145 results on '"Epskamp S"'
Search Results
2. Author Correction: Network analysis of multivariate data in psychological science (Nature Reviews Methods Primers, (2021), 1, 1, (58), 10.1038/s43586-021-00055-w)
- Author
-
Borsboom D., Borsboom, D, Deserno, M, Rhemtulla, M, Epskamp, S, Fried, E, Mcnally, R, Robinaugh, D, Perugini, M, Dalege, J, Costantini, G, Isvoranu, A, Wysocki, A, van Borkulo, C, van Bork, R, Waldorp, L, Borsboom D., Deserno M. K., Rhemtulla M., Epskamp S., Fried E. I., McNally R. J., Robinaugh D. J., Perugini M., Dalege J., Costantini G., Isvoranu A. -M., Wysocki A. C., van Borkulo C. D., van Bork R., Waldorp L. J., Borsboom D., Borsboom, D, Deserno, M, Rhemtulla, M, Epskamp, S, Fried, E, Mcnally, R, Robinaugh, D, Perugini, M, Dalege, J, Costantini, G, Isvoranu, A, Wysocki, A, van Borkulo, C, van Bork, R, Waldorp, L, Borsboom D., Deserno M. K., Rhemtulla M., Epskamp S., Fried E. I., McNally R. J., Robinaugh D. J., Perugini M., Dalege J., Costantini G., Isvoranu A. -M., Wysocki A. C., van Borkulo C. D., van Bork R., and Waldorp L. J.
- Abstract
In the original version of the article, the node labels in Figure 4 were incorrectly represented, and Figure 8 was inaccurate due to a coding error. In the legend of Figure 7b, the grey squares and dark squares refer to the value zero and not, respectively. These errors have been corrected in the HTML and PDF versions of the article and the original versions of Figures 4 and 8 are shown below.
- Published
- 2022
3. Reply to ‘Critiques of network analysis of multivariate data in psychological science’
- Author
-
Borsboom, D, Deserno, M, Rhemtulla, M, Epskamp, S, Fried, E, Mcnally, R, Robinaugh, D, Perugini, M, Dalege, J, Costantini, G, Isvoranu, A, Wysocki, A, van Borkulo, C, van Bork, R, Waldorp, L, Borsboom D., Deserno M. K., Rhemtulla M., Epskamp S., Fried E. I., McNally R. J., Robinaugh D. J., Perugini M., Dalege J., Costantini G., Isvoranu A. -M., Wysocki A. C., van Borkulo C. D., van Bork R., Waldorp L. J., Borsboom, D, Deserno, M, Rhemtulla, M, Epskamp, S, Fried, E, Mcnally, R, Robinaugh, D, Perugini, M, Dalege, J, Costantini, G, Isvoranu, A, Wysocki, A, van Borkulo, C, van Bork, R, Waldorp, L, Borsboom D., Deserno M. K., Rhemtulla M., Epskamp S., Fried E. I., McNally R. J., Robinaugh D. J., Perugini M., Dalege J., Costantini G., Isvoranu A. -M., Wysocki A. C., van Borkulo C. D., van Bork R., and Waldorp L. J.
- Published
- 2022
4. Bayesian uncertainty estimation for Gaussian graphical models and centrality indices
- Author
-
Jongerling, J., Epskamp, S., Williams, D. R., Jongerling, J., Epskamp, S., and Williams, D. R.
- Abstract
In the network approach to psychopathology, psychological constructs are conceptualized as networks of interacting components (e.g., the symptoms of a disorder). In this network view, interest is on the degree to which symptoms influence each other, both directly and indirectly. These direct and indirect influences are often captured with centrality indices, however, the estimation method often used with these networks, the frequentist graphical LASSO (GLASSO), has difficulty estimating (uncertainty in) these measures. Bayesian estimation might provide a solution, as it is better suited to deal with bias in the sampling distribution of centrality indices. This study therefore compares estimation of symptom networks with Bayesian GLASSO- and Horseshoe priors to estimation using the frequentist GLASSO using extensive simulations. Results showed that the Bayesian GLASSO performed better than the Horseshoe, and that the Bayesian GLASSO outperformed the frequentist GLASSO with respect to bias in edge weights, centrality measures, correlation between estimated and true partial correlations, and specificity. Sensitivity was better for the frequentist GLASSO, but performance of the Bayesian GLASSO is usually close. With respect to uncertainty in the centrality measures, the Bayesian GLASSO shows good coverage for strength and closeness centrality, but uncertainty in betweenness centrality is estimated less well.
- Published
- 2023
5. Network Perspectives
- Author
-
Borsboom, D., Cramer, A.O.J., Fried, E.I., Isvoranu, A.M., Robinaugh, D.J., Dalege, J., Maas, H.L.J. van der, Epskamp, S., Waldorp, L.J., Isvoranu, A.M., Epskamp, S., Waldorp, L.J., and Borsboom, D.
- Subjects
Social Development - Abstract
Item does not contain fulltext This chapter discusses theoretical foundations of network analysis by outlining different ways in which networks can be used. In the broadest of these ways, often denoted the network approach, the researcher views a substantive phenomenon through the lens of networks. In this case, networks mainly function to organize observations and to suggest theoretical ideas. The network approach can subsequently be specified in at least two ways. First, by constructing psychometric network models, which formulate a probability distribution for a set of observations, typically by representing variables as nodes and conditional associations between these variables as edges. Second, by constructing network theories, which offer putative explanations of empirical phenomena. Thus, where network theories are tied to a particular empirical domain, network models are generic, i.e., independent of any particular domain. Finally, the chapter discusses ways in which the relation between network approaches, network theories, and network models can be understood.
- Published
- 2022
6. Descriptive, Predictive and Explanatory Personality Research: Different Goals, Different Approaches, but a Shared Need to Move Beyond the Big Few Traits
- Author
-
Mottus, R, Wood, D, Condon, D, Back, M, Baumert, A, Costantini, G, Epskamp, S, Greiff, S, Johnson, W, Lukaszewski, A, Murray, A, Revelle, W, Wright, A, Yarkoni, T, Ziegler, M, Zimmermann, J, Mottus R., Wood D., Condon D. M., Back M. D., Baumert A., Costantini G., Epskamp S., Greiff S., Johnson W., Lukaszewski A., Murray A., Revelle W., Wright A. G. C., Yarkoni T., Ziegler M., Zimmermann J., Mottus, R, Wood, D, Condon, D, Back, M, Baumert, A, Costantini, G, Epskamp, S, Greiff, S, Johnson, W, Lukaszewski, A, Murray, A, Revelle, W, Wright, A, Yarkoni, T, Ziegler, M, Zimmermann, J, Mottus R., Wood D., Condon D. M., Back M. D., Baumert A., Costantini G., Epskamp S., Greiff S., Johnson W., Lukaszewski A., Murray A., Revelle W., Wright A. G. C., Yarkoni T., Ziegler M., and Zimmermann J.
- Abstract
We argue that it is useful to distinguish between three key goals of personality science—description, prediction and explanation—and that attaining them often requires different priorities and methodological approaches. We put forward specific recommendations such as publishing findings with minimum a priori aggregation and exploring the limits of predictive models without being constrained by parsimony and intuitiveness but instead maximizing out-of-sample predictive accuracy. We argue that naturally occurring variance in many decontextualized and multidetermined constructs that interest personality scientists may not have individual causes, at least as this term is generally understood and in ways that are human-interpretable, never mind intervenable. If so, useful explanations are narratives that summarize many pieces of descriptive findings rather than models that target individual cause–effect associations. By meticulously studying specific and contextualized behaviours, thoughts, feelings and goals, however, individual causes of variance may ultimately be identifiable, although such causal explanations will likely be far more complex, phenomenon-specific and person-specific than anticipated thus far. Progress in all three areas—description, prediction and explanation—requires higher dimensional models than the currently dominant ‘Big Few’ and supplementing subjective trait-ratings with alternative sources of information such as informant-reports and behavioural measurements. Developing a new generation of psychometric tools thus provides many immediate research opportunities. © 2020 European Association of Personality Psychology.
