156 results on '"Ideational Apraxia"'
Search Results
2. A Brief History of Upper Limb Ideomotor Apraxia
- Author
-
Heilman, Kenneth M., Barr, William B., book editor, and Bieliauskas, Linas A., book editor
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
3. The impact of visual and motor skills on ideational apraxia and transcortical sensory aphasia.
- Author
-
Afshangian, Fazlallah, Wellington, Jack, Pashmoforoosh, Radnoosh, Farzadfard, Mohammad Taghi, Noori, Narges Khatoon, Jaberi, Abbas Rahimi, Ostovan, Vahid Reza, Soltani, Ahmad, Safari, Hosein, Abolhasani Foroughi, Amin, Resid Onen, Mehmet, Montemurro, Nicola, Chaurasia, Bipin, Akgul, Erol, Freddi, Tomas, Ermis, Abdulkadir, Amirifard, Hamed, Habibi, Saiyed Amir Hasan, Manzarinezad, Motahereh, and Bozkurt, Ismail
- Abstract
Abstract Background Purpose Method Results Conclusion Patients with extensive left hemisphere damage frequently have ideational apraxia (IA) and transcortical sensory aphasia (TSA). Difficulty with action coordination, phonological processing, and complex motor planning may not be indicative of higher-order motor programming or higher-order complex formation. We report on the effects of IA and TSA on the visual and motor skill of stroke patients.The study aims to address the question of whether IA and TSA in bilingual individuals are the results of an error of motor function alone or due to a combined motor plus and cognitive dysfunction effect.Twelve bilingual patients (seven males, and five females) were diagnosed with IA and TSA, and are divided into two groups of six patients. Then, 12 healthy bilingual controls were evaluated for comparing with both groups. Bilingual aphasia testing (BAT) and appropriate behavioral evaluation were used to assess motor skills, including coordination, visual-motor testing, and phonological processing.Findings (pointing skills) show that the performance of the L1 and L2 languages are consistently significant (
p < 0.001) in healthy individuals compared to the IA and TSA groups. Command skills for L1 and L2 languages were significantly higher in healthy individuals compared to IA and TSA controls (p < 0.001). Further, the orthographic skills of IA and TSA vs controls in both groups were significantly reduced (p < 0.01). Visual skills in the L1 language were significantly improved (p < 0.05) in IA and TSA patients compared to healthy controls after 2 months. Unlike orthographic skills which were improved in IA and TSA patients, languages in bilingual patients did not simultaneously improve.Dyspraxia is a condition that affects both motor and visual cognitive functions, and patients who have it often have less referred motor skills. The current dataset shows that accurate visual cognition requires both cognitive-linguistic and sensory-motor processes. Motor issues should be highlighted, and skills and functionality should be reinforced along with the significance of treatment between IA and TSA corresponding to age and education. This can be a good indicator for treating semantic disorders. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
4. Left Hemisphere Syndromes: Apraxias
- Author
-
Hoffmann, Michael and Hoffmann, Michael
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
5. Qualitative analysis for the "ideational apraxia" score from the Alzheimer's disease assessment scale cognitive subscale.
- Author
-
Ito, Sayuri, Sato, Shigeru, Saito, Naohiro, Ohnuma, Ayumu, Tobita, Muneshige, Kimpara, Teiko, Iseki, Chifumi, and Suzuki, Kyoko
- Subjects
- *
ALZHEIMER'S disease , *MILD cognitive impairment , *APRAXIA , *MINI-Mental State Examination , *TASK analysis - Abstract
Background: Various types of cognitive dysfunction could make it difficult to use multiple objects properly in Alzheimer's disease (AD). Aim: To observe the qualitative feature of action deficits during a widely used praxis task and to clarify cognitive dysfunctions underlying the deficits. Methods: We examined 80 patients with mild cognitive impairment (MCI), 84 patients with AD (69 mild and 15 moderate AD) and 40 healthy controls (HC) using the Alzheimer's Disease Assessment Scale cognitive subscale—Japanese version (ADAS‐J cog) and the Mini‐Mental State Examination (MMSE). Errors in the "praxis task" of ADAS‐J cog were classified into 4 types: apraxic, attentional, visuospatial and writing errors based on their qualitative features. The ratio of each error type was calculated and compared with each other. Results: Participants with mild and moderate AD made significantly more errors in the praxis task compared with those in the HC and MCI groups. Qualitative analysis revealed that attentional errors occur more frequently in the MCI group than those in the HC group. For apraxic errors, there were significant differences between the MCI group and the mild/moderate AD groups. The more severe their general cognitive dysfunctions, the greater the variety of errors patients exhibit in the praxis task. Conclusion: The results indicate that a main factor affecting the praxis task in MCI patients was attentional deficits, while various cognitive dysfunctions were related to action deficits in patients with AD. Our qualitative analysis for the praxis task revealed difference in action deficits in MCI and AD patients. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
6. Limb apraxia and the 'affordance competition hypothesis'
- Author
-
Elisabeth eRounis and Glyn eHumphreys
- Subjects
Ideomotor apraxia ,Ideational Apraxia ,affordance competition ,route to action model ,limb apraxia ,Neurosciences. Biological psychiatry. Neuropsychiatry ,RC321-571 - Abstract
Limb apraxia, a disorder of higher order motor control, has long been a challenge for clinical assessment and understanding (Leiguarda and Marsden 2000).The deficits originally described in limb apraxia (Liepmann 1908) have been classified by the nature of the errors made by the patients leading to, namely, ideational and ideomotor apraxia. The dual stream hypothesis (Goodale and Milner 1992) has been used to explain these categories: ideational apraxia is thought to relate to a deficit in the concept of a movement (coded in the ventral stream), whereas ideomotor apraxia, is thought to arise from problems in the accurate implementation of movements within the dorsal stream. One of the limitations on understanding apraxia is the failure by the clinical literature to draw on knowledge of the factors determining actions in the environment. Here we emphasize the role of affordance. There is much recent work indicating that our responses to stimuli are strongly influenced by the actions that the objects ‘afford’, based on their physical properties and the intentions of the actor (e.g, Ellis & Tucker, 1998; Humphreys et al., 2010). The concept of affordance, originally suggested by Gibson (1979) has been incorporated in a recent model of interactive behaviour that draws from findings in non-human primates, namely the ‘affordance competition hypothesis’ (Cisek 2007). This postulates that interactive behaviour arises by a process of competition between possible actions elicited by the environment. In this paper we argue that ‘affordance competition’ may play a role in apraxia. We review evidence that at least some aspects of apraxia may reflect an abnormal sensitivity to competition when multiple affordances are present (Riddoch et al., 1998) and/or a poor ability to exert cognitive control over this competition when it occurs. This framework suggests a new way of conceptualising deficits in apraxia which invites further investigations in the field.
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
7. Intervention Focused on Qualitative Error in A Patient with Ideational Apraxia ; A Single Case Experimental Design Study
- Author
-
Rumi Tanemura and Daisuke Shimizu
- Subjects
medicine.medical_specialty ,Physical medicine and rehabilitation ,Ideational apraxia ,Intervention (counseling) ,medicine ,Single-subject design ,medicine.disease ,Psychology - Published
- 2019
8. Qualitative analysis for the 'ideational apraxia' score from the Alzheimer's disease assessment scale cognitive subscale
- Author
-
Teiko Kimpara, Kyoko Suzuki, Muneshige Tobita, Sayuri Ito, Ayumu Ohnuma, Shigeru Sato, Chifumi Iseki, and Naohiro Saito
- Subjects
Qualitative analysis ,Neurology ,Adas cog ,Scale (ratio) ,business.industry ,Ideational apraxia ,medicine ,Cognition ,Neurology (clinical) ,Disease assessment ,medicine.disease ,business ,Clinical psychology - Published
- 2019
9. Progressive transcortical sensory aphasia and progressive ideational apraxia owing to temporoparietal cortical atrophy.
- Author
-
Michitaka Funayama, Asuka Nakajima, Funayama, Michitaka, and Nakajima, Asuka
- Subjects
- *
TRANSCORTICAL sensory aphasia , *APRAXIA , *ATROPHY , *SYNTAX (Grammar) , *REPETITION (Learning process) , *THERAPEUTICS - Abstract
Background: In contrast to frontotemporal lobar degeneration, atrophy of the focal posterior lateral cortex has not been thoroughly studied. Three clinical types of focal cortical atrophy have been described: 1) logopenic variant of primary progressive aphasia, which presents with impaired repetition despite normal articulation; 2) posterior cortical atrophy, which presents with prominent visuospatial deficits; and 3) primary progressive apraxia. All three clinical types are characterized by specific patterns of hypometabolism/hypoperfusion: the left posterior perisylvian area in the logopenic variant of primary progressive aphasia, bilateral parietooccipital areas in posterior cortical atrophy, and the parietal cortex in primary progressive apraxia. However, not every patient clearly fits into one of these categories.Case Presentation: Here we describe two patients with atypical focal cortical presentations. They presented with a history of a few years of progressive transcortical sensory aphasia characterized by fluent output with normal grammar and syntax, normal repetition, sentence comprehension deficits, and anomia without loss of word meaning. They also presented with progressive apraxia that began at the initial stages. Some forms of posterior symptoms including acalculia, agraphia, and visuospatial deficits were also observed. Hypoperfusion was noted mainly in the left temporoparietal region, which is slightly posterior to the perisylvian area.Conclusions: Although our cases lack in CSF findings and PIB scan, these two cases and previous reports might suggest the existence of a subgroup of patients presenting with transcortical sensory aphasia, apraxia, and posterior symptoms (acalculia, agraphia, and visuospatial deficits) in the setting of Alzheimer's disease. This subgroup may reflect the spectrum of clinical manifestations between logopenic variant of primary progressive aphasia and posterior cortical atrophy. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
10. Hugo Liepmann, Parkinson's disease and upper limb apraxia
- Author
-
Kenneth M. Heilman
- Subjects
medicine.medical_specialty ,Parkinson's disease ,genetic structures ,Apraxias ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Signs and symptoms ,Disease ,behavioral disciplines and activities ,Apraxia ,050105 experimental psychology ,Upper Extremity ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,Physical medicine and rehabilitation ,Rating scale ,medicine ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,05 social sciences ,Parkinson Disease ,Ideomotor apraxia ,medicine.disease ,nervous system diseases ,body regions ,Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology ,medicine.anatomical_structure ,Ideational apraxia ,Upper limb ,Psychology ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery - Abstract
It has now been 100 years since Hugo Liepmann, in his classic 1920 paper described limb kinetic, ideomotor, and ideational apraxia. There are now several rating scales used to assess and grade the signs and symptoms associated with Parkinson’s disease; however, none of these assesses patients for the presence of these disabling forms of upper limb apraxia. This paper, reviews the four types of apraxia that can be associated with Parkinson’s disease, how they can be tested, the disabilities associated with these disorders and possible treatments.
