The nectar-foraging behavior of male euglossine bees (Hymenoptera: Apidae) was studied at a population of Calathea latifolia (Marantaceae) on Barro Colorado Island, Panama, for two weeks. Several marked males of Exaerete smaragdina and Euglossa imperialis foraged at C. latifolia on a daily basis. Immigration rates were low, and the site fidelity of bees was high. Selection of inflorescences by the marked individuals was not random. The bees learned specific locations of inflorescences at which they consistently foraged and ignored nearby inflorescences. However, not all inflorescences in a bee's repertoire were included in each foraging bout. The sequence of inflorescences visited was not the same within or between days nor were the foraging routes uni-directional. Foraging bouts of both male and female bees did not conform to rigid time schedules. Thus, male euglossine bees are not necessarily as transient and vagabond as previously portrayed. Those males that are site-constant forage in a fashion reminiscent of the traplining behavior once described for females. We propose that euglossine bees change their foraging habits as shifts in resource availability and dispersion occur. EUGLOSSINE BEES ARE IMPORTANT long-distance pollinators of many lowto mid-elevation neotropical plants (Dressler 1968, Dodson 1966, Janzen 1971, Williams and Dodson 1972). Females visit flowers for resin, nectar, and pollen to construct and provision their nests (Michener 1974, Armbruster and Webster 1979, Zucchi et al. 1969 for a review). These bees are thought to have specific foraging routes known as "traplines" where they visit the same flowering individuals in particular sequence on a daily basis. Janzen (1971) noted that females often fly these circuits about the same time on consecutive days. The plants incorporated in traplines are "steady state" bloomers (terminology: Gentry 1974) which produce few flowers each day and flower over a long period (Janzen 1971). Although traplining behavior implies site fidelity, Janzen (1971) reports that females can fly considerable distances rapidly so that a given trapline may cover a large area. Male euglossine bees may visit the same nectar hosts as females (Zucchi et al. 1969 for a review). However, their foraging behavior may be very different (Kroodsma 1975) because males are thought to lead the nomadic, "vagabond" life (Dodson 1970, Dodson et al. 1969). Unlike females of some species, once males eclose they leave the nest never to return (Dodson 1966, Zucchi et al. 1969, Roberts and Dodson 1967). Individual males may spend each night at a different locality (Williams and Dodson 1972) although exceptions have been noted (Dodson 1966). Like female euglossine bees-, males are capable of rapid, long-distance flight and have been seen flying several kilometers over open water and across Andean mountain passes (Dodson in Dressler 1968; Dressler 1968; Janzen 1971; Ackerman pers. obs. 1979, 1980). However, since males are not tied to a nesting site, Williams and Dodson (1972) suggest that they have a wider foraging range than females, and Janzen (1981) suggests that they may occupy in great part different habitats from females. During August and September 1979, we studied the nectar-foraging behavior of two euglossine species, Euglossa imperialis (Cockerell) and Exaerete smaragdina (Guerin-Meneville), on Ca/athea latifolia (Willd. ex Link) Klotzsch (Marantaceae) at the Laboratory Clearing of Barro Colorado Island (BCI), Lake Gatun, Panama. We present evidence that male euglossines are not necessarily as transient and vagabond as portrayed and that their food-foraging behavior resembles that described for females. THE PLANT AND FLOWER VISITORS Calathea latifolia is common in central Panama where it grows at the forest edge and in adjacent, very often disturbed areas (Kennedy 1978). It is common in the Laboratory Clearing and sparse in the surrounding secondary and primary tropical moist forest. It is an herbaceous, rhizomatous perennial, up to 2.5 m tall (mostly 1-1.5 m at our site), and produces a single, cone-like, terminal inflorescence per aerial shoot. Flowers are produced continuously for several months BIOTROPICA 14(4): 241-248 1982 241 lPresent address: Department of Biology, University of Puerto Rico, Rio Piedras, Puerto Rico 00931, U.S.A. This content downloaded from 207.46.13.111 on Tue, 09 Aug 2016 04:40:13 UTC All use subject to http://about.jstor.org/terms duri.ng the rainy season. We found an average of 7.4 flowers per inflorescence (n 102, s.d. 3.6) along Pipeline Road (north of Gamboa, Panama). Kennedy (1978) reported a slightly smaller average (5.1, n=45). Each flower lasts one day. Flowers of C. latifolia do not open spontaneously so the first forager must force open the bud to feed on the nectar. An unopened, unvisited flower contains an average of 14 pul of nectar (n= 52, s.d.= 4.9) composed of 38.5 percent sugar (weight by total weight, n _ 24, s.d. 3.9). Nectar samples were drawn from flowers collected between 0800 and 0830. Sugar content from these was determined by pooling contents of two flowers per sample in order to obtain a pocket-refractometer reading. The nectar reward is comparable to other bee-pollinated neotropical plants (Baker 1978). Calathea latifolia is visited by a variety of euglossine bees across its range (Kennedy 1978), and is successfully pollinated by many. The most important pollinators on BCI were Eg. imperialis and Ex. smaragdina. Euglossa imperialis is medium-sized, 13-16 mm long, and is common in lowland forests of central Panama (Ackerman 1981). Exaerete smaragdina is a larger bee, relatively uncommon, 18-20 mm long, and a nest parasite of other large euglossines, e.g., Eulaemra nigrita Lepeliter (Kimsey 1979). It is never as abundant as Eg. imperialis in central Panama. Less commonly observed visitors were Eulaema meriana (Oliver), Eulaemat leucopyga (Friese), El. nigrita, and Exaerete frontalis (GuerinMeneville).