- Published
- 2020
7. Network perspectives
- Author
-
Isvoranu, A.M., Epskamp, S., Waldorp, L.J., Borsboom, D., Cramer, A.O.J., Fried, E.I., Robinaugh, D.J., Dalege, J., Maas, H.L.J. van der, Isvoranu, A.M., Epskamp, S., Waldorp, L.J., Borsboom, D., Cramer, A.O.J., Fried, E.I., Robinaugh, D.J., Dalege, J., and Maas, H.L.J. van der
- Abstract
Item does not contain fulltext, This chapter discusses theoretical foundations of network analysis by outlining different ways in which networks can be used. In the broadest of these ways, often denoted the network approach, the researcher views a substantive phenomenon through the lens of networks. In this case, networks mainly function to organize observations and to suggest theoretical ideas. The network approach can subsequently be specified in at least two ways. First, by constructing psychometric network models, which formulate a probability distribution for a set of observations, typically by representing variables as nodes and conditional associations between these variables as edges. Second, by constructing network theories, which offer putative explanations of empirical phenomena. Thus, where network theories are tied to a particular empirical domain, network models are generic, i.e., independent of any particular domain. Finally, the chapter discusses ways in which the relation between network approaches, network theories, and network models can be understood.
- Published
- 2022
8. Psychopathological networks: Theory, methods and practice
- Author
-
Bringmann, L.F., Albers, C.J., Bockting, C.L.H., Borsboom, D., Ceulemans, E., Cramer, A.O.J., Epskamp, S., Eronen, M.I., Hamaker, E.L., Kuppens, P., Lutz, W., McNally, R.J., Molenaar, P., Tio, P., Völkle, M.C., Wichers, M.C., Bringmann, L.F., Albers, C.J., Bockting, C.L.H., Borsboom, D., Ceulemans, E., Cramer, A.O.J., Epskamp, S., Eronen, M.I., Hamaker, E.L., Kuppens, P., Lutz, W., McNally, R.J., Molenaar, P., Tio, P., Völkle, M.C., and Wichers, M.C.
- Abstract
Item does not contain fulltext, In recent years, network approaches to psychopathology have sparked much debate and have had a significant impact on how mental disorders are perceived in the field of clinical psychology. However, there are many important challenges in moving from theory to empirical research and clinical practice and vice versa. Therefore, in this article, we bring together different points of view on psychological networks by methodologists and clinicians to give a critical overview on these challenges, and to present an agenda for addressing these challenges. In contrast to previous reviews, we especially focus on methodological issues related to temporal networks. This includes topics such as selecting and assessing the quality of the nodes in the network, distinguishing between- and within-person effects in networks, relating items that are measured at different time scales, and dealing with changes in network structures. These issues are not only important for researchers using network models on empirical data, but also for clinicians, who are increasingly likely to encounter (person-specific) networks in the consulting room.
- Published
- 2022
9. Modeling Change in Networks
- Author
-
Haslbeck, J.M.B., Ryan, O., van der Maas, H.L.J., Waldorp, L.J., Isvoranu, A.-M., Epskamp, S., Waldrop, L., Borsboom, D., Psychology Other Research (FMG), Brain and Cognition, and Psychologische Methodenleer (Psychologie, FMG)
- Abstract
This chapter discusses time-varying network models, which allow for network models with parameters that change across time. These models are relevant when one is interested in how individuals change across time. The chapter introduces the reader to time-varying models and discusses various methods to estimate them, which make different assumptions about the true time-varying nature of parameters. Finally, the chapter illustrates one of the methods by fitting a time-varying Gaussian graphical model (GGM) to a time series of mood related measurements.
- Published
- 2022
10. Idealized Modeling of Psychological Dynamics
- Author
-
Dalege, J., Haslbeck, J.M.B., Marsman, M., Isvoranu, A.-M., Epskamp, S., Waldrop, L., Borsboom, D., Psychology Other Research (FMG), and Psychologische Methodenleer (Psychologie, FMG)
- Abstract
This chapter introduces the reader to studying psychological dynamics operating on network models. The chapter will illustrate such dynamics using the Ising model, an undirected network model for binary variables in which nodes mutually influence one-another. First, the chapter discusses the basics of the Ising model. Second, the chapter introduces the reader to dynamics emerging from the Ising model, such as polarization and hysteresis. These dynamics will be illustrated using the example of attitude networks. Third, the chapter will illustrate how the Ising model can be used to model cross-sectional phenomena as an alternative to latent trait theories. General intelligence will be used as an example for this illustration to show how the positive manifold and block structures in correlation matrices can arise from a network implemented in the Ising model.
- Published
- 2022
11. Pairwise Markov Random Fields
- Author
-
Epskamp, S., Haslbeck, J.M.B., Isvoranu, A.-M., van Borkulo, C.D., Waldrop, L.J., Borsboom, D., Urban Mental Health, Psychology Other Research (FMG), and FMG
- Abstract
This chapter introduces pairwise Markov random fields (PMRFs), a class of models of which the parameters can be represented as an undirected network. In this undirected network nodes represent variables and edges represent the strength of association between two variables after conditioning on all other variables included in the model. The chapter focuses on specific classes of PMRFs often used in network psychometrics: Gaussian graphical models (GGM; a network of partial correlations) for continuous data, Ising models for binary data, and mixed graphical models (MGM) for data with different types of variables. PMRFs can be interpreted in multiple ways: the models can be used as a general statistical modeling framework, as an exploratory tool to investigate predictive relationships between variables, as a tool to generate causal hypotheses, as a causal model itself, and as an exploratory tool to uncover latent variables. The chapter concludes with an introduction to estimating PMRFs from data using the bootnet and psychonetrics packages.
- Published
- 2022
12. Bayesian Uncertainty Estimation for Gaussian Graphical Models and Centrality Indices
- Author
-
Jongerling, J., primary, Epskamp, S., additional, and Williams, D. R., additional
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
13. Verhogen van testdeelname tijdens de pilot grootschalig testen in de gemeente Dronten en gemeente Bunschoten
- Author
-
Sanders, J, Zomer, C, Hoekstra, R, de Ron, J, Blanken, T, Epskamp S, van Dijken, S, Gerkema, M, Hart, L, Visser, O, Borsboom, D, and de Bruin, M
- Subjects
RIVM rapport 2021-0089 - Abstract
Het ministerie van VWS neemt verschillende maatregelen om de verspreiding van het coronavirus (SARS-CoV-2) onder controle te krijgen. Zo is het belangrijk dat mensen zich laten testen bij klachten of na contact met een besmet persoon. Een onderdeel van de aanpak van VWS is dat grote groepen mensen zich meerdere keren laten testen, ook als ze geen klachten hebben (grootschalig testen). Hiervoor kunnen bijvoorbeeld alle inwoners van een dorp of buurt worden uitgenodigd als er daar veel mensen besmet zijn. VWS wil weten welke omstandigheden stimuleren dat zoveel mogelijk mensen zich laten testen. In twee gemeenten – Dronten en Bunschoten - is onderzocht welk effect twee factoren hebben op het aantal mensen dat zich laat testen en hoe vaak ze dat doen: de afstand tot een testlocatie en de informatie in een uitnodigingsbrief. Het blijkt dat meer mensen zich laten testen als de testlocatie dichtbij is, op minder dan 2 kilometer van hun woning. Ook laten ze zich dan vaker testen. Het effect van de informatie in de uitnodigingsbrieven is niet goed te bepalen. Dat komt omdat er naast de brieven ook in de media (radio, sociale media, reclameborden) veel aandacht aan de pilots is besteed. In beide gemeenten zijn twee soorten brieven gestuurd met een verschillende inhoud. Een deel van de inwoners ontving een brief met alleen basisinformatie over grootschalig testen. Het andere deel ontving een brief waarin de basisinformatie werd aangevuld met informatie over het risico op een besmetting met het coronavirus en de verspreiding ervan. De opkomst was bij beide groepen inwoners bijna even hoog. De Corona Gedragsunit van het RIVM heeft dit onderzoek in samenwerking met de Universiteit van Amsterdam, GGD’en en gemeenten uitgevoerd.
