, and higher parent-reported psychological/behavioral distress, r = .43. Implications for theory-building, risk screening, and directions for future research with bereaved youth are discussed., (Copyright © 2018 International Society for Traumatic Stress Studies.)
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
39. That's how "you" do it: Generic you expresses norms during early childhood.
- Author
-
Orvell A, Kross E, and Gelman SA
- Subjects
- Child, Child, Preschool, Female, Humans, Male, Single-Blind Method, Child Language, Psychology, Child, Social Norms
- Abstract
Prior research indicates that children construe norms as general and construe preferences as individual. The current studies tested whether this expectation is built into how children interpret and use language. We focused on the pronoun you, which is ambiguous between a canonical interpretation (referring to the addressee) and a generic interpretation (referring to people in general). In Study 1, children (N=132, ages 3-10years) were asked a series of questions containing "you," referring to either descriptive norms (e.g., "What do you do with bikes?") or preferences (e.g., "What do you like to do with bikes?"). In Study 2, parents conversed with their children (N=28, ages 2-4years) about prescriptive norms (e.g., "What should you do with books?") and preferences (e.g., "What do you like about books?"). In both studies, children's choice of pronoun in their answer revealed whether they interpreted you in the questions as generic or canonical. Results indicated that children more often interpreted you as generic in the normative contexts (i.e., responded with generic you, e.g., "You read them") and as canonical in the preference contexts (i.e., responded with I, e.g., "I read them"). This pattern emerged by early preschool, providing the first evidence that the distinction between norms and preferences directs young children's interpretation and use of everyday language., (Copyright © 2017 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.)
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
40. A Preliminary Study of Central Nervous System Arousal and Sleep Quality in Bipolar Disorder.
- Author
-
Cardinale R, O'Donnell L, Chun J, Park J, Kross E, Kamali M, McInnis M, and Deldin P
- Subjects
- Adult, Female, Humans, Male, Middle Aged, Young Adult, Arousal physiology, Bipolar Disorder complications, Central Nervous System physiopathology, Polysomnography methods, Sleep Stages physiology
- Abstract
Sleep disturbances are commonly reported in patients with bipolar I disorder (BPI) and are risk factors for mood episodes. In other populations, central nervous system (CNS) hyperarousal is associated with sleep initiation and maintenance problems, and CNS hypoarousal is associated with increased sleep drive. However, it is unclear whether CNS arousal levels are a useful index of sleep disruption in BPI. This study aimed to investigate daytime CNS arousal levels in relation to perceived sleep quality in BPI. Resting EEG, mood state, and self-reported sleep quality data were collected from 34 individuals with BPI. CNS hyperarousal was associated with pervasive poor subjective sleep quality including increased sleep disturbances, increased sleep latency, and reduced global sleep quality. CNS hypoarousal was associated with greater daytime sleepiness, indicating reduced arousal. These preliminary findings suggest that CNS arousal may be a useful index for identifying individuals at high risk for relapse into a mood episode. A limitation of this study is the use of self-report instruments for sleep quality assessment. Future research should investigate the temporal relationship of CNS arousal to sleep disturbances using objective measurements of sleep quality such as polysomnography. If these findings are replicated, measures of CNS arousals may allow for identification of high-risk patients with BPI., (© 2018 S. Karger AG, Basel.)