- Published
- 2020
11. Action programming disorders associated with Parkinson's disease
- Author
-
Kenneth M. Heilman
- Subjects
medicine.medical_specialty ,Weakness ,Parkinson's disease ,genetic structures ,Sensory loss ,Ideomotor apraxia ,medicine.disease ,behavioral disciplines and activities ,Apraxia ,nervous system diseases ,body regions ,Comprehension ,Physical medicine and rehabilitation ,Action (philosophy) ,Ideational apraxia ,medicine ,medicine.symptom ,Psychology - Abstract
Apraxia is a disorder of the skilled movements that are needed successfully perform transitive or intransitive movements, when this disorder is not caused by failures of comprehension, weakness, sensory loss, or involuntary movements. The following forms of upper limb apraxia have been reported to be associated with Parkinson's disease: (1) limb-kinetic apraxia, a loss of the ability to make precise, independent but coordinated finger-hand movements; (2) ideomotor apraxia, a failure to make the correct joint movements, to coordinate these movements with the correct timing, speed, and force; and (3) ideational apraxia, the incorrect sequencing of a series of actions required to complete a goal. This chapter summarizes the means of testing for these different forms of apraxia, the types of errors made by patients with these forms of apraxia, and the pathophysiology and treatment of these disorders.
- Published
- 2020
12. Ideational Apraxia
- Author
-
Kreutzer, Jeffrey S., editor, DeLuca, John, editor, and Caplan, Bruce, editor
- Published
- 2011
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
13. Everyday Action Impairment in Parkinson's Disease Dementia.
- Author
-
Giovannetti, Tania, Britnell, Priscilla, Brennan, Laura, Siderowf, Andrew, Grossman, Murray, Libon, David J., Bettcher, Brianne M., Rouzard, Francesca, Eppig, Joel, and Seidel, Gregory A.
- Subjects
- *
PARKINSON'S disease , *DEMENTIA , *COGNITION disorders , *COMPARATIVE studies , *NEUROPSYCHOLOGY , *PERFORMANCE evaluation , *COGNITIVE ability - Abstract
This study examined everyday action impairment in participants with Parkinson's disease dementia (PDD) by comparison with participants with Parkinson's disease-no dementia (PD) or Alzheimer's disease (AD) and in reference to a neuropsychological model. Participants with PDD (n = 20), PD (n = 20), or AD (n = 20) were administered performance-based measures of everyday functioning that allowed for the quantification of overall performance and error types. Also, caregiver ratings of functional independence were obtained. On performance-based tests, the PDD group exhibited greater functional impairment than the PD group but comparable overall impairment relative to the AD group. Error patterns did not differ between PDD and PD participants but the PDD group demonstrated a higher proportion of commission errors and lower proportion of omission errors relative to the AD group. Hierarchical regression analyses showed omission errors were significantly predicted by neuropsychological measures of episodic memory, whereas commission errors were predicted by both measures of general dementia severity (MMSE) and executive control. Everyday action impairment in PDD differs quantitatively from PD but qualitatively from AD and may be characterized by a relatively high proportion of commission errors—an error type associated with executive control deficits. (JINS, 2012, 18, 1–12) [ABSTRACT FROM PUBLISHER]
- Published
- 2012
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
14. The cognitive rehabilitation of limb apraxia in patients with stroke.
- Author
-
Cantagallo, Anna, Maini, Manuela, and Rumiati, RaffaellaIda
- Subjects
- *
PSYCHOMOTOR disorders , *APRAXIA , *ETIOLOGY of diseases , *CEREBRAL dominance , *NEUROBEHAVIORAL disorders - Abstract
Apraxia is a higher level motor deficit that occurs when processing a goal-directed action. The apraxic deficit can manifest itself in absence of sensory input deficits or motor output deficits, neglect, frontal inertia or dementia. According to a clinical classification still largely in use, there are two main forms of limb apraxia: ideomotor (IMA) and ideational (IA), observed when a patient is required to imitate a gesture or use an object, respectively. In the present review, we examined only the cognitive treatments of both types of limb apraxia of a vascular aetiology. Despite the high prevalence of limb apraxia caused by left brain damage, and the fact that apraxia has been known for over a century, the literature regarding its rehabilitation is still very limited. This is partly due to the nature of the recovery from the deficit, and in part to the automatic-voluntary dissociation. Here we review those treatments that have proved most successful in helping patients to recover from limb apraxia. [ABSTRACT FROM PUBLISHER]
- Published
- 2012
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
15. Target-related distractors disrupt object selection in everyday action: Evidence from participants with dementia.
- Author
-
Giovannetti, Tania, Bettcher, Brianne Magouirk, Brennan, Laura, Libon, David J., Wambach, Denene, and Seter, Colette
- Subjects
- *
DEMENTIA patients , *ALZHEIMER'S patients , *APRAXIA , *HUMAN activity recognition , *BRAIN degeneration , *PSYCHOMOTOR disorders - Abstract
This study evaluated the impact of distractor objects and their similarity to target objects on everyday task performance in dementia. Twenty participants with dementia due to Alzheimer's disease ( n = 12) or subcortical vascular disease ( n = 8) were videotaped while they performed 3 discrete tasks: (1) make a cup of coffee, (2) wrap a gift, and (3) pack a lunch under two conditions that were counterbalanced across participants. The conditions differed in terms of the type of distractor objects included in the workspace: (1) Target-Related Distractor Condition - distractor objects were functionally and visually similar to target objects (e.g., salt for sugar) (2) Unrelated Distractor Condition - distractors were neither visually nor functionally similar to targets (e.g., glue for sugar). Participants touched ( t = 4.19; p < .01) and used ( z = 3.00; p < .01) significantly more distractors, made more distractor errors (i.e., substitutions; t = 2.93; p < .01), and took longer to complete tasks ( t = 2.27; p < .05) in the Target-Related Distractor condition. The percent of steps accomplished and non-distractor errors did not differ across conditions ( t < 1.26; p > .05 for both). In summary, distractors that were similar to targets elicited significant interference effects circumscribed to object selection. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2010
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
16. Semantic memory in object use
- Author
-
Silveri, Maria Caterina and Ciccarelli, Nicoletta
- Subjects
- *
SEMANTICS , *MEMORY disorders , *DEMENTIA , *HERPES simplex virus , *CONCEPTUALISM , *FORM perception , *APRAXIA , *PATIENTS - Abstract
Abstract: We studied five patients with semantic memory disorders, four with semantic dementia and one with herpes simplex virus encephalitis, to investigate the involvement of semantic conceptual knowledge in object use. Comparisons between patients who had semantic deficits of different severity, as well as the follow-up, showed that the ability to use objects was largely preserved when the deficit was mild but progressively decayed as the deficit became more severe. Naming was generally more impaired than object use. Production tasks (pantomime execution and actual object use) and comprehension tasks (pantomime recognition and action recognition) as well as functional knowledge about objects were impaired when the semantic deficit was severe. Semantic and unrelated errors were produced during object use, but actions were always fluent and patients performed normally on a novel tools task in which the semantic demand was minimal. Patients with severe semantic deficits scored borderline on ideational apraxia tasks. Our data indicate that functional semantic knowledge is crucial for using objects in a conventional way and suggest that non-semantic factors, mainly non-declarative components of memory, might compensate to some extent for semantic disorders and guarantee some residual ability to use very common objects independently of semantic knowledge. [Copyright &y& Elsevier]
- Published
- 2009
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
17. The deficit for the word-class “verb” in corticobasal degeneration: Linguistic expression of the movement disorder?
- Author
-
Silveri, Maria Caterina and Ciccarelli, Nicoletta
- Subjects
- *
APRAXIA , *PSYCHOMOTOR disorders , *NEUROLOGICAL disorders , *EUGENICS - Abstract
Abstract: We describe five patients with corticobasal degeneration who had apraxia with an ideational component and reduced action/verb naming ability. Patients also had difficulty in a series of tasks devised to explore the conceptual representation of actions associated with manipulable objects, such as action recognition, action miming and pantomime recognition; however, their ability to name manipulable objects was comparatively preserved. According to the current interpretation of ideational apraxia [De Renzi, E., & Lucchelli, F. (1988). Ideational apraxia. Brain, 111, 1173–1185] we considered the patients’ apraxic disorder as the motor expression of decay of the action representation and we hypothesized that this may also have contributed to the action-naming deficit. The results are discussed within a “multimodal model” of semantic memory in which the concept of action is seen as the product of the integration between sensorial and motor attributes. We suggest that corticobasal degeneration might offer a unique opportunity to validate this model because it is typically characterized by a frontoparietal damage [Gibb, W. R., Luthert, P. J., & Marsden, C. D. (1989). Corticobasal degeneration. Brain, 1, 1171–1192] that prevents integration of sensory and motor information. We conclude that the selective impairment of action/verb should also be studied from the point of view of a movement disorder and not only in terms of a lexical-semantic deficit. [Copyright &y& Elsevier]
- Published
- 2007
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
18. Ideational action impairments in Alzheimer’s disease
- Author
-
Chainay, H., Louarn, C., and Humphreys, G.W.