- Published
- 2021
14. Mental health and social contact during the COVID-19 pandemic
- Author
-
Fried, E.I., Papanikolaou, F., and Epskamp, S.
- Published
- 2021
15. Exploring the underlying structure of mental disorders: cross-diagnostic differences and similarities from a network perspective using both a top-down and a bottom-up approach
- Author
-
Wigman, J. T. W., van Os, J., Borsboom, D., Wardenaar, K. J., Epskamp, S., Klippel, A., Viechtbauer, W., Myin-Germeys, I., and Wichers, M.
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
16. Network analysis of multivariate data in psychological science
- Author
-
Borsboom, D, Deserno, M, Rhemtulla, M, Epskamp, S, Fried, E, Mcnally, R, Robinaugh, D, Perugini, M, Dalege, J, Costantini, G, Isvoranu, A, Wysocki, A, van Borkulo, C, van Bork, R, Waldorp, L, Borsboom, Denny, Deserno, Marie K., Rhemtulla, Mijke, Epskamp, Sacha, Fried, Eiko I., McNally, Richard J., Robinaugh, Donald J., Perugini, Marco, Dalege, Jonas, Costantini, Giulio, Isvoranu, Adela-Maria, Wysocki, Anna C., van Borkulo, Claudia D., van Bork, Riet, Waldorp, Lourens J., Borsboom, D, Deserno, M, Rhemtulla, M, Epskamp, S, Fried, E, Mcnally, R, Robinaugh, D, Perugini, M, Dalege, J, Costantini, G, Isvoranu, A, Wysocki, A, van Borkulo, C, van Bork, R, Waldorp, L, Borsboom, Denny, Deserno, Marie K., Rhemtulla, Mijke, Epskamp, Sacha, Fried, Eiko I., McNally, Richard J., Robinaugh, Donald J., Perugini, Marco, Dalege, Jonas, Costantini, Giulio, Isvoranu, Adela-Maria, Wysocki, Anna C., van Borkulo, Claudia D., van Bork, Riet, and Waldorp, Lourens J.
- Abstract
In recent years, network analysis has been applied to identify and analyse patterns of statistical association in multivariate psychological data. In these approaches, network nodes represent variables in a data set, and edges represent pairwise conditional associations between variables in the data, while conditioning on the remaining variables. This Primer provides an anatomy of these techniques, describes the current state of the art and discusses open problems. We identify relevant data structures in which network analysis may be applied: cross-sectional data, repeated measures and intensive longitudinal data. We then discuss the estimation of network structures in each of these cases, as well as assessment techniques to evaluate network robustness and replicability. Successful applications of the technique in different research areas are highlighted. Finally, we discuss limitations and challenges for future research.
- Published
- 2021
17. Transdiagnostic symptom dynamics during psychotherapy.
- Author
-
O'Driscoll, C., Epskamp, S., Fried, E. I., Saunders, R., Cardoso, A., Stott, J., Wheatley, J., Cirkovic, M., Naqvi, S. A., Buckman, J. E. J., and Pilling, S.
- Subjects
- *
ANXIETY , *PSYCHOTHERAPY , *SYMPTOMS , *MENTAL illness , *PATIENTS' attitudes , *MENTAL depression - Abstract
Psychotherapy is an effective treatment for many common mental health problems, but the mechanisms of action and processes of change are unclear, perhaps driven by the focus on a single diagnosis which does not reflect the heterogeneous symptom experiences of many patients. The objective of this study was to better understand therapeutic change, by illustrating how symptoms evolve and interact during psychotherapy. Data from 113,608 patients from psychological therapy services who completed depression and anxiety symptom measures across three to six therapy sessions were analysed. A panel graphical vector-autoregression model was estimated in a model development sample (N = 68,165) and generalizability was tested in a confirmatory model, fitted to a separate (hold-out) sample of patients (N = 45,443). The model displayed an excellent fit and replicated in the confirmatory holdout sample. First, we found that nearly all symptoms were statistically related to each other (i.e. dense connectivity), indicating that no one symptom or association drives change. Second, the structure of symptom interrelations which emerged did not change across sessions. These findings provide a dynamic view of the process of symptom change during psychotherapy and give rise to several causal hypotheses relating to structure, mechanism, and process. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
18. Psychological Networks in Clinical Populations: A tutorial on the consequences of Berksons Bias
- Author
-
Epskamp S, de Ron J, and Fried Ei
- Subjects
PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences ,bepress|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Psychology|Clinical Psychology ,bepress|Social and Behavioral Sciences ,bepress|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Psychology|Quantitative Psychology ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Quantitative Methods ,Psychology ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Clinical Psychology ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
In clinical research, populations are often selected on the sum-score of diagnostic criteria such as symptoms. Estimating statistical models where a subset of the data is selected based on a function of the analyzed variables introduces Berkson’s bias, which presents a potential threat to the validity of findings in the clinical literature. The aim of the present paper is to investigate the effect of Berkson’s bias on the performance of the two most commonly used psychological network models: the Gaussian Graphical Model (GGM) for continuous and ordinal data, and the Ising Model for binary data. In two simulation studies, we test how well the two models recover a true network structure when estimation is based on a subset of the data typically seen in clinical studies. The network is based on a dataset of 2,807 patients diagnosed with major depression, and nodes in the network are items from the Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression (HRSD). The simulation studies test different scenarios by varying (1) sample size and (2) the cut-off value of the sum-score which governs the selection of participants. The results of both studies indicate that higher cut-off values are associated with worse recovery of the network structure. As expected from the Berkson’s bias literature, selection reduced recovery rates by inducing negative connections between the items. Our findings provide evidence that Berkson’s bias is a considerable and underappreciated problem in the clinical network literature. Furthermore, we discuss potential solutions to circumvent Berkson’s bias and their pitfalls.
- Published
- 2019
19. The dynamics in Health-Related Quality of Life of patients with Stable Coronary Artery Disease were revealed: a network analysis
- Author
-
Oreel, T.H., Boorsboom, D., Epskamp, S., Hartog, I.D., Netjes, J.E., Nieuwkerk, P., Henriques, J.P.S., Scherer - Rath, M., Laarhoven, H.W. van, Sprangers, M.A.G., Oreel, T.H., Boorsboom, D., Epskamp, S., Hartog, I.D., Netjes, J.E., Nieuwkerk, P., Henriques, J.P.S., Scherer - Rath, M., Laarhoven, H.W. van, and Sprangers, M.A.G.
- Abstract
08 december 2018, Contains fulltext : 200246pub.pdf (publisher's version ) (Open Access), Objective: Health-related quality of life (HRQoL) is a dynamic construct. Experience sampling methods (ESM) are becoming increasingly popular to capture within-person fluctuations in HRQoL. An emerging approach to analyze such momentary data is network analysis. Our aim was to explore the use of network analysis for investigating the dynamics within individual’s HRQoL. Study Design and Setting: We analyzed ESM data of 30 patients with stable coronary artery disease (CAD). Patients completed eight HRQoL items representing four scales (i.e., positive mood, negative mood, CAD symptoms, and physical state) at nine times a day for seven consecutive days. Network analysis was used to analyze the data at group level to estimate the average HRQoL dynamics and at patient level to estimate HRQoL dynamics of individual patients. Results: Group-level analysis showed that, on average, feeling ‘‘tired’’ and feeling ‘‘anxious’’ are the most central items in patients’ HRQoL. Patient-level analysis revealed differences in patients’ network structures, indicating within-person differences in HRQoL dynamics. Conclusion: This study is one of the first to apply network analysis to momentary HRQoL data. To the extent that network models are meaningful representations of HRQoL dynamics, they may help deepening our insight into experienced HRQoL and provide targets for personalized treatment.