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
41. Third-Person Self-Talk Reduces Ebola Worry and Risk Perception by Enhancing Rational Thinking.
- Author
-
Kross E, Vickers BD, Orvell A, Gainsburg I, Moran TP, Boyer M, Jonides J, Moser J, and Ayduk O
- Subjects
- Adult, Anxiety therapy, Female, Humans, Male, Middle Aged, Thinking, Young Adult, Anxiety psychology, Emotions, Health Knowledge, Attitudes, Practice, Hemorrhagic Fever, Ebola psychology, Psychotherapy methods, Self-Control psychology, Verbal Behavior
- Abstract
Background: During the fall of 2014, the threat of an Ebola outbreak gripped the United States (Poll, 8-12 October 2014; see Harvard School of Public Health & SSRS, 2014), creating a unique opportunity to advance basic knowledge concerning how emotion regulation works in consequential contexts and translate existing research in this area to inform public health and policy., Method: We addressed these issues by examining whether third-person self-talk, a simple technique that promotes emotion regulation, could nudge people into reasoning about Ebola more rationally. In all, 1,257 people from across the United States were asked to write about their feelings about Ebola using their name or I (i.e. third-person self-talk vs. first-person self-talk) as concerns about Ebola swelled (24 October 2014-26 October 2014)., Results: Third-person self-talk led participants who scored high on Ebola worry at baseline to generate more fact-based reasons not to worry about Ebola, which predicted reductions in their Ebola worry and risk perception. These findings held when controlling for several theoretically relevant covariates, highlighting their robustness., Conclusion: These results demonstrate how a simple linguistic technique can enhance rational thinking and quell worry about a pressing public health threat., (© 2017 The International Association of Applied Psychology.)
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
42. Being present: Focusing on the present predicts improvements in life satisfaction but not happiness.
- Author
-
Felsman P, Verduyn P, Ayduk O, and Kross E
- Subjects
- Female, Humans, Male, Rumination, Cognitive, Time Factors, Young Adult, Happiness, Mindfulness, Personal Satisfaction
- Abstract
Mindfulness theorists suggest that people spend most of their time focusing on the past or future rather than the present. Despite the prevalence of this assumption, no research that we are aware of has evaluated whether it is true or what the implications of focusing on the present are for subjective well-being. We addressed this issue by using experience sampling to examine how frequently people focus on the present throughout the day over the course of a week and whether focusing on the present predicts improvements in the 2 components of subjective well-being over time-how people feel and how satisfied they are with their lives. Results indicated that participants were present-focused the majority of the time (66%). Moreover, focusing on the present predicted improvements in life satisfaction (but not happiness) over time by reducing negative rumination. These findings advance our understanding of how temporal orientation and well-being relate. (PsycINFO Database Record, ((c) 2017 APA, all rights reserved).)
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
43. The "Batman Effect": Improving Perseverance in Young Children.
- Author
-
White RE, Prager EO, Schaefer C, Kross E, Duckworth AL, and Carlson SM
- Subjects
- Age Factors, Child, Child, Preschool, Female, Humans, Male, Attention physiology, Child Behavior physiology, Child Development physiology, Executive Function physiology, Theory of Mind physiology
- Abstract
This study investigated the benefits of self-distancing (i.e., taking an outsider's view of one's own situation) on young children's perseverance. Four- and 6-year-old children (N = 180) were asked to complete a repetitive task for 10 min while having the option to take breaks by playing an extremely attractive video game. Six-year-olds persevered longer than 4-year-olds. Nonetheless, across both ages, children who impersonated an exemplar other-in this case a character, such as Batman-spent the most time working, followed by children who took a third-person perspective on the self, or finally, a first-person perspective. Alternative explanations, implications, and future research directions are discussed., (© 2016 The Authors. Child Development © 2016 Society for Research in Child Development, Inc.)
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
44. Third-person self-talk facilitates emotion regulation without engaging cognitive control: Converging evidence from ERP and fMRI.
- Author
-
Moser JS, Dougherty A, Mattson WI, Katz B, Moran TP, Guevarra D, Shablack H, Ayduk O, Jonides J, Berman MG, and Kross E
- Subjects
- Female, Humans, Male, Self-Control, Brain Mapping, Cognition, Emotions, Evoked Potentials, Magnetic Resonance Imaging methods
- Abstract
Does silently talking to yourself in the third-person constitute a relatively effortless form of self control? We hypothesized that it does under the premise that third-person self-talk leads people to think about the self similar to how they think about others, which provides them with the psychological distance needed to facilitate self control. We tested this prediction by asking participants to reflect on feelings elicited by viewing aversive images (Study 1) and recalling negative autobiographical memories (Study 2) using either "I" or their name while measuring neural activity via ERPs (Study 1) and fMRI (Study 2). Study 1 demonstrated that third-person self-talk reduced an ERP marker of self-referential emotional reactivity (i.e., late positive potential) within the first second of viewing aversive images without enhancing an ERP marker of cognitive control (i.e., stimulus preceding negativity). Conceptually replicating these results, Study 2 demonstrated that third-person self-talk was linked with reduced levels of activation in an a priori defined fMRI marker of self-referential processing (i.e., medial prefrontal cortex) when participants reflected on negative memories without eliciting increased levels of activity in a priori defined fMRI markers of cognitive control. Together, these results suggest that third-person self-talk may constitute a relatively effortless form of self-control.