- Subjects
- *
ALZHEIMER'S disease , *BRAIN , *COGNITION , *COGNITIVE ability - Abstract
Abstract: We report data from a group of patients with mild Alzheimer’s disease on a range of tasks requiring either stored semantic knowledge about objects (e.g., naming object use) or the execution of action to objects (e.g., miming and using objects). We found that the patients were impaired at miming in response to objects, even when they could describe the object’s function. On the other hand, copying gestures was not impaired relative to naming gestures, indicating that an ideomotor deficit in action execution, per se, was unlikely to explain the impairments in object use. We suggest instead that the patients had an impairment in stored motor programmes for action, over and above their deficits in semantic knowledge. Despite this, the patients were better at using than at miming to objects, consistent with the view that proprioceptive input (when using objects) can directly constrain selection of the appropriate motor programme for action. [Copyright &y& Elsevier]
- Published
- 2006
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
19. Ideomotor and Ideational Apraxia in Corticobasal Degeneration: A Case Study.
- Author
-
Chainay, Hanna and Humphreys, Glyn W.
- Subjects
- *
CASE studies , *APRAXIA , *AGNOSIA , *PERCEPTUAL disorders , *PSYCHOMOTOR disorders - Abstract
Corticobasal degeneration (CBD) is a progressive disorder that can be characterised by asymmetrical akinetic rigidity, involuntary movements, cortical sensory loss, alien limb syndrome and asymmetrical apraxia (Gibb et al ., 1989; Rinnie et al ., 1994). Diagnosis of praxic disabilities is thought to be essential for distinguishing CBD, in its early stage, from other akinetic-rigid syndromes. However, the nature of apraxia in CBD, and the relations between ideomotor and ideational apraxia, are not well understood. For example, if there is an ideational deficit in a given patient, does this deficit occur independently of any ideomotor disorder, or are the two impairments linked in some manner? In the present paper we report a case study of a patient with apraxia due to CBD. We examine whether the disorder is confined to production tasks, or whether there is also a related deficit in recognising the correct actions performed with objects (an ideational deficit). We also evaluate whether a disorder found for action with single objects dissociates from the ability to link multiple actions into more complex, everyday tasks. The performance of our patient showed an impairment in both action production and action recognition system, suggesting a component of ideational as well as ideomotor apraxia in CBD. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2003
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
20. Apraxia of Tool Use: An Autopsy Case of Biparietal Infarction.
- Author
-
Fukutake, Toshio
- Subjects
- *
APRAXIA , *PARIETAL lobe , *CEREBRAL infarction , *AUTOPSY , *MAGNETIC resonance imaging - Abstract
Although disorders in the use of single objects have been reported, there have been few detailed analyses. We describe the autopsy case of a 65-year-old, right-handed male patient with severe impairment of actual tool use which was caused by biparietal infarctions. He persistently and stably showed a severely defective use of actual objects, single or multiple, and relatively well-preserved pantomimes of object use and intransitive gestures. He did not have aphasia or dementia, and his ability for tool naming and function description was completely preserved. The author identified similar patterns of errors about a tool-action(-target) relationship both in single-object use and multiple-object use. Lesion analysis showed cortical infarcts mainly located in the bilateral inferior parietal lobules, extending into the temporal lobe on the right side.Copyright © 2003 S. Karger AG, Basel [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2003
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
21. Order and Disorder in Everyday Action: the Roles of Contention Scheduling and Supervisory Attention.
- Author
-
Cooper, Richard
- Abstract
This paper describes the contention scheduling/supervisory attentional system approach to action selection and uses this account to structure a survey of current theories of the control of action. The focus is on how such theories account for the types of error produced by some patients with frontal and/or left temporoparietal damage when attempting everyday tasks. Four issues, concerning both the theories and their accounts of everyday action breakdown, emerge: first, whether multiple control systems, each capable of controlling action in different situations, exist; second, whether different forms of damage at the neural level result in conceptually distinct disorders; third, whether semantic/conceptual knowledge of objects and actions can be dissociated from control mechanisms, and if so what computational principles govern sequential control; and fourth, whether disorders of everyday action should be attributed to a loss of semantic/conceptual knowledge, a malfunction of control, or some combination of the two. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2002
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
22. Apraxia of Single Tool Use.
- Author
-
Hayakawa, Yuko, Yamadori, Atsushi, Fujii, Toshikatsu, Suzuki, Kyoko, and Tobita, Mari
- Subjects
- *
APRAXIA , *CEREBRAL infarction , *AGNOSIA , *PSYCHOMOTOR disorders , *CEREBROVASCULAR disease - Abstract
We report a 72-year-old right-handed man who showed an ‘apraxia of tool use’ after a cerebral infarct in the territory of the left middle cerebral artery. His apraxia of tool use was characterized by a clear dissociation between the inability to use a single tool and the ability to use plural tools. Most of the errors occurred in selecting an appropriate target where a tool is expected to be applied. Detailed examinations confirmed that his conceptual knowledge of tool use was well preserved. Furthermore, when a target of a tool was provided as a cue, he used a single tool correctly. These results suggest that his inability to use a single tool originated from his inability to evoke a target image from an actual tool.Copyright © 2000 S. Karger AG, Basel [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2000
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
23. Symposium 5
- Author
-
von Steinbüchel, Nicole, Steffen, Alexander, Wittmann, Marc, von Steinbüchel, Nicole, editor, Steffen, Alexander, editor, and Wittmann, Marc, editor
- Published
- 1997
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
24. Apraxia: another view
- Author
-
Nick Miller
- Subjects
Neurologic Examination ,Apraxias ,05 social sciences ,General Medicine ,Engram ,Limb apraxia ,Neuropsychological Tests ,Ideomotor apraxia ,medicine.disease ,Apraxia ,050105 experimental psychology ,Apperceptive agnosia ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,Aphasia ,Ideational apraxia ,medicine ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Neurology (clinical) ,medicine.symptom ,Neural transmission ,Psychology ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
A previous exposition of apraxia in Practical Neurology 1 offered an account of underlying impairment and accompanying clinical assessment based on a distinction between ideational, ideomotor and limb-kinetic apraxia. This conceptualisation stems from 19th-century models of higher cortical motor, language and visual function, revived by Geschwind2 3 in the 1960s. Liepmann in 19004 had posited a hierarchical model of action control over three discrete levels. At the top, movement formulae (‘visual engrams’ of the action) provided overall targets, which activate innervatory patterns to stimulate the appropriate muscles, coordinated contraction of muscles then led to execution of the action. (1) Ideational apraxia arose when movement formulae were either impaired or disconnected from the innervatory patterns. (2) Ideomotor apraxia resulted from disruption to innervatory patterns. (3) Limb-kinetic apraxia , assumed to be a disruption to the smooth neural transmission of the motor commands, was, even for Liepmann, not a full apraxia. It existed between apraxia and paresis only within a broader view of dysfunction. Liepmann’s model proved valuable in studying apraxia. However, despite surviving in several neurology textbooks, the ideational–ideomotor dichotomy has now been replaced as a way of understanding and classifying apraxias,5–8 similar to the disbandment of Broca’s versus Wernicke’s aphasia, or the dichotomy of associative versus apperceptive agnosia9 (although these distinctions, too, still persist in some areas of clinical practice). A radical rethink was needed, owing to issues with the underlying theoretical model of apraxia and difficulties in distinguishing between ideational and ideomotor clinically. Goldenberg7 made the plea to ‘relegate the dichotomy of ideational …
- Published
- 2017
25. An autopsy case of frontotemporal lobar degeneration with the appearance of fused in sarcoma inclusions (basophilic inclusion body disease) clinically presenting corticobasal syndrome
- Author
-
Hiroshi Shimizu, Hiroyoshi Suzuki, Yasushi Suzuki, Arifumi Matsumoto, Reiko Fukatsu, and Kinya Hisanaga
- Subjects
0301 basic medicine ,Pathology ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Apraxia ,Pathology and Forensic Medicine ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,Atrophy ,mental disorders ,medicine ,Corticobasal degeneration ,Primary Lateral Sclerosis ,business.industry ,nutritional and metabolic diseases ,General Medicine ,Frontotemporal lobar degeneration ,Ideomotor apraxia ,medicine.disease ,nervous system diseases ,030104 developmental biology ,Gliosis ,Ideational apraxia ,Neurology (clinical) ,medicine.symptom ,business ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery - Abstract
We describe an autopsy case of basophilic inclusion body disease (BIBD), a subtype of frontotemporal lobar degeneration (FTLD) with the appearance of fused in sarcoma (FUS) inclusions (FTLD-FUS), clinically presenting corticobasal syndrome (CBS). A 54-year-old man initially developed worsening of stuttering and right hand clumsiness. Neurological examinations revealed rigidity in the right upper and lower extremities, buccofacial apraxia, and right-side dominant limb-kinetic and ideomotor apraxia. Neuroimaging showed asymmetric left-dominant brain atrophy and a cerebral blood flow reduction in the ipsilateral frontal region. At 56 years, his apraxia had advanced, and ideational apraxia was observed. Furthermore, the asymmetry in the limb-kinetic and ideomotor apraxia had disappeared, and both conditions had become bilateral. He had a new onset of aphasia. His symptoms progressed and he died 9 years after the initial symptoms. The brain weighed 955 g. Diffuse brain atrophy was most obvious in the bilateral frontotemporal regions. The atrophy of the left superior frontal and precentral gyri and bilateral basal ganglia was remarkable. Histologically, there was a marked loss of neurons with gliosis in the affected areas, where basophilic neuronal cytoplasmic inclusions were observed. The inclusions were immunoreactive for FUS, p62, and TATA-binding protein-associated factor 15 (TAF15), but not for phosphorylated tau, transactive response DNA-binding protein of 43 kDa (TDP-43), neurofilament protein, or Ewing sarcoma (EWS). From these pathological findings, this case was diagnosed as having BIBD as an FTLD-FUS variant. Spinal cord lower motor neurons were spared in number, similar to primary lateral sclerosis. Mutations in FUS were undetectable. Common background pathologies for CBS include corticobasal degeneration, Alzheimer's disease, PSP, FTLD with phosphorylated TDP-43 inclusions (FTLD-TDP), Pick's disease, Lewy body disease and CJD. However, FTLD-FUS (BIBD) has been rarely reported. Our case suggested further pathological heterogeneity in CBS than had previously been reported. It is necessary to consider FTLD-FUS (BIBD) as a background pathology for CBS in the future.