- Published
- 2019
20. Bayesian inference for psychology. Part II: Example applications with JASP
- Author
-
Wagenmakers, E.-J. (Eric-Jan), Love, J. (Jonathon), Marsman, M. (Maarten), Jamil, T. (Tahira), Ly, A. (Alexander), Verhagen, J.H. (Josine), Selker, R. (Ravi), Gronau, Q. F. (Quentin), Dropmann, D. (Damian), Boutin, B. (Bruno), Meerhoff, F. (Frans), Knight, P. (Patrick), Raj, A. (Akash), Kesteren, E.-J. (Erik-Jan), Doorn, J. (Johnny) van, Šmíra, M. (Martin), Epskamp, S. (Sacha), Etz, A. (Alexander), Matzke, D. (Dora), Jong, T. (Tim) de, Bergh, D. (Don) van den, Sarafoglou, A. (Alexandra), Steingroever, H. (Helen), Derks, K. (Koen), Rouder, J. (Jeffrey), Morey, R.D. (Richard), Wagenmakers, E.-J. (Eric-Jan), Love, J. (Jonathon), Marsman, M. (Maarten), Jamil, T. (Tahira), Ly, A. (Alexander), Verhagen, J.H. (Josine), Selker, R. (Ravi), Gronau, Q. F. (Quentin), Dropmann, D. (Damian), Boutin, B. (Bruno), Meerhoff, F. (Frans), Knight, P. (Patrick), Raj, A. (Akash), Kesteren, E.-J. (Erik-Jan), Doorn, J. (Johnny) van, Šmíra, M. (Martin), Epskamp, S. (Sacha), Etz, A. (Alexander), Matzke, D. (Dora), Jong, T. (Tim) de, Bergh, D. (Don) van den, Sarafoglou, A. (Alexandra), Steingroever, H. (Helen), Derks, K. (Koen), Rouder, J. (Jeffrey), and Morey, R.D. (Richard)
- Abstract
Bayesian hypothesis testing presents an attractive alternative to p value hypothesis testing. Part I of this series outlined several advantages of Bayesian hypothesis testing, including the ability to quantify evidence and the ability to monitor and update this evidence as data come in, without the need to know the intention with which the data were collected. Despite these and other practical advantages, Bayesian hypothesis tests are still reported relatively rarely. An important impediment to the widespread adoption of Bayesian tests is arguably the lack of user-friendly software for the run-of-the-mill statistical problems that confront psychologists for the analysis of almost every experiment: the t -test, ANOVA, correlation, regression, and contingency tables. In Part II of this series we introduce JASP (www.jasp-stats.org), an open-source, cross-platform, user-friendly graphical software package that allows users to carry out Bayesian hypothesis tests for standard statistical problems. JASP is based in part on the Bayesian analysesimplemented in Morey and Rouder’s BayesFactor package for R. Armed with JASP, the practical advantages of Bayesian hypothesis testing are only a mouse click away.
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
21. JASP: Graphical statistical software for common statistical designs
- Author
-
Love, J. (Jonathon), Selker, R. (Ravi), Marsman, M. (Maarten), Jamil, T. (Tahira), Dropmann, D. (Damian), Verhagen, J.H. (Josine), Ly, A. (Alexander), Gronau, Q. F. (Quentin), Šmíra, M. (Martin), Epskamp, S. (Sacha), Matzke, D. (Dora), Wild, A., Knight, P. (Patrick), Rouder, J. (Jeffrey), Morey, R.D. (Richard), Wagenmakers, E.-J. (Eric-Jan), Love, J. (Jonathon), Selker, R. (Ravi), Marsman, M. (Maarten), Jamil, T. (Tahira), Dropmann, D. (Damian), Verhagen, J.H. (Josine), Ly, A. (Alexander), Gronau, Q. F. (Quentin), Šmíra, M. (Martin), Epskamp, S. (Sacha), Matzke, D. (Dora), Wild, A., Knight, P. (Patrick), Rouder, J. (Jeffrey), Morey, R.D. (Richard), and Wagenmakers, E.-J. (Eric-Jan)
- Abstract
This paper introduces JASP, a free graphical software package for basic statistical procedures such as t tests, ANOVAs, linear regression models, and analyses of contingency tables. JASP is open-source and differentiates itself from existing open-source solutions in two ways. First, JASP provides several innovations in user interface design; specifically, results are provided immediately as the user makes changes to options, output is attractive, minimalist, and designed around the principle of progressive disclosure, and analyses can be peer reviewed without requiring a “syntax”. Second, JASP provides some of the recent developments in Bayesian hypothesis testing and Bayesian parameter estimation. The ease with which these relatively complex Bayesian techniques are available in JASP encourages their broader adoption and furthers a more inclusive statistical reporting practice. The JASP analyses are implemented in R and a series of R packages. © 2019, American Statistical Association. All rights reserved.
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
22. Stability and variability of personality networks. A tutorial on recent developments in network psychometrics
- Author
-
Costantini, G, Richetin, J, Preti, E, Casini, E, Epskamp, S, Perugini, M, COSTANTINI, GIULIO, RICHETIN, JULIETTE, PRETI, EMANUELE, CASINI, ERICA, PERUGINI, MARCO, Costantini, G, Richetin, J, Preti, E, Casini, E, Epskamp, S, Perugini, M, COSTANTINI, GIULIO, RICHETIN, JULIETTE, PRETI, EMANUELE, CASINI, ERICA, and PERUGINI, MARCO
- Abstract
Networks have been recently proposed for modeling dynamics in several kinds of psychological phenomena, such as personality and psychopathology. In this work, we introduce techniques that allow disentangling between-subject networks, which encode dynamics that involve stable individual differences, from within-subject networks, which encode dynamics that involve momentary levels of certain individual characteristics. Furthermore, we show how networks can be simultaneously estimated in separate groups of individuals, using a technique called the Fused Graphical Lasso. This technique allows also performing meaningful comparisons among groups. The unique properties of each kind of network are discussed. A tutorial to implement these techniques in the "R" statistical software is presented, together with an example of application
- Published
- 2019
23. The dynamics of Borderline Personality Disorder
- Author
-
Costantini, G, Alì, P, Di Pierro, R, Richetin, J, Preti, E, Epskamp, S, Costantini, Giulio, Alì, Paolo Alessandro, Di Pierro, Rossella, Richetin, Juliette, Preti, Emanuele, Epskamp, Sacha., Costantini, G, Alì, P, Di Pierro, R, Richetin, J, Preti, E, Epskamp, S, Costantini, Giulio, Alì, Paolo Alessandro, Di Pierro, Rossella, Richetin, Juliette, Preti, Emanuele, and Epskamp, Sacha.
- Abstract
Recent reasoning in psychopathology sees mental disorders as phenomena that emerge, at least in part, from complex patterns of symptom-symptom interactions. Borderline Personality Disorder (BPD) is characterized by instability of self-image, interpersonal relationships, and affects, as well as by marked impulsivity. In this study, we investigated the dynamical interplay of BPD symptoms over time. We employed an Ecological Momentary Assessment (EMA) protocol assessing BPD symptoms five times a day for 31 days in a sample of 156 participants. Participants also completed a comprehensive assessment of personality, including BPD, before the EMA, after the EMA, and one month later. We examined BPD symptoms at two timescales: The short scale of EMA and the long scale offered by the three questionnaires. At the short timescale, we employed a multilevel vector auto-regressive network model, which provided insights into the contemporaneous and cross-lagged relationships among symptoms. At the long timescale, we observed a decrease in all BPD symptoms immediately after EMA and after one month, suggesting beneficial effects of the self-reflection induced by EMA. We discuss how each analysis gave unique insights into the dynamics of BPD.