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
45. Is Psychology Headed in the Right Direction?
- Author
-
Kross E
- Subjects
- Communication, Humans, Psychology trends, Public Opinion, Research, Research Design trends
- Abstract
Is psychology headed in the right direction? In this essay, I share my views on the answer to this question. I begin by describing how recent advances in technology, heightened levels of interdisciplinary collaboration, a renewed emphasis on considering the broader implications of basic psychological research, and field-wide efforts to encourage optimal research practices have combined to tilt psychology's trajectory upward. I then offer three suggestions for how to maintain the field's upward slanting course: (a) collaborate more to build cumulative knowledge, (b) improve the way we communicate contextual factors in psychological research, and (c) examine the psychological effects of technology. I conclude by offering a single piece of advice for new researchers and a few closing comments.
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
46. Self-distancing improves interpersonal perceptions and behavior by decreasing medial prefrontal cortex activity during the provision of criticism.
- Author
-
Leitner JB, Ayduk O, Mendoza-Denton R, Magerman A, Amey R, Kross E, and Forbes CE
- Subjects
- Adult, Electroencephalography, Female, Humans, Magnetic Resonance Imaging methods, Male, Mentors psychology, Race Relations, Self Concept, Signal Processing, Computer-Assisted, Social Perception, Young Adult, Black People psychology, Interpersonal Relations, Prefrontal Cortex physiology, Prejudice, Psychological Distance, Social Behavior, White People psychology
- Abstract
Previous research suggests that people show increased self-referential processing when they provide criticism to others, and that this self-referential processing can have negative effects on interpersonal perceptions and behavior. The current research hypothesized that adopting a self-distanced perspective (i.e. thinking about a situation from a non-first person point of view), as compared with a typical self-immersed perspective (i.e. thinking about a situation from a first-person point of view), would reduce self-referential processing during the provision of criticism, and in turn improve interpersonal perceptions and behavior. We tested this hypothesis in an interracial context since research suggests that self-referential processing plays a role in damaging interracial relations. White participants prepared for mentorship from a self-immersed or self-distanced perspective. They then conveyed negative and positive evaluations to a Black mentee while electroencephalogram (EEG) was recorded. Source analysis revealed that priming a self-distanced (vs self-immersed) perspective predicted decreased activity in regions linked to self-referential processing (medial prefrontal cortex; MPFC) when providing negative evaluations. This decreased MPFC activity during negative evaluations, in turn, predicted verbal feedback that was perceived to be more positive, warm and helpful. Results suggest that self-distancing can improve interpersonal perceptions and behavior by decreasing self-referential processing during the provision of criticism., (© The Author (2016). Published by Oxford University Press. For Permissions, please email: journals.permissions@oup.com.)