- Published
- 2015
26. Hemichorea in a diabetes mellitus patient following acute ischemic stroke with changes in regional cerebral blood flow
- Author
-
Minoru Oishi, Mariko Fujisawa, Yutaka Suzuki, Katsuhiko Ogawa, Akira Kanno, and Satoshi Kamei
- Subjects
Thalamus ,Ischemia ,Brain Ischemia ,Diabetes Complications ,chemistry.chemical_compound ,Chorea ,Diabetes mellitus ,ECD-SPECT ,medicine ,Edaravone ,Humans ,Aged ,Medicine(all) ,Tomography, Emission-Computed, Single-Photon ,lcsh:R5-920 ,business.industry ,Cerebral infarction ,General Medicine ,regional cerebral blood flow ,medicine.disease ,cerebral infarction ,Stroke ,Cerebral blood flow ,chemistry ,Cerebrovascular Circulation ,Anesthesia ,Ideational apraxia ,diabetes mellitus ,Female ,medicine.symptom ,hemichorea ,business ,lcsh:Medicine (General) - Abstract
It is not unusual to observe hemichorea in patients with diabetes mellitus, with origins attributable to recent ischemia. Our patient was a 66-year-old female with diabetes mellitus who suddenly developed right hemichorea, mild muscle weakness of the right upper extremity, ideational apraxia, and acalculia. Her blood glucose was 600 mg/dL, and HbA1c was 13.3%. After the patient underwent head magnetic resonance imaging (MRI), a new cerebral infarction was observed in the left frontal lobe, and treatment was started with edaravone and cilostazol. At the same time, insulin treatment was also started for hyperglycemia. The acalculia and ideational apraxia improved approximately 1 week after treatment initiated, and the hemichorea also decreased. ECD-SPECT was performed on admission, and it was observed that blood flow was decreased in the left frontal lobe and striatum, but increased in the thalamus; two weeks later on follow-up ECD-SPECT, blood flow had increased slightly in the left forebrain and striatum, while it had decreased slightly in the thalamus. This suggests that the cause of hemichorea was related to ischemia. When the activity of the pallidum is impaired, it is presumed that the inhibitory activity towards the thalamus weakens and the thalamic cells become over-excited, causing chorea.
- Published
- 2015
27. Voluntary control of facial musculature in Parkinson's disease
- Author
-
Geoff Hammond and Michelle Marneweck
- Subjects
Male ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Apraxias ,Movement ,Emotions ,Hypomimia ,Facial Muscles ,Context (language use) ,Severity of Illness Index ,Apraxia ,Developmental psychology ,Facial Action Coding System ,Physical medicine and rehabilitation ,medicine ,Humans ,Emotional expression ,Aged ,Aged, 80 and over ,Facial expression ,Parkinson Disease ,Middle Aged ,medicine.disease ,Facial Expression ,Facial muscles ,medicine.anatomical_structure ,Neurology ,Ideational apraxia ,Female ,Neurology (clinical) ,medicine.symptom ,Psychology - Abstract
Aside from being measured in the context of producing facial expressions of emotion, the ability to voluntarily control a range of facial muscles in Parkinson's disease (PD) has not been systematically measured. We used in three enrollment phases an adaptation of the Upper and Lower Face Apraxia test, a measure of the ability to make voluntary movements of the upper and lower face in PD patients and healthy controls. Errors were scored due to (1) pauses prior to movement initiation, (2) loss of individuation, (3) impoverished movement, (4) no movement at all, or (5) content errors (likened to ideational apraxia errors). The results show impaired voluntary control of facial musculature in most but not all with PD (with large effect sizes) which correlated positively and highly with disease severity. Errors by PD patients were predominantly due to impoverished movement and individuation loss whereas those made by controls were predominantly due to individuation loss. Patients committed more errors than controls due to impoverishment and no movement, with negligible differences between groups in other errors. In summary, similarly to spontaneous and voluntary emotional expressions, voluntary non-emotional facial movements are impoverished in PD; impoverishment of all movement types will likely contribute to the mask-like facial appearance that is seen with disease progression. These findings also illustrate the utility of an adapted Face Apraxia test as a practical and sensitive measure of voluntary facial musculature control in PD. The test can be used to supplement clinical observations and as a research tool.
- Published
- 2014
28. Acute disseminated encephalomyelitis following influenza vaccination: report of a case with callosal disconnection syndrome
- Author
-
Motomi Arai, Ryosuke Nagao, and Daisuke Takagi
- Subjects
Apraxias ,business.industry ,Encephalomyelitis, Acute Disseminated ,Splenium ,Syndrome ,Anatomy ,Kana ,Middle Aged ,Ideomotor apraxia ,medicine.disease ,Corpus callosum ,Influenza Vaccines ,Agraphia ,Ideational apraxia ,Immunology ,Acute disseminated encephalomyelitis ,Disconnection syndrome ,medicine ,Humans ,Female ,Neurology (clinical) ,medicine.symptom ,business - Abstract
We present a case of callosal disconnection syndrome as a rare manifestation of acute disseminated encephalomyelitis (ADEM). A dextral 48-year-old Japanese woman received trivalent inactivated influenza vaccine in mid-November 2011. Twenty days later, she was found to be in a daze. Subsequently, she developed abnormal behavior and gait disturbance, and she was disoriented regarding time and place. Nystagmus and abnormal ocular movements were absent. Upper limb power was normal, whereas her lower limbs were mildly weak. Tendon reflexes were normally evoked without pathological reflexes. There was no sensory impairment. Serum CRP levels were slightly elevated; other routine laboratory tests, thyroid functions, and vitamin B1 levels were within the normal range. Cerebrospinal fluid examination revealed that it was acellular with a protein level of 54 mg/dl and high myelin basic protein level. Fluid-attenuated inversion recovery MR images revealed a large hyperintense lesion in the corpus callosum, but the lower part of the splenium was spared. Flow voids were observed in the pericallosal arteries. She was diagnosed with post-vaccination ADEM and vigorously treated with an intravenous infusion of methylprednisolone (1 g/day for 6 days) and immunoglobulin (1.2 g/kg). Gait disturbance and disorientation rapidly improved; however, tactile anomia, ideomotor apraxia, ideational apraxia, and agraphia of the left hand were present one month after onset. She had no aphasia or alexia.Interestingly, the patient's left unilateral agraphia was more prominent in kana than kanji (an article in Japanese text) for polysyllabic words, whereas she could write kana characters to dictation. Changes in the sequential order of kana characters within a word were observed. These findings were similar to those observed in pure agraphia associated with lesions in the posterior part of the left middle frontal gyrus. Thus, an interhemispheric mechanism is probably involved in the selection and arrangement of kana characters to form words.
- Published
- 2014
29. Distinct contribution of the parietal and temporal cortex to hand configuration and contextual judgements about tools
- Author
-
Etienne Olivier, Michael Andres, and Barbara Pelgrims
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Apraxias ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,Posture ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Context (language use) ,Brain mapping ,Temporal lobe ,Judgment ,Parietal Lobe ,medicine ,Humans ,Semantic memory ,Temporal cortex ,Brain Mapping ,Tool Use Behavior ,medicine.diagnostic_test ,Parietal lobe ,Hand ,medicine.disease ,Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation ,Temporal Lobe ,Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology ,Ideational apraxia ,Psychology ,Functional magnetic resonance imaging ,Psychomotor Performance ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Neuropsychological studies showed that manipulatory and semantic knowledge can be independently impaired in patients with upper-limb apraxia, leading to different tool use disorders. The present study aimed to dissociate the brain regions involved in judging the hand configuration or the context associated to tool use. We focussed on the left supramarginalis gyrus (SMG) and left middle temporal gyrus (MTG), whose activation, as evidenced by functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies, suggests that they may play a critical role in tool use. The distinctive location of SMG in the dorsal visual stream led us to postulate that this parietal region could play a role in processing incoming information about tools to shape hand posture. In contrast, we hypothesized that MTG, because of its interconnections with several cortical areas involved in semantic memory, could contribute to retrieving semantic information necessary to create a contextual representation of tool use. To test these hypotheses, we used neuronavigated transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) to interfere transiently with the function of either left SMG or left MTG in healthy participants performing judgement tasks about either hand configuration or context of tool use. We found that SMG virtual lesions impaired hand configuration but not contextual judgements, whereas MTG lesions selectively interfered with judgements about the context of tool use while leaving hand configuration judgements unaffected. This double dissociation demonstrates that the ability to infer a context of use or a hand posture from tool perception relies on distinct processes, performed in the temporal and parietal regions. The present findings suggest that tool use disorders caused by SMG lesions will be characterized by difficulties in selecting the appropriate hand posture for tool use, whereas MTG lesions will yield difficulties in using tools in the appropriate context.