- Published
- 2019
24. The Fused Graphical Lasso for computing psychological networks
- Author
-
Costantini, G, Epskamp, S, Costantini, G, and Epskamp, S
- Subjects
M-PSI/03 - PSICOMETRIA ,Network analysis, fused graphical lasso, Gaussian graphical model - Abstract
Networks have been recently proposed as plausible models of psychological phenomena in several domains, such as personality psychology and psychopathology. In these fields, nodes represent variables such as cognitions, behaviors, emotions, motivations, and symptoms, and edges represent their pairwise associations. Edge weights are typically estimated using regularized partial correlations, for instance via the graphical lasso. In several situations, it is necessary to compute networks on observations that belong to different classes (e.g., patients vs. controls). Previous studies estimated either a single network for all classes or several networks in each class independently. These strategies may be both suboptimal. The Fused Graphical Lasso (FGL) has been recently proposed for dealing with such situations (Danaher et al., 2014), but it has never been applied to psychology before. FGL allows simultaneously estimating multiple partial correlation networks from observations belonging to different classes. FGL does not assume that the networks are similar, but if similarities are present, they are exploited to improve parameter estimates. This method requires setting two tuning parameters: One is akin to the graphical lasso parameter and controls sparsity, the second one controls the amount of similarity among classes. We developed an R package that implements automatic tuning parameter selection according to information criteria (AIC, BIC, and extended BIC) or relying on k-fold crossvalidation. We present FGL from a theoretical point of view, discuss its performance in simulation studies, and show examples of its applications to personality psychology and psychopathology.
- Published
- 2017
25. From loss to loneliness: The relationship between bereavement and depressive symptoms
- Author
-
Fried, E. I., Bockting, C. L. H., Arjadi, R., Borsboom, D., Amshoff, M., Cramer, A. O. J., Epskamp, S., Tuerlinckx, F., Carr, D., Stroebe, M., Trauma and Grief, Leerstoel Bockting, Clinical Psychology and Experimental Psychopathology, Adult Psychiatry, APH - Mental Health, Amsterdam Neuroscience - Mood, Anxiety, Psychosis, Stress & Sleep, Psychologische Methodenleer (Psychologie, FMG), Trauma and Grief, and Leerstoel Bockting
- Subjects
Male ,medicine.medical_specialty ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Poison control ,Affect (psychology) ,Suicide prevention ,Social network analyses ,Injury prevention ,80 and over ,loneliness ,medicine ,Humans ,Psychiatry ,Biological Psychiatry ,Aged ,media_common ,Aged, 80 and over ,bereavement ,Loneliness/psychology ,Loneliness ,Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale ,Sadness ,Clinical Psychology ,Psychiatry and Mental health ,depression ,Female ,Grief ,medicine.symptom ,Psychology ,Depression/psychology ,Follow-Up Studies ,Clinical psychology - Abstract
Spousal bereavement can cause a rise in depressive symptoms. This study empirically evaluates 2 competing explanations concerning how this causal effect is brought about: (a) a traditional latent variable explanation, in which loss triggers depression which then leads to symptoms; and (b) a novel network explanation, in which bereavement directly affects particular depression symptoms which then activate other symptoms. We used data from the Changing Lives of Older Couples (CLOC) study and compared depressive symptomatology, assessed via the 11-item Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D), among those who lost their partner (N = 241) with a still-married control group (N = 274). We modeled the effect of partner loss on depressive symptoms either as an indirect effect through a latent variable, or as a direct effect in a network constructed through a causal search algorithm. Compared to the control group, widow(er)s' scores were significantly higher for symptoms of loneliness, sadness, depressed mood, and appetite loss, and significantly lower for happiness and enjoyed life. The effect of partner loss on these symptoms was not mediated by a latent variable. The network model indicated that bereavement mainly affected loneliness, which in turn activated other depressive symptoms. The direct effects of spousal loss on particular symptoms are inconsistent with the predictions of latent variable models, but can be explained from a network perspective. The findings support a growing body of literature showing that specific adverse life events differentially affect depressive symptomatology, and suggest that future studies should examine interventions that directly target such symptoms. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2015 APA, all rights reserved).
- Published
- 2015
26. Critical Slowing Down as a Personalized Early Warning Signal for Depression
- Author
-
Wichers, Marieke, Groot, PC, Borsboom, D, Cramer, AOJ, Epskamp, S, Kendler, KS, van der Maas, HLJ, Tuerlinckx, Francis, Wigman, JTW, Delespaul, P, Peeters, F, Simons, CJP, Snippe, E, van de Leemput, IA, Scheffer, M, RS: MHeNs - R2 - Mental Health, Psychiatrie & Neuropsychologie, Interdisciplinary Centre Psychopathology and Emotion regulation (ICPE), and Psychologische Methodenleer (Psychologie, FMG)
- Subjects
050103 clinical psychology ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Warning system ,05 social sciences ,General Medicine ,Variance (accounting) ,Tipping point (climatology) ,Correlation ,03 medical and health sciences ,Psychiatry and Mental health ,Clinical Psychology ,0302 clinical medicine ,Mood ,Healthy individuals ,medicine ,ComputingMethodologies_DOCUMENTANDTEXTPROCESSING ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Falling (sensation) ,Psychiatry ,Psychology ,GeneralLiterature_REFERENCE(e.g.,dictionaries,encyclopedias,glossaries) ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Applied Psychology ,Depression (differential diagnoses) ,Clinical psychology - Abstract
About 17% of humanity goes through an episode of major depression at some point in their lifetime. Despite the enormous societal costs of this incapacitating disorder, it is largely unknown how the likelihood of falling into a depressive episode can be assessed. Here, we show for a large group of healthy individuals and patients that the probability of an upcoming shift between a depressed and a normal state is related to elevated temporal autocorrelation, variance, and correlation between emotions in fluctuations of autorecorded emotions. These are indicators of the general phenomenon of critical slowing down, which is expected to occur when a system approaches a tipping point. Our results support the hypothesis that mood may have alternative stable states separated by tipping points, and suggest an approach for assessing the likelihood of transitions into and out of depression.
- Published
- 2016
27. Network psychometrics
- Author
-
Epskamp, S., Borsboom, Denny, Waldorp, Lourens, Psychologische Methodenleer (Psychologie, FMG), and FMG
- Abstract
In recent years, research on dynamical systems in psychology has emerged, which is analogous to other fields such as biology and physics. One popular and promising line of research involves the modeling of psychological systems as causal systems or networks of cellular automat. The general hypothesis is that noticeable macroscopic behavior—the co-occurrence of aspects of psychology such as cognitive abilities, psychopathological symptoms, or behavior—is not due to the influence of unobserved common causes, such as general intelligence, psychopathological disorders, or personality traits, but rather to emergent behavior in a network of interacting psychological, sociological, biological, and other components. This dissertation concerns the estimation of such psychological networks from datasets. While this line of research originated from a dynamical systems perspective, the developed methods have shown strong utility as exploratory data analysis tools, highlighting unique variance between variables rather than shared variance across variables (e.g., factor analysis). In addition, this dissertation shows that network modeling and latent variable modeling are closely related and can complement one-another. The methods are thus widely applicable in diverse fields of psychological research. To this end, the dissertation is split in three parts. Part I is aimed at empirical researchers with an emphasis on clinical psychology, and introduces the methods in conceptual terms and tutorials. Part II is aimed at psychometricians and methodologists, and discusses the methods in technical terms. Finally, Part III is aimed at R users with an emphasis on personality research.