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
47. Frontal-Brainstem Pathways Mediating Placebo Effects on Social Rejection.
- Author
-
Koban L, Kross E, Woo CW, Ruzic L, and Wager TD
- Subjects
- Adolescent, Adult, Affect, Brain Stem drug effects, Female, Frontal Lobe drug effects, Humans, Male, Nerve Net diagnostic imaging, Nerve Net physiology, Neural Pathways drug effects, Neural Pathways physiology, Pain Perception drug effects, Placebo Effect, Placebos administration & dosage, Young Adult, Analgesia psychology, Brain Stem physiology, Frontal Lobe physiology, Pain Perception physiology, Psychological Distance, Suggestion
- Abstract
Placebo treatments can strongly affect clinical outcomes, but research on how they shape other life experiences and emotional well-being is in its infancy. We used fMRI in humans to examine placebo effects on a particularly impactful life experience, social pain elicited by a recent romantic rejection. We compared these effects with placebo effects on physical (heat) pain, which are thought to depend on pathways connecting prefrontal cortex and periaqueductal gray (PAG). Placebo treatment, compared with control, reduced both social and physical pain, and increased activity in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) in both modalities. Placebo further altered the relationship between affect and both dlPFC and PAG activity during social pain, and effects on behavior were mediated by a pathway connecting dlPFC to the PAG, building on recent work implicating opioidergic PAG activity in the regulation of social pain. These findings suggest that placebo treatments reduce emotional distress by altering affective representations in frontal-brainstem systems. SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Placebo effects are improvements due to expectations and the socio-medical context in which treatment takes place. Whereas they have been extensively studied in the context of somatic conditions such as pain, much less is known of how treatment expectations shape the emotional experience of other important stressors and life events. Here, we use brain imaging to show that placebo treatment reduces the painful feelings associated with a recent romantic rejection by recruiting a prefrontal-brainstem network and by shifting the relationship between brain activity and affect. Our findings suggest that this brain network may be important for nonspecific treatment effects across a wide range of therapeutic approaches and mental health conditions., (Copyright © 2017 the authors 0270-6474/17/373621-11$15.00/0.)
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
48. How "you" makes meaning.
- Author
-
Orvell A, Kross E, and Gelman SA
- Subjects
- Behavior, Humans, Semantics, Emotions, Language, Speech
- Abstract
"You" is one of the most common words in the English language. Although it typically refers to the person addressed ("How are you?"), "you" is also used to make timeless statements about people in general ("You win some, you lose some."). Here, we demonstrate that this ubiquitous but understudied linguistic device, known as "generic-you," has important implications for how people derive meaning from experience. Across six experiments, we found that generic-you is used to express norms in both ordinary and emotional contexts and that producing generic-you when reflecting on negative experiences allows people to "normalize" their experience by extending it beyond the self. In this way, a simple linguistic device serves a powerful meaning-making function., (Copyright © 2017, American Association for the Advancement of Science.)
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
49. Poster 104 Early Mobility Considerations and Psychological Outcomes in Critically Ill Patients.
- Author
-
Wittry SA and Kross E
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
50. When perceptions defy reality: The relationships between depression and actual and perceived Facebook social support.
- Author
-
Park J, Lee DS, Shablack H, Verduyn P, Deldin P, Ybarra O, Jonides J, and Kross E
- Subjects
- Adolescent, Adult, Depressive Disorder diagnosis, Female, Humans, Male, Self Disclosure, Statistics as Topic, Students psychology, Young Adult, Depressive Disorder psychology, Depressive Disorder therapy, Reality Testing, Social Media, Social Perception, Social Support
- Abstract
Background: Although the relationship between depression and "offline" social support is well established, numerous questions surround the relationship between "online" social support and depression. We explored this issue by examining the social support dynamics that characterize the way individuals with varying levels of depression (Study 1) and SCID-diagnosed clinically depressed and non-depressed individuals (Study 2) interact with Facebook, the world's largest online social network., Method: Using a novel methodology, we examined how disclosing positive or negative information on Facebook influences the amount of social support depressed individuals (a) actually receive (based on actual social support transactions recorded on Facebook walls) and (b) think they receive (based on subjective assessments) from their Facebook network., Results: Contrary to prior research indicating that depression correlates with less actual social support from "offline" networks, across both studies depression was positively correlated with social support from Facebook networks when participants disclosed negative information (p=.02 in Study 1 and p=.06 in Study 2). Yet, depression was negatively correlated with how much social support participants thought they received from their Facebook networks (p=.005 in Study 1 and p=.001 in Study 2)., Limitations: The sample size was relatively small in Study 2, reflecting difficulties of recruiting individuals with Major Depressive Disorder., Conclusions: These results demonstrate that an asymmetry characterizes the relationship between depression and different types of Facebook social support and further identify perceptions of Facebook social support as a potential intervention target. (243 words; 250 max)., (Copyright © 2016 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.)
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.