- Published
- 2013
30. Neuropsychological Approach to Dementia
- Author
-
Capitani, E., Della Sala, S., Spinnler, H., Poeck, Klaus, editor, Freund, Hans-Joachim, editor, and Gänshirt, Heinz, editor
- Published
- 1986
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
31. A Brief History of Upper Limb Ideomotor Apraxia
- Author
-
Kenneth M. Heilman
- Subjects
body regions ,medicine.anatomical_structure ,genetic structures ,Ideational apraxia ,medicine ,Upper limb ,Ideomotor apraxia ,Psychology ,medicine.disease ,behavioral disciplines and activities ,nervous system diseases ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
To successfully interact with the environment, goal-oriented movements made by human limbs must be guided by instructions from the brain. Loss of the ability to program purposeful skilled movements, in the absence of any motor, sensory, or cognitive deficit that could fully account for this disability, is called apraxia. Several types of apraxia were described by Hugo Liepmann in the beginning of the 20th century: ideomotor apraxia, where patients make spatial movement and postural errors as well as temporal errors, limb-kinetic apraxia, where patients are unable to perform precise independend and coordinated finger movements and ideational apraxia, where patients fail to correctly sequence a series of action. More recently, three other types of apraxia have been described: conceptual apraxia, where patients have a loss of mechanical knowledge; dissociation apraxia, where patients are impaired at performing a skilled act in response to stimuli in one modality but can perform normally when the stimulus is given in another modality; and conduction apraxia, where patients are impaired at action imitation. This chapter, using an historical approach, reviews the signs associated with each of these forms of apraxia, as well as their pathophysiology.
- Published
- 2016
32. Ideational action impairments in Alzheimer's disease
- Author
-
H Chainay, C Louarn, and Glyn W. Humphreys
- Subjects
Male ,genetic structures ,Concept Formation ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,Object (grammar) ,Anomia ,Poison control ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Severity of Illness Index ,Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,Alzheimer Disease ,Reference Values ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,medicine ,Humans ,Semantic memory ,Aged ,Analysis of Variance ,Communication ,business.industry ,Apraxia, Ideomotor ,Recognition, Psychology ,Cognition ,Ideomotor apraxia ,medicine.disease ,Imitative Behavior ,Semantics ,Form Perception ,Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology ,Action (philosophy) ,Motor Skills ,Ideational apraxia ,Female ,business ,Psychology ,Cognitive psychology ,Gesture - Abstract
We report data from a group of patients with mild Alzheimer's disease on a range of tasks requiring either stored semantic knowledge about objects (e.g., naming object use) or the execution of action to objects (e.g., miming and using objects). We found that the patients were impaired at miming in response to objects, even when they could describe the object's function. On the other hand, copying gestures was not impaired relative to naming gestures, indicating that an ideomotor deficit in action execution, per se, was unlikely to explain the impairments in object use. We suggest instead that the patients had an impairment in stored motor programmes for action, over and above their deficits in semantic knowledge. Despite this, the patients were better at using than at miming to objects, consistent with the view that proprioceptive input (when using objects) can directly constrain selection of the appropriate motor programme for action.
- Published
- 2016
33. The clinical assessment of apraxia
- Author
-
Adam Cassidy
- Subjects
genetic structures ,05 social sciences ,Motor control ,General Medicine ,Ideomotor apraxia ,medicine.disease ,Corpus callosum ,behavioral disciplines and activities ,Apraxia ,050105 experimental psychology ,Lateralization of brain function ,nervous system diseases ,body regions ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,Motor cognition ,Ideational apraxia ,medicine ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Neurology (clinical) ,Primary motor cortex ,Psychology ,Neuroscience ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Neurologists are familiar with the standard definition of apraxia: ‘an inability to perform a motor task that cannot be adequately explained by motor weakness, sensory loss or a lack of understanding’. Being a definition of exclusion, this has led to a bewildering number of motor disorders being described as forms of apraxia, despite many of these failing to capture the essence of what apraxia really is: a disorder of motor cognition. Apraxia reflects an impairment of the storage and transformation of motor representations in the brain, either through degradation of the semantic knowledge of gestures and tool use or through inability to translate the neural representations of higher level goals accurately into lower level patterns of muscle activation and inhibition. Our current clinical approach to apraxia is similar to that proposed by Liepman in the early 20th century.1 He recognised that left hemispheric lesions tend to cause bilateral upper limb apraxia and suggested a model of motor control in which the left parietal lobe stores a ‘space–time form picture’ of a movement. For a movement to be executed, its picture must be retrieved and activated and then be associated via cortical projections with the relevant motor engrams in the prefrontal regions. From here the information passes to the primary motor cortex before being fed down the corticospinal tracts. For the right upper limb to move the information remains contained within the left hemisphere, but for the left upper limb to move the information from the left parietal lobe must first be sent to the right prefrontal and frontal regions through the corpus callosum. Using this scheme, Liepman delineated three forms of apraxia. He described ideational apraxia as a disruption of the space–time picture, in which the idea of the movement itself is lost or degraded. In ideomotor apraxia , …
- Published
- 2016
34. Right hemisphere dominance for language in a woman with schizophrenia and a porencephalic cyst of the left hemisphere
- Author
-
Bruno Palazzo Nazar, Paulo Mattos, Ricardo de Oliveira-Souza, Fernanda Tovar-Moll, Myriam Monteiro, Paula Gibin Pacheco, and Jorge Moll
- Subjects
medicine.medical_specialty ,Planum temporale ,Audiology ,Wernicke's area ,Functional Laterality ,Lateralization of brain function ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,Aphasia ,medicine ,Humans ,Language ,Cysts ,Inferior parietal lobule ,Middle Aged ,medicine.disease ,Porencephaly ,030227 psychiatry ,Agraphia ,Ideational apraxia ,Schizophrenia ,Female ,Neurology (clinical) ,medicine.symptom ,Psychology ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
A large left hemisphere porencephalic cyst was incidentally found in a 48-year-old woman (MS) with a Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM)-5 diagnosis of schizophrenia. The encephaloclastic characteristics of the cyst indicated that it was acquired between the 22nd and 24th gestational weeks, after the major waves of neuronal migration had tapered off. The cyst destroyed the left temporal and occipital lobes, and the inferior parietal lobule. Surprisingly, MS had no evidence of aphasia, alexia, agraphia, or ideational apraxia; in contrast, cognitive functions dependent on the integrity of the right hemisphere were severely impaired. To test the hypothesis that the development of language in MS took place at the expense of functions that are normally carried out by the right hemisphere, we investigated MS’s correlates of oral comprehension with fMRI as a proxy for auditory comprehension and other cognitive functions strongly lateralized to the posterior left hemisphere, such as ideational praxis and reading. Comprehension of spoken language engaged the homologous of Wernicke’s area in the right planum temporale. Porencephaly may represent a natural model of neuroplasticity supervening at predictable epochs of prenatal development.
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
35. Left Hemisphere Syndromes: Apraxias
- Author
-
Michael Hoffmann
- Subjects
medicine.medical_specialty ,Movement disorders ,genetic structures ,Gait apraxia ,Context (language use) ,Cognition ,Ideomotor apraxia ,medicine.disease ,behavioral disciplines and activities ,Apraxia ,Lateralization of brain function ,Physical medicine and rehabilitation ,Ideational apraxia ,medicine ,medicine.symptom ,Psychology - Abstract
Apraxia refers to an impairment in the performance of skilled motor acts. This is in the context of normal or near-normal elementary neurological function such as motor power and sensation. In addition a long list of specific exclusions and conditions that may masquerade as apraxia need to be excluded. These include the domains of attention, cognition, motor deficits, and movement disorders
- Published
- 2016
36. Gesture comprehension, knowledge and production in Alzheimer’s disease
- Author
-
J. Rénier, Florence Pasquier, M. A. Mackowiak-Cordoliani, L. Anicet, and Marc Rousseaux
- Subjects
genetic structures ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Ideomotor apraxia ,medicine.disease ,behavioral disciplines and activities ,Apraxia ,Neurology ,Gesture recognition ,Ideational apraxia ,medicine ,Dementia ,Neurology (clinical) ,Psychology ,Imitation ,Visual agnosia ,media_common ,Cognitive psychology ,Gesture - Abstract
Background and purpose: Although apraxia is a typical consequence of Alzheimer’s disease (AD), the profile of apraxic impairments is still subject to debate. Here, we analysed apraxia components in patients with AD with mild-to-moderate or moderately severe dementia. Methods: Thirty-one patients were included. We first evaluated simple gestures, that is, the imitation of finger and hand configurations, symbolic gestures (recognition, production on verbal command and imitation), pantomimes (recognition, production on verbal command, imitation and production with the object), general knowledge and complex gestures (tool–object association, function–tool association, production of complex actions and knowledge about action sequences). Tests for dementia (Mini Mental State Examination and the Dementia Rating Scale), language disorders, visual agnosia and executive function were also administered. Results: Compared with controls, patients showed significant difficulties (P ≤ 0.01) in subtests relating to simple gestures (except for the recognition and imitation of symbolic gestures). General knowledge about tools, objects and action sequences was less severely impaired. Performance was frequently correlated with the severity of dementia. Multiple-case analyses revealed that (i) the frequency of apraxia depended on the definition used, (ii) ideomotor apraxia was more frequent than ideational apraxia, (iii) conceptual difficulties were slightly more frequent than production difficulties in the early stage of AD and (iv) difficulties in gesture recognition were frequent (especially for pantomimes). Conclusion: Patients with AD can clearly show gesture apraxia from the mild–moderate stage of dementia onwards. Recognition and imitation disorders are relatively frequent (especially for pantomimes). We did not find conceptual difficulties to be the main problem in early-stage AD.