- Published
- 2017
28. Mapping the manuals of madness: Comparing the ICD-10 and DSM-IV-TR using a network approach
- Author
-
Tio, P., Epskamp, S., Noordhof, A., Borsboom, D., Psychologische Methodenleer (Psychologie, FMG), Klinische Psychologie (Psychologie, FMG), and Department of Methodology and Statistics
- Abstract
The International Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems (ICD) and the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM) represent dominant approaches to diagnosis of mental disorders. However, it is unclear how these alternative systems relate to each other when taking into account the symptoms that make up the disorders. This study uses a network approach to investigate the overlap in structure between diagnostic networks pertaining to ICD-10 and DSM-IV-TR. Networks are constructed by representing individual symptoms as nodes, and connecting nodes whenever the corresponding symptoms feature as diagnostic criteria for the same mental disorder. Results indicate that, relative to the DSM-IV-TR network, the ICD-10 network contains (a) more nodes, (b) lower level of clustering, and (c) a higher level of connectivity. Both networks show features of a small world, and have similar (of “the same”) high centrality nodes. Comparison to empirical data indicates that the DSM-IV-TR network structure follows comorbidity rates more closely than the ICD-10 network structure. We conclude that, despite their apparent likeness, ICD-10 and DSM-IV-TR harbour important structural differences, and that both may be improved by matching diagnostic categories more closely to empirically determined network structures.
- Published
- 2016
29. Bayesian inference for psychology. Part I: Theoretical advantages and practical ramifications
- Author
-
Wagenmakers, E.-J. (Eric-Jan), Marsman, M. (Maarten), Jamil, T. (Tahira), Ly, A. (Alexander), Verhagen, J.H. (Josine), Love, J. (Jonathon), Selker, R. (Ravi), Gronau, Q. F. (Quentin), Smıra, M. (Martin), Epskamp, S. (Sacha), Matzke, D. (Dora), Rouder, J. (Jeffrey), Morey, R.D. (Richard), Wagenmakers, E.-J. (Eric-Jan), Marsman, M. (Maarten), Jamil, T. (Tahira), Ly, A. (Alexander), Verhagen, J.H. (Josine), Love, J. (Jonathon), Selker, R. (Ravi), Gronau, Q. F. (Quentin), Smıra, M. (Martin), Epskamp, S. (Sacha), Matzke, D. (Dora), Rouder, J. (Jeffrey), and Morey, R.D. (Richard)
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
30. Network Psychometrics
- Author
-
Epskamp, S., Maris, G., Waldorp, L.J., Borsboom, D., Irwing, P., Booth, T., Hughes, D.J., Psychologische Methodenleer (Psychologie, FMG), and FMG
- Subjects
Methodology (stat.ME) ,FOS: Computer and information sciences ,010104 statistics & probability ,Statistics::Applications ,05 social sciences ,050109 social psychology ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Computer Science::Human-Computer Interaction ,0101 mathematics ,01 natural sciences ,Statistics - Methodology - Abstract
This chapter provides a general introduction of network modeling in psychometrics. The chapter starts with an introduction to the statistical model formulation of pairwise Markov random fields (PMRF), followed by an introduction of the PMRF suitable for binary data: the Ising model. The Ising model is a model used in ferromagnetism to explain phase transitions in a field of particles. Following the description of the Ising model in statistical physics, the chapter continues to show that the Ising model is closely related to models used in psychometrics. The Ising model can be shown to be equivalent to certain kinds of logistic regression models, loglinear models and multi-dimensional item response theory (MIRT) models. The equivalence between the Ising model and the MIRT model puts standard psychometrics in a new light and leads to a strikingly different interpretation of well-known latent variable models. The chapter gives an overview of methods that can be used to estimate the Ising model, and concludes with a discussion on the interpretation of latent variables given the equivalence between the Ising model and MIRT., In Irwing, P., Hughes, D., and Booth, T. (2018). The Wiley Handbook of Psychometric Testing, 2 Volume Set: A Multidisciplinary Reference on Survey, Scale and Test Development. New York: Wiley
- Published
- 2016
31. Regularized Gaussian Psychological Networks: Brief Report on the Performance of Extended BIC Model Selection
- Author
-
Epskamp, S., Psychologische Methodenleer (Psychologie, FMG), and FMG
- Abstract
In recent literature, the Gaussian Graphical model (GGM; Lauritzen, 1996), a network of partial correlation coefficients, has been used to capture potential dynamic relationships between psychological variables. The GGM can be estimated using regularization in combination with model selection using the extended Bayesian Information Criterion (Foygel and Drton, 2010). I term this methodology GeLasso, and asses its performance using a plausible psychological network structure with both continuous and ordinal datasets. Simulation results indicate that GeLasso works well as an out-of-the-box method to estimate a psychological network structure.
- Published
- 2016
32. Encouraging vaccination behavior through online social media
- Author
-
Langley, D.J., Wijn, R., Epskamp, S., and Bork, R. van
- Subjects
Urban Mobility & Environment Human & Operational Modelling ,Informatics ,ELSS - Earth, Life and Social Sciences ,SBA - Strategic Business Analysis HOI - Human Behaviour & Organisational Innovations - Published
- 2016
33. An Introduction to Network Psychometrics: Relating Ising Network Models to Item Response Theory Models
- Author
-
Marsman, M., primary, Borsboom, D., additional, Kruis, J., additional, Epskamp, S., additional, van Bork, R., additional, Waldorp, L. J., additional, Maas, H. L. J. van der, additional, and Maris, G., additional
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
34. Software to sharpen your stats
- Author
-
Love, J., Selker, R., Verhagen, J., Marsman, M., Gronau, Q.F., Jamil, T., Smira, M., Epskamp, S., Wild, A., Ly, A., Matzke, D., Wagenmakers, E.-J., Morey, R.D., Rouder, J.N., and Psychologische Methodenleer (Psychologie, FMG)
- Abstract
While there is a call for the use of novel statistical inference methods in the social sciences, the scientific community is slow in adapting to using such statistical methods. A possible reason for this is that state-of-the-art statistical methods are mostly implemented in the statistical programming language R whereas empirical social scientists more often rely on propitiatory statistical software such as SPSS. We introduce JASP, an graphical environment for statistical analyses that combines the strength of R, open-source and state-of-the-art statistical methods, with the flexibility and accessibility of proprietary statistical software. JASP provides a rich graphical user interface and implements a wide range of classical as well as Bayesian analyses. JASP is freely available for Windows, Mac OS X and Linux under an open source license from https://jasp-stats.org.
- Published
- 2015
35. Should I Get That Jab? : Exploring Influence To Encourage Vaccination Via Online Social Media
- Author
-
Langley, D.J., Wijn, R., Epskamp, S., Bork, R. van, Psychologische Methodenleer (Psychologie, FMG), and SOM I&O
- Subjects
Informatics ,SBA - Strategic Business Analysis ,Urban Mobility & Environment ,Healthcare ,Social Influence ,ELSS - Earth, Life and Social Sciences ,Social Network Sites ,Network Analysis - Abstract
This paper explores the suitability of social media networks (SMNs) as a means of influencing the public’s decision-making process regarding vaccinations, specifically a vaccination to protect girls against HPV, a virus associated with cervical cancer. Parents of girls in the target cohort were invited to online discussion forums where they could discuss their opinions on the vaccination. We varied the posts on the forums in different experimental condition, such that they were exposed to promotion of the vaccination in one of four different ways, and coming from one of two different sources, i.e., peers or government health representatives. Following the health belief model (HBM), these messages served as cues to action. After their active participation on the forums, participants filled out a ques-tionnaire with items related to the HBM. Analyses revealed no effect of our experimental manipula-tions of the cue to action. However, using an exploratory novel network analysis approach, we find that the HBM does not adequately account for influence via SMNs. Specifically we show that vaccina-tion decisions are not taken in social isolation, a fact thus far ignored by various forms of the HBM. Implications for studies assessing the use of online channels for health communication are discussed.
- Published
- 2015
36. State of the aRt personality research: A tutorial on network analysis of personality data in R
- Author
-
Costantini, G, Epskamp, S, Borsboom, D, Perugini, M, Mõttus, R, Waldorp, L, Cramer, A, COSTANTINI, GIULIO, PERUGINI, MARCO, Cramer, A., Costantini, G, Epskamp, S, Borsboom, D, Perugini, M, Mõttus, R, Waldorp, L, Cramer, A, COSTANTINI, GIULIO, PERUGINI, MARCO, and Cramer, A.