- Published
- 2012
37. Aphasia and Ideational apraxia
- Author
-
Kazumi Kawahira and Atsuko Ogata
- Subjects
medicine.medical_specialty ,Physical medicine and rehabilitation ,Rehabilitation ,Ideational apraxia ,medicine.medical_treatment ,Aphasia ,medicine ,Motor program ,medicine.symptom ,medicine.disease ,Psychology - Published
- 2012
38. Ideational Apraxia in Parkinson Disease
- Author
-
Kenneth M. Heilman, John B. Williamson, and Mohammad Qureshi
- Subjects
Male ,genetic structures ,Apraxias ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,Parkinson Disease ,General Medicine ,Disease ,Middle Aged ,Neuropsychological Tests ,medicine.disease ,behavioral disciplines and activities ,nervous system diseases ,Psychiatry and Mental health ,Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology ,Case-Control Studies ,Ideational apraxia ,medicine ,Humans ,Female ,Psychology ,Photic Stimulation ,Psychomotor Performance ,Aged ,Cognitive psychology ,Sequence (medicine) - Abstract
The objective of the study was to determine whether ideational apraxia (IA), a loss of ability to plan the sequence of actions needed to achieve a goal, is associated with Parkinson disease (PD).: The frontal lobes play an important role in planning and sequencing, and many patients with PD have frontal lobe dysfunction.: Ten right-handed patients with PD and 10 right-handed neurologically and psychiatrically healthy people participated. To assess for IA, participants were given sets of pictures that showed the steps in completing a task, but the steps were shown out of order. The participants were required to point to the pictures in the correct sequence to complete each task. The participants also performed a control task of sequencing randomly arranged printed single words to create a sentence that described an accompanying picture.: The patients with PD performed more poorly than the controls on the action-sequencing tasks (P0.05). Errors were predominantly in sequencing rather than repetition or omission, indicating that the poor performance was not caused by perseveration. There were no group differences in the task of sequencing words to make a sentence.: These results indicate that patients with PD do have IA, an action-sequence planning deficit. Further research is needed to better understand mechanisms, ecological implications, and potential treatments.
- Published
- 2011
39. Target-related distractors disrupt object selection in everyday action: Evidence from participants with dementia
- Author
-
Tania Giovannetti, Colette Seter, Denene Wambach, Brianne M. Bettcher, David J. Libon, and Laura Brennan
- Subjects
Male ,Apraxias ,Object (grammar) ,Neuropsychological Tests ,Choice Behavior ,Severity of Illness Index ,Task (project management) ,Activities of Daily Living ,medicine ,Selection (linguistics) ,Humans ,Dementia ,Attention ,Aged ,General Neuroscience ,medicine.disease ,Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders ,Psychiatry and Mental health ,Clinical Psychology ,Action (philosophy) ,Ideational apraxia ,Female ,Neurology (clinical) ,Psychomotor Disorders ,Psychology ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
This study evaluated the impact of distractor objects and their similarity to target objects on everyday task performance in dementia. Twenty participants with dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease (n = 12) or subcortical vascular disease (n = 8) were videotaped while they performed 3 discrete tasks: (1) make a cup of coffee, (2) wrap a gift, and (3) pack a lunch under two conditions that were counterbalanced across participants. The conditions differed in terms of the type of distractor objects included in the workspace: (1) Target-Related Distractor Condition - distractor objects were functionally and visually similar to target objects (e.g., salt for sugar) (2) Unrelated Distractor Condition - distractors were neither visually nor functionally similar to targets (e.g., glue for sugar). Participants touched (t = 4.19; p < .01) and used (z = 3.00; p < .01) significantly more distractors, made more distractor errors (i.e., substitutions; t = 2.93; p < .01), and took longer to complete tasks (t = 2.27; p < .05) in the Target-Related Distractor condition. The percent of steps accomplished and non-distractor errors did not differ across conditions (t < 1.26; p > .05 for both). In summary, distractors that were similar to targets elicited significant interference effects circumscribed to object selection. (JINS, 2010, 16, 484–494.)
- Published
- 2010
40. Hugo Liepmann, Parkinson's disease and upper limb apraxia.
- Author
-
Heilman KM
- Subjects
- Humans, Upper Extremity, Apraxias, Parkinson Disease complications
- Abstract
It has now been 100 years since Hugo Liepmann, in his classic 1920 paper described limb kinetic, ideomotor, and ideational apraxia. There are now several rating scales used to assess and grade the signs and symptoms associated with Parkinson's disease; however, none of these assesses patients for the presence of these disabling forms of upper limb apraxia. This paper, reviews the four types of apraxia that can be associated with Parkinson's disease, how they can be tested, the disabilities associated with these disorders and possible treatments., (Published by Elsevier Ltd.)
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
41. Apraxia in Parkinson’s disease and multiple system atrophy
- Author
-
Ö. Ertürk, Sibel Ertan, Ethem Erginöz, Derya Uluduz, Sibel Özekmekçi, Gülay Kenangil, and Hulya Apaydin
- Subjects
medicine.medical_specialty ,Parkinson's disease ,business.industry ,Ideomotor apraxia ,medicine.disease ,Apraxia ,Trunk ,nervous system diseases ,Physical medicine and rehabilitation ,Atrophy ,Neurology ,Ideational apraxia ,mental disorders ,Severity of illness ,medicine ,Anxiety ,Neurology (clinical) ,medicine.symptom ,business - Abstract
Objective: To determine praxis function in patients with Parkinson’s disease (PD) and multiple system atrophy (MSA). Methods: Nineteen patients with PD and 16 patients with probable MSA were recruited into study. Twenty-five age-matched, healthy subjects were included as controls. The Mayo Clinic praxis test battery was applied. Pantomime tasks, including oral/facial, trunk, and upper extremity movement, were used to evaluate ideomotor apraxia (IMA). Sequential tasks, including Luria test for ideational apraxia (IDA) and use of actual objects, were also tested. In addition, Standardized Mini Mental Test (MMSE), Hamilton Depression (HAM-D), and Anxiety (HAM-A) Scales were used. Results: Mean ages of the study participants were 66 ± 7, 68 ± 5, and 65 ± 7 years in PD, MSA, and control groups, respectively. Mean total praxis score was significantly lower for patients with PD (92.4 ± 4) and MSA (75.9 ± 18) than for controls (97.4 ± 2) (P = 0.000). Transitive performances of upper extremities and sequential tasks were significantly impaired in patients with PD compared to control subjects (P
- Published
- 2009
42. Neue Erkenntnisse zur Pathophysiologie der Apraxien durch funktionelle Bildgebung
- Author
-
Gereon R. Fink and Peter Weiss-Blankenhorn
- Subjects
genetic structures ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Posterior parietal cortex ,Ideomotor apraxia ,medicine.disease ,behavioral disciplines and activities ,Apraxia ,nervous system diseases ,Functional imaging ,Psychiatry and Mental health ,Neurology ,Aphasia ,Motor cognition ,Ideational apraxia ,medicine ,Neurology (clinical) ,medicine.symptom ,Psychology ,Imitation ,Neuroscience ,Cognitive psychology ,media_common - Abstract
Apraxias are disorders of motor cognition that cannot be explained by basic sensorimotor deficits or aphasia. The relatively high frequency of apraxia (approximately half of all patients with left-hemispheric stroke suffer from apraxia during the acute phase) as well as its prognostic value for determining the outcome of rehabilitative therapy clearly convey the necessity of more comprehensive research into the pathophysiology of apraxia in order to develop new therapeutic strategies. In recent years, functional imaging (PET and fMRI) has helped to provide important new insights into the pathophysiology of ideomotor apraxia (defective movement plan) and ideational apraxia (defective action concept). In this review, the neural bases for the clinically observed dissociations between the imitation of abstract and symbolic movements (as in ideomotor apraxia) and for the object-trigger system (which is disturbed in ideational apraxia) will be exemplified. Furthermore, we will recapitulate recent studies that provide evidence for the complementary functions of the right and left parietal cortices in the spatial and temporal organization of complex, object-related actions. The particular importance of the left parietal cortex for motor cognition is further supported by studies examining the integration of spatial and temporal movement information during the generation of a movement plan as well as by the generation of such movement plans in the left parietal cortex independent from the hand that executes the movement.
- Published
- 2008
43. Coffee with jelly or unbuttered toast: Commissions and omissions are dissociable aspects of everyday action impairment in Alzheimer's disease
- Author
-
Brianne M. Bettcher, Laura Brennan, Katia Duey, Rachel K. Kessler, Tania Giovannetti, and David J Libron
- Subjects
Male ,Neuropsychological Tests ,Apraxia ,Developmental psychology ,Alzheimer Disease ,Cognitive resource theory ,Activities of Daily Living ,medicine ,Humans ,Dementia ,Aged ,Language ,Aged, 80 and over ,Neuropsychology ,Middle Aged ,medicine.disease ,Cross-Sectional Studies ,Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology ,Caregivers ,Action (philosophy) ,Ideational apraxia ,Female ,Alzheimer's disease ,Construct (philosophy) ,Psychology ,Psychomotor Performance - Abstract
Relative to our understanding of the memory and language deficits associated with Alzheimer's disease (AD), little is known about problems with everyday action performance (i.e., meal preparation, grooming). The resource theory proposes that everyday action problems are best explained by a unitary deficit in general cognitive resources. However, recent research suggests that omission and commission errors may reflect dissociable aspects of action impairment, with only omissions associated with resource limitations. This study examined everyday action performance in 70 participants with AD who also underwent a neuropsychological evaluation. First, correlation and principal component analyses were performed to examine the construct(s) that might explain everyday action impairment. Second, relations between everyday task component(s) and neuropsychological tests were examined by using correlation and regression analyses. Third, differences in everyday action error patterns were examined among participants of comparable overall impairment levels. Results showed omission and commission errors were uncorrelated and distinct components of everyday action performance, predicted by different neuropsychological tests, and differentially distributed even among participants with comparable overall impairment.
- Published
- 2008
44. Functional rehabilitation of upper limb apraxia in poststroke patients: study protocol for a randomized controlled trial
- Author
-
Ted Brown, Guadalupe Molina-Torres, Francisco Javier Barrero-Hernández, María Encarnación Aguilar-Ferrándiz, Mª Carmen García-Ríos, José Manuel Pérez-Mármol, [Pérez-Mármol,JM, García-Ríos,MC, and Aguilar-Ferrándiz,ME] Department of Physical Therapy, University of Granada (UGR), Granada, Spain. [Barrero-Hernandez,FJ] Hospital Clínico San Cecilio, Granada, Spain. [Molina-Torres,G] Department of Nursing and Physical Therapy, University of Almeria (UAL), Almeria, Spain. [Brown,T] Department of Occupational Therapy, School of Primary Health Care, Faculty of Medicine, Nursing and Health Sciences, Monash University, Melbourne, Australia. [Aguilar-Ferrándiz,ME] Fisioterapia, Universidad de Granada, Granada, Spain.
- Subjects
Male ,Activities of daily living ,Time Factors ,Analytical, Diagnostic and Therapeutic Techniques and Equipment::Therapeutics::Rehabilitation::Occupational Therapy [Medical Subject Headings] ,medicine.medical_treatment ,Phenomena and Processes::Physical Phenomena::Time::Time Factors [Medical Subject Headings] ,España ,Named Groups::Persons::Age Groups::Adult::Aged::Aged, 80 and over [Medical Subject Headings] ,Medicine (miscellaneous) ,Analytical, Diagnostic and Therapeutic Techniques and Equipment::Investigative Techniques::Methods::Research Design [Medical Subject Headings] ,Named Groups::Persons::Age Groups::Adult::Middle Aged [Medical Subject Headings] ,Apraxia ,Functional Laterality ,law.invention ,Disability Evaluation ,Study Protocol ,Randomized controlled trial ,Clinical Protocols ,Occupational Therapy ,law ,Surveys and Questionnaires ,Activities of Daily Living ,Analytical, Diagnostic and Therapeutic Techniques and Equipment::Therapeutics::Physical Therapy Modalities [Medical Subject Headings] ,Phenomena and Processes::Musculoskeletal and Neural Physiological Phenomena::Musculoskeletal Physiological Phenomena::Musculoskeletal Physiological Processes::Movement::Motor Activity [Medical Subject Headings] ,Pharmacology (medical) ,Analytical, Diagnostic and Therapeutic Techniques and Equipment::Therapeutics::Clinical Protocols [Medical Subject Headings] ,Stroke ,Aged, 80 and over ,Geographicals::Geographic Locations::Europe::Spain [Medical Subject Headings] ,Rehabilitation ,Recuperación de la función ,Evaluación de la discapacidad ,Lateralidad funcional ,Stroke Rehabilitation ,Analytical, Diagnostic and Therapeutic Techniques and Equipment::Investigative Techniques::Epidemiologic Methods::Data Collection::Questionnaires [Medical Subject Headings] ,Middle Aged ,Combined Modality Therapy ,Encuestas y Cuestionarios ,Treatment Outcome ,Research Design ,Ideational apraxia ,Female ,Rehabilitation neurológica ,Diseases::Nervous System Diseases::Central Nervous System Diseases::Brain Diseases::Cerebrovascular Disorders::Stroke [Medical Subject Headings] ,Occupational therapy ,Adult ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Analytical, Diagnostic and Therapeutic Techniques and Equipment::Therapeutics::Combined Modality Therapy [Medical Subject Headings] ,Apraxias ,Check Tags::Male [Medical Subject Headings] ,Motor Activity ,Accidente cerebrovascular ,Actividades cotidianas ,Upper Extremity ,Modalidades de fisioterapia ,Physical medicine and rehabilitation ,Terapia ocupacional ,Double-Blind Method ,medicine ,Named Groups::Persons::Age Groups::Adult [Medical Subject Headings] ,Humans ,Named Groups::Persons::Age Groups::Adult::Aged [Medical Subject Headings] ,Physical Therapy Modalities ,Aged ,Analytical, Diagnostic and Therapeutic Techniques and Equipment::Diagnosis::Prognosis::Treatment Outcome [Medical Subject Headings] ,Proyectos de investigación ,business.industry ,Terapia combinada ,Recovery of Function ,Ideomotor apraxia ,medicine.disease ,Actividad motora ,Phenomena and Processes::Biological Phenomena::Recovery of Function [Medical Subject Headings] ,Analytical, Diagnostic and Therapeutic Techniques and Equipment::Therapeutics::Rehabilitation::Activities of Daily Living [Medical Subject Headings] ,Método doble ciego ,Check Tags::Female [Medical Subject Headings] ,Spain ,Factores de tiempo ,Physical therapy ,Protocolos clínicos ,Analytical, Diagnostic and Therapeutic Techniques and Equipment::Investigative Techniques::Epidemiologic Methods::Epidemiologic Research Design::Double-Blind Method [Medical Subject Headings] ,Resultado del tratamiento ,Phenomena and Processes::Musculoskeletal and Neural Physiological Phenomena::Nervous System Physiological Phenomena::Dominance, Cerebral::Functional Laterality [Medical Subject Headings] ,business - Abstract
Background Upper limb apraxia is a common disorder associated with stroke that can reduce patients’ independence levels in activities of daily living and increase levels of disability. Traditional rehabilitation programs designed to promote the recovery of upper limb function have mainly focused on restorative or compensatory approaches. However, no previous studies have been completed that evaluate a combined intervention method approach, where patients concurrently receive cognitive training and learn compensatory strategies for enhancing daily living activities. Methods/Design This study will use a two-arm, assessor-blinded, parallel, randomized controlled trial design, involving 40 patients who present a left- or right-sided unilateral vascular lesion poststroke and a clinical diagnosis of upper limb apraxia. Participants will be randomized to either a combined functional rehabilitation or a traditional health education group. The experimental group will receive an 8-week combined functional program at home, including physical and occupational therapy focused on restorative and compensatory techniques for upper limb apraxia, 3 days per week in 30-min intervention periods. The control group will receive a conventional health education program once a month over 8 weeks, based on improving awareness of physical and functional limitations and facilitating the adaptation of patients to the home. Study outcomes will be assessed immediately postintervention and at the 2-month follow-up. The primary outcome measure will be basic activities of daily living skills as assessed with the Barthel Index. Secondary outcome measures will include the following: 1) the Lawton and Brody Instrumental Activities of Daily Living Scale, 2) the Observation and Scoring of ADL-Activities, 3) the De Renzi Test for Ideational Apraxia, 4) the De Renzi Test for Ideomotor Apraxia, 5) Recognition of Gestures, 6) the Test of Upper Limb Apraxia (TULIA), and 7) the Quality of Life Scale For Stroke (ECVI-38). Discussion This trial is expected to clarify the effectiveness of a combined functional rehabilitation approach compared to a conservative intervention for improving upper limb movement and function in poststroke patients. Trial registration Clinical Trial Gov number NCT02199093. The protocol registration was received 23 July 2014. Participant enrollment began on 1 May 2014. The trial is expected to be completed in March 2016. Electronic supplementary material The online version of this article (doi:10.1186/s13063-015-1034-1) contains supplementary material, which is available to authorized users.
- Published
- 2015
45. Limb apraxia and the 'affordance competition hypothesis'
- Author
-
Rounis, Elisabeth and Humphreys, Glyn
- Subjects
ideomotor apraxia ,Behavioral Neuroscience ,Psychiatry and Mental health ,Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology ,genetic structures ,limb apraxia ,Neurology ,Mini Review ,ideational apraxia ,route to action model ,affordance competition hypothesis ,Biological Psychiatry ,Neuroscience - Abstract
Limb apraxia, a disorder of higher order motor control, has long been a challenge for clinical assessment and understanding (Leiguarda and Marsden, 2000). The deficits originally described in limb apraxia (Liepmann, 1920) have been classified by the nature of the errors made by the patients leading to, namely, ideational and ideomotor apraxia. The dual stream hypothesis (Goodale and Milner, 1992) has been used to explain these categories: ideational apraxia is thought to relate to a deficit in the concept of a movement (coded in the ventral stream). Patients have difficulty using objects, sequencing actions to interact with them or pantomiming their use. Ideomotor apraxia, on the other hand, is thought to arise from problems in the accurate implementation of movements within the dorsal stream. One of the limitations on understanding apraxia is the failure by the clinical literature to draw on knowledge of the factors determining actions in the environment. Here we emphasize the role of affordance. There is much recent work indicating that our responses to stimuli are strongly influenced by the actions that the objects "afford", based on their physical properties and the intentions of the actor (e.g., Tucker and Ellis, 1998). The concept of affordance, originally suggested by Gibson (1979) has been incorporated in a recent model of interactive behavior that draws from findings in non-human primates, namely the "affordance competition hypothesis" (Cisek, 2007). This postulates that interactive behavior arises by a process of competition between possible actions elicited by the environment. In this paper we argue that "affordance competition" may play a role in apraxia. We review evidence that at least some aspects of apraxia may reflect an abnormal sensitivity to competition when multiple affordances are present (Riddoch et al., 1998) and/or a poor ability to exert cognitive control over this competition when it occurs. This framework suggests a new way of conceptualizing deficits in apraxia which invites further investigations in the field.