- Abstract
Network analysis represents a novel theoretical approach to personality. Network approaches motivate alternative ways of analyzing data, and suggest new ways of modeling and simulating personality processes. In the present paper, we provide an overview of network analysis strategies as they apply to personality data. We discuss different ways to construct networks from typical personality data, show how to compute and interpret important measures of centrality and clustering, and illustrate how one can simulate on networks to mimic personality processes. All analyses are illustrated using a data set on the commonly used HEXACO questionnaire using elementary R-code that readers may easily adapt to apply to their own data.
- Published
- 2015
37. From loss to loneliness: The relationship between bereavement and depressive symptoms
- Author
-
Trauma and Grief, Leerstoel Bockting, Fried, E. I., Bockting, C. L. H., Arjadi, R., Borsboom, D., Amshoff, M., Cramer, A. O. J., Epskamp, S., Tuerlinckx, F., Carr, D., Stroebe, M., Trauma and Grief, Leerstoel Bockting, Fried, E. I., Bockting, C. L. H., Arjadi, R., Borsboom, D., Amshoff, M., Cramer, A. O. J., Epskamp, S., Tuerlinckx, F., Carr, D., and Stroebe, M.
- Published
- 2015
38. Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science
- Author
-
Aarts, A, Anderson, J, Anderson, C, Attridge, P, Attwood, A, Axt, J, Babel, M, Bahník, Š, Baranski, E, Barnett Cowan, M, Bartmess, E, Beer, J, Bell, R, Bentley, H, Beyan, L, Binion, G, Borsboom, D, Bosch, A, Bosco, F, Bowman, S, Brandt, M, Braswell, E, Brohmer, H, Brown, B, Brown, K, Brüning, J, Calhoun Sauls, A, Callahan, S, Chagnon, E, Chandler, J, Chartier, C, Cheung, C, Cd, Cillessen, L, Clay, R, Cleary, H, Cloud, M, Cohn, M, Cohoon, J, Columbus, S, Cordes, A, Costantini, G, Cramblet Alvarez, L, Cremata, E, Crusius, J, Decoster, J, Degaetano, M, Della Penna, N, den Bezemer, B, Deserno, M, Devitt, O, Dewitte, L, Dobolyi, D, Dodson, G, Donnellan, M, Donohue, R, Dore, R, Dorrough, A, Dreber, A, Dugas, M, Dunn, E, Easey, K, Eboigbe, S, Eggleston, C, Embley, J, Epskamp, S, Errington, T, Estel, V, Farach, F, Feather, J, Fedor, A, Fernández Castilla, B, Fiedler, S, Field, J, Fitneva, S, Flagan, T, Forest, A, Forsell, E, Foster, J, Frank, M, Frazier, R, Fuchs, H, Gable, P, Galak, J, Galliani, E, Gampa, A, Garcia, S, Gazarian, D, Gilbert, E, Giner Sorolla, R, Glöckner, A, Goellner, L, Goh, J, Goldberg, R, Goodbourn, P, Gordon McKeon, S, Gorges, B, Gorges, J, Goss, J, Graham, J, Grange, J, Gray, J, Hartgerink, C, Hartshorne, J, Hasselman, F, Hayes, T, Heikensten, E, Henninger, F, Hodsoll, J, Holubar, T, Hoogendoorn, G, Humphries, D, Hung, C, Immelman, N, Irsik, V, Jahn, G, Jäkel, F, Jekel, M, Johannesson, M, Johnson, L, Johnson, D, Johnson, K, Johnston, W, Jonas, K, Joy Gaba, J, Kappes, H, Kelso, K, Kidwell, M, Kim, S, Kirkhart, M, Kleinberg, B, Kneževic, G, Kolorz, F, Kossakowski, J, Krause, R, Krijnen, J, Kuhlmann, T, Kunkels, Y, Kyc, M, Lai, C, Laique, A, Lakens, D, Lane, K, Lassetter, B, Lazarevic, L, Lebel, E, Lee, K, Lee, M, Lemm, K, Levitan, C, Lewis, M, Lin, L, Lin, S, Lippold, M, Loureiro, D, Luteijn, I, Mackinnon, S, Mainard, H, Marigold, D, Martin, D, Martinez, T, Masicampo, E, Matacotta, J, Mathur, M, May, M, Mechin, N, Mehta, P, Meixner, J, Melinger, A, Miller, J, Miller, M, Moore, K, Möschl, M, Motyl, M, Müller, S, Munafo, M, Neijenhuijs, K, Nervi, T, Nicolas, G, Nilsonne, G, Nosek, B, Nuijten, M, Olsson, C, Osborne, C, Ostkamp, L, Pavel, M, Penton Voak, I, Perna, O, Pernet, C, Perugini, M, Pipitone, N, Pitts, M, Plessow, F, Prenoveau, J, Rahal, R, Ratliff, K, Reinhard, D, Renkewitz, F, Ricker, A, Rigney, A, Rivers, A, Roebke, M, Rutchick, A, Ryan, R, Sahin, O, Saide, A, Sandstrom, G, Santos, D, Saxe, R, Schlegelmilch, R, Schmidt, K, Scholz, S, Seibel, L, Selterman, D, Shaki, S, Simpson, E, Sinclair, H, Skorinko, J, Slowik, A, Snyder, J, Soderberg, C, Sonnleitner, C, Spencer, N, Spies, J, Steegen, S, Stieger, S, Strohminger, N, Sullivan, G, Talhelm, T, Tapia, M, te Dorsthorst, A, Thomae, M, Thomas, S, Tio, P, Traets, F, Tsang, S, Tuerlinckx, F, Turchan, P, Valášek, M, van 't Veer, A, Van Aert, R, van Assen, M, van Bork, R, van de Ven, M, van den Bergh, D, van der Hulst, M, van Dooren, R, van Doorn, J, van Renswoude, D, van Rijn, H, Vanpaemel, W, Vásquez Echeverría, A, Vazquez, M, Velez, N, Vermue, M, Verschoor, M, Vianello, M, Voracek, M, Vuu, G, Wagenmakers, E, Weerdmeester, J, Welsh, A, Westgate, E, Wissink, J, Wood, M, Woods, A, Wright, E, Wu, S, Zeelenberg, M, Zuni, K, Aarts, AA, Anderson, JE, Anderson, CJ, Attridge, PR, Bosco, FA, Bowman, SD, Brandt, MJ, Brown, BT, Callahan, SP, Chartier, CR, Cheung, Christopherson, CD, Cloud, MD, COSTANTINI, GIULIO, Cramblet Alvarez, LD, DeCoster, J, DeGaetano, MA, Deserno, MK, Dobolyi, DG, Dodson, GT, Donnellan, MB, Dore, RA, Dunn, EW, Errington, TM, Farach, FJ, Field, JG, Fitneva, SA, Forest, AL, Foster, JD, Frank, MC, Frazier, RS, Galliani, EM, Goh, JX, Goodbourn, PT, Grange, JA, Humphries, DJ, Hung, COY, Irsik, VC, Johnson, LG, Johnson, DJ, Johnson, KM, Johnston, WJ, Joy Gaba, JA, Kappes, HB, Kidwell, MC, Kim, SK, Kolorz, FM, Kossakowski, JJ, Krause, RM, Kunkels, YK, Kyc, MM, Lai, CK, Lane, KA, Lazarevic, LB, LeBel, EP, Lee, KJ, Levitan, CA, Lin, Lin, Mainard, HN, Marigold, DC, Martin, DP, Masicampo, EJ, Miller, JK, Möschl,M, Müller, SM, Neijenhuijs, KI, Nosek, BA, Nuijten, MB, Penton Voak, IS, PERUGINI, MARCO, Prenoveau, JM, Rahal, RM, Ratliff, KA, Ricker, AA, Rivers, AM, Rutchick, AM, Ryan, RS, Sandstrom, GM, Selterman, DF, Simpson, EB, Sinclair, HC, Skorinko, JLM, Snyder, JS, Spies, JR, Sullivan, GB, Thomas, SL, van 