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
46. Progressive transcortical sensory aphasia and progressive ideational apraxia owing to temporoparietal cortical atrophy
- Author
-
Asuka Nakajima and Michitaka Funayama
- Subjects
Male ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Apraxias ,Transcortical sensory aphasia ,Clinical Neurology ,Posterior parietal cortex ,Dyscalculia ,Case Report ,Audiology ,Apraxia ,Primary progressive aphasia ,Alzheimer Disease ,Aphasia ,medicine ,Humans ,Agraphia ,Aged ,Spatial Memory ,Tomography, Emission-Computed, Single-Photon ,Memory Disorders ,business.industry ,Conceptual apraxia ,Posterior cortical atrophy ,Brain ,General Medicine ,Middle Aged ,medicine.disease ,Logopenic variant of primary progressive aphasia ,Aphasia, Primary Progressive ,Ideational apraxia ,Neurology (clinical) ,medicine.symptom ,Atrophy ,business ,Neuroscience - Abstract
Background In contrast to frontotemporal lobar degeneration, atrophy of the focal posterior lateral cortex has not been thoroughly studied. Three clinical types of focal cortical atrophy have been described: 1) logopenic variant of primary progressive aphasia, which presents with impaired repetition despite normal articulation; 2) posterior cortical atrophy, which presents with prominent visuospatial deficits; and 3) primary progressive apraxia. All three clinical types are characterized by specific patterns of hypometabolism/hypoperfusion: the left posterior perisylvian area in the logopenic variant of primary progressive aphasia, bilateral parietooccipital areas in posterior cortical atrophy, and the parietal cortex in primary progressive apraxia. However, not every patient clearly fits into one of these categories. Case presentation Here we describe two patients with atypical focal cortical presentations. They presented with a history of a few years of progressive transcortical sensory aphasia characterized by fluent output with normal grammar and syntax, normal repetition, sentence comprehension deficits, and anomia without loss of word meaning. They also presented with progressive apraxia that began at the initial stages. Some forms of posterior symptoms including acalculia, agraphia, and visuospatial deficits were also observed. Hypoperfusion was noted mainly in the left temporoparietal region, which is slightly posterior to the perisylvian area. Conclusions Although our cases lack in CSF findings and PIB scan, these two cases and previous reports might suggest the existence of a subgroup of patients presenting with transcortical sensory aphasia, apraxia, and posterior symptoms (acalculia, agraphia, and visuospatial deficits) in the setting of Alzheimer’s disease. This subgroup may reflect the spectrum of clinical manifestations between logopenic variant of primary progressive aphasia and posterior cortical atrophy.
- Published
- 2015
47. A case of variably protease-sensitive prionopathy treated with doxycyclin
- Author
-
James W. Ironside, Romana Höftberger, Thomas Ströbel, Jasmin Rahimi, Raffi Topakian, Helen Yull, Fahmy Aboulenein-Djamshidian, Gabor G. Kovacs, Mark Head, Serge Weis, Johannes Trenkler, Herbert Budka, Hamid Assar, University of Zurich, and Kovacs, G G
- Subjects
medicine.medical_specialty ,Pathology ,Crying ,business.industry ,Neuropsychology ,10208 Institute of Neuropathology ,Variably protease-sensitive prionopathy ,610 Medicine & health ,Neuropathology ,Disease ,medicine.disease ,2746 Surgery ,Psychiatry and Mental health ,2738 Psychiatry and Mental Health ,2728 Neurology (clinical) ,Internal medicine ,Ideational apraxia ,Gait Ataxia ,medicine ,570 Life sciences ,biology ,Surgery ,Neurology (clinical) ,Family history ,medicine.symptom ,business - Abstract
Variably protease-sensitive prionopathy (VPSPr) is a recently described neurodegenerative disorder characterised by the presence of spongiform encephalopathy and an unusual immunostaining and immunoblotting pattern for the disease-associated prion protein (PrPSc).1 This links VPSPr to human prion diseases, which are uniformly fatal disorders. The clinical symptoms and the longer duration of illness make VPSPr distinct from sporadic or idiopathic Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease (sCJD).1 Doxycycline treatment has been evaluated in patients with prion disease, however, there is little evidence that it can reverse the clinical symptoms or reduce the underlying disease progression once established.2 We present a patient with VPSPr who received doxycycline and survived for an extended period of time in an akinetic and mute state. Neuropathological examination was performed using published methods and various anti-PrP antibodies.3 Frozen tissues from selected brain regions were available for biochemical analysis. Tissues were analysed for the presence of protease-resistant PrP (PrPres) as previously described (see online supplementary material).4 In May 2007, a registered psychiatrist suspected an organic affective disorder in a 54-year-old Austrian woman. Two months earlier, medical work-up for presumed weight loss of 16 kg within the past 18 months had been unremarkable. In June 2007, the patient was admitted to a clinic that specialised in disorders of the nervous system. Her family history was negative for neurodegenerative diseases and there was no evidence of exposure to toxins. She reported depressed mood and short-term memory problems, and difficulties with balance, walking, driving and cooking. On neuropsychological examination she was oriented to time, place, person and situation, Mini-Mental State Examination score was 22/30, clock drawing test score was 3/9. She had word-finding difficulties, ideational apraxia, acalculia and visuoconstructive deficits. She displayed affective incontinence with crying fits. She had gait ataxia, and extensor plantar responses were observed with increased tone …
- Published
- 2015
48. Apraxie — Neurowissenschaft und Klinik
- Author
-
T. Platz
- Subjects
Ataxia ,genetic structures ,Context (language use) ,General Medicine ,Ideomotor apraxia ,Somatosensory system ,medicine.disease ,behavioral disciplines and activities ,Apraxia ,body regions ,Psychiatry and Mental health ,medicine.anatomical_structure ,Neurology ,Ideational apraxia ,medicine ,Neurology (clinical) ,medicine.symptom ,Psychology ,Neuroscience ,Paresis ,Neuroanatomy - Abstract
Apraxic phenomena occur in various neurological conditions. Selective motion control is viewed as the basic capacity to make fine and precise, isolated or independent face or limb movements. Its deficit can indicate limb-kinetic apraxia if it is not explained by paresis, somatosensory deafferentation, or ataxia. The core deficit in ideomotor apraxia could be deficient movement representations, i.e. the combination of invariant features of intrinsic and extrinsic coding for a given movement, which are most important when movements have to be performed outside their typical context. Ideational apraxia would be defined by a semantic deficit related to action. Frontal apraxia is characterised by an action-sequencing deficit. A detailed model is proposed regarding processes relevant to praxis.
- Published
- 2005
49. Dissociable distal and proximal motor components: Evidence from perseverative errors in three apraxic patients
- Author
-
Nancy Venable, Raffaella I. Rumiati, and Tim Shallice
- Subjects
Cognitive Neuroscience ,Perseveration ,Cognitive disorder ,Posterior parietal cortex ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Body movement ,Ideomotor apraxia ,medicine.disease ,Apraxia ,Developmental psychology ,Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology ,Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,Ideational apraxia ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,medicine ,medicine.symptom ,Psychology ,Perseverative behaviour ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
In the present study we analysed the perseverative behaviour of three apraxic patients (FG, CEO, and VCR) while they were pantomiming the use of common objects and during their real use. The three patients were found to perseverate from one trial to later ones aspects of a given action in the pantomiming and in real use tasks. In particular, our main result was a striking double dissociation between patients in perseverating hand and arm movements. Patients FG and CEO made more perseveration errors involving the hand than VCR did, whereas the perseverative errors made by VCR involving the arm were more numerous than those made by FG and CEO. The patients differed also in other aspects of the perseverations, e.g., their median lags. Results are used to exend previous theories developed in the linguistic domain to that of actions.
- Published
- 2005
50. Goal-directed imitation in patients with ideomotor apraxia
- Author
-
Marcel Brass, Susanne Woschina, Arthur M. Jacobs, and Harold Bekkering
- Subjects
CORTEX ,REPRESENTATION ,DIMENSIONS ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,LEFT-HEMISPHERE DAMAGE ,HAND ,LIMB APRAXIA ,MEANINGLESS GESTURES ,Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,ACTION PERCEPTION ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,medicine ,media_common ,Action, intention, and motor control ,IDEATIONAL APRAXIA ,Perspective (graphical) ,Cognition ,Limb apraxia ,Ideomotor apraxia ,MOTOR FACILITATION ,medicine.disease ,Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology ,Action (philosophy) ,Ideational apraxia ,Psychology ,Imitation ,Cognitive psychology ,Gesture - Abstract
Contains fulltext : 54642.pdf (Publisher’s version ) (Closed access) The present study compared imitation performance in patients with ideomotor apraxia (IMA), eight right hemispheric-damaged patients, and eight control participants without neurological damage in three experiments. Experiment 1 confirmed in the Goldenberg test that IMA patients were particularly impaired in hand gestures and combined finger and hand gestures, but not in the imitation of finger gestures, compared to the other two groups. Experiment 2, however, demonstrated that finger selection is not per se preserved in imitative behaviour in patients with IMA. Experiment 3 confirmed this finding in an experiment under visual control. Together, the results add evidence to the idea that imitation should be viewed from a goal-directed rather than a body-mapping perspective, and that highest priority is given to more distal aspects of imitation as reaching for the correct object, rather than the means used to achieve the goal of a modelled action.
- Published
- 2005
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.