't Veer, AE, van Renswoude, DR, Wagenmakers, EJ, Westgate, EC, Zuni, K., Aarts, A, Anderson, J, Anderson, C, Attridge, P, Attwood, A, Axt, J, Babel, M, Bahník, Š, Baranski, E, Barnett Cowan, M, Bartmess, E, Beer, J, Bell, R, Bentley, H, Beyan, L, Binion, G, Borsboom, D, Bosch, A, Bosco, F, Bowman, S, Brandt, M, Braswell, E, Brohmer, H, Brown, B, Brown, K, Brüning, J, Calhoun Sauls, A, Callahan, S, Chagnon, E, Chandler, J, Chartier, C, Cheung, C, Cd, Cillessen, L, Clay, R, Cleary, H, Cloud, M, Cohn, M, Cohoon, J, Columbus, S, Cordes, A, Costantini, G, Cramblet Alvarez, L, Cremata, E, Crusius, J, Decoster, J, Degaetano, M, Della Penna, N, den Bezemer, B, Deserno, M, Devitt, O, Dewitte, L, Dobolyi, D, Dodson, G, Donnellan, M, Donohue, R, Dore, R, Dorrough, A, Dreber, A, Dugas, M, Dunn, E, Easey, K, Eboigbe, S, Eggleston, C, Embley, J, Epskamp, S, Errington, T, Estel, V, Farach, F, Feather, J, Fedor, A, Fernández Castilla, B, Fiedler, S, Field, J, Fitneva, S, Flagan, T, Forest, A, Forsell, E, Foster, J, Frank, M, Frazier, R, Fuchs, H, Gable, P, Galak, J, Galliani, E, Gampa, A, Garcia, S, Gazarian, D, Gilbert, E, Giner Sorolla, R, Glöckner, A, Goellner, L, Goh, J, Goldberg, R, Goodbourn, P, Gordon McKeon, S, Gorges, B, Gorges, J, Goss, J, Graham, J, Grange, J, Gray, J, Hartgerink, C, Hartshorne, J, Hasselman, F, Hayes, T, Heikensten, E, Henninger, F, Hodsoll, J, Holubar, T, Hoogendoorn, G, Humphries, D, Hung, C, Immelman, N, Irsik, V, Jahn, G, Jäkel, F, Jekel, M, Johannesson, M, Johnson, L, Johnson, D, Johnson, K, Johnston, W, Jonas, K, Joy Gaba, J, Kappes, H, Kelso, K, Kidwell, M, Kim, S, Kirkhart, M, Kleinberg, B, Kneževic, G, Kolorz, F, Kossakowski, J, Krause, R, Krijnen, J, Kuhlmann, T, Kunkels, Y, Kyc, M, Lai, C, Laique, A, Lakens, D, Lane, K, Lassetter, B, Lazarevic, L, Lebel, E, Lee, K, Lee, M, Lemm, K, Levitan, C, Lewis, M, Lin, L, Lin, S, Lippold, M, Loureiro, D, Luteijn, I, Mackinnon, S, Mainard, H, Marigold, D, Martin, D, Martinez, T, Masicampo, E, Matacotta, J, Mathur, M, May, M, Mechin, N, Mehta, P, Meixner, J, Melinger, A, Miller, J, Miller, M, Moore, K, Möschl, M, Motyl, M, Müller, S, Munafo, M, Neijenhuijs, K, Nervi, T, Nicolas, G, Nilsonne, G, Nosek, B, Nuijten, M, Olsson, C, Osborne, C, Ostkamp, L, Pavel, M, Penton Voak, I, Perna, O, Pernet, C, Perugini, M, Pipitone, N, Pitts, M, Plessow, F, Prenoveau, J, Rahal, R, Ratliff, K, Reinhard, D, Renkewitz, F, Ricker, A, Rigney, A, Rivers, A, Roebke, M, Rutchick, A, Ryan, R, Sahin, O, Saide, A, Sandstrom, G, Santos, D, Saxe, R, Schlegelmilch, R, Schmidt, K, Scholz, S, Seibel, L, Selterman, D, Shaki, S, Simpson, E, Sinclair, H, Skorinko, J, Slowik, A, Snyder, J, Soderberg, C, Sonnleitner, C, Spencer, N, Spies, J, Steegen, S, Stieger, S, Strohminger, N, Sullivan, G, Talhelm, T, Tapia, M, te Dorsthorst, A, Thomae, M, Thomas, S, Tio, P, Traets, F, Tsang, S, Tuerlinckx, F, Turchan, P, Valášek, M, van 't Veer, A, Van Aert, R, van Assen, M, van Bork, R, van de Ven, M, van den Bergh, D, van der Hulst, M, van Dooren, R, van Doorn, J, van Renswoude, D, van Rijn, H, Vanpaemel, W, Vásquez Echeverría, A, Vazquez, M, Velez, N, Vermue, M, Verschoor, M, Vianello, M, Voracek, M, Vuu, G, Wagenmakers, E, Weerdmeester, J, Welsh, A, Westgate, E, Wissink, J, Wood, M, Woods, A, Wright, E, Wu, S, Zeelenberg, M, Zuni, K, Aarts, AA, Anderson, JE, Anderson, CJ, Attridge, PR, Bosco, FA, Bowman, SD, Brandt, MJ, Brown, BT, Callahan, SP, Chartier, CR, Cheung, Christopherson, CD, Cloud, MD, COSTANTINI, GIULIO, Cramblet Alvarez, LD, DeCoster, J, DeGaetano, MA, Deserno, MK, Dobolyi, DG, Dodson, GT, Donnellan, MB, Dore, RA, Dunn, EW, Errington, TM, Farach, FJ, Field, JG, Fitneva, SA, Forest, AL, Foster, JD, Frank, MC, Frazier, RS, Galliani, EM, Goh, JX, Goodbourn, PT, Grange, JA, Humphries, DJ, Hung, COY, Irsik, VC, Johnson, LG, Johnson, DJ, Johnson, KM, Johnston, WJ, Joy Gaba, JA, Kappes, HB, Kidwell, MC, Kim, SK, Kolorz, FM, Kossakowski, JJ, Krause, RM, Kunkels, YK, Kyc, MM, Lai, CK, Lane, KA, Lazarevic, LB, LeBel, EP, Lee, KJ, Levitan, CA, Lin, Lin, Mainard, HN, Marigold, DC, Martin, DP, Masicampo, EJ, Miller, JK, Möschl,M, Müller, SM, Neijenhuijs, KI, Nosek, BA, Nuijten, MB, Penton Voak, IS, PERUGINI, MARCO, Prenoveau, JM, Rahal, RM, Ratliff, KA, Ricker, AA, Rivers, AM, Rutchick, AM, Ryan, RS, Sandstrom, GM, Selterman, DF, Simpson, EB, Sinclair, HC, Skorinko, JLM, Snyder, JS, Spies, JR, Sullivan, GB, Thomas, SL, van 't Veer, AE, van Renswoude, DR, Wagenmakers, EJ, Westgate, EC, and Zuni, K.
- Abstract
Reproducibility is a defining feature of science, but the extent to which it characterizes current research is unknown. We conducted replications of 100 experimental and correlational studies published in three psychology journals using high-powered designs and original materials when available. Replication effects were half the magnitude of original effects, representing a substantial decline. Ninety-seven percent of original studies had statistically significant results. Thirty-six percent of replications had statistically significant results; 47% of original effect sizes were in the 95% confidence interval of the replication effect size; 39% of effects were subjectively rated to have replicated the original result; and if no bias in original results is assumed, combining original and replication results left 68% with statistically significant effects. Correlational tests suggest that replication success was better predicted by the strength of original evidence than by characteristics of the original and replication teams.
- Published
- 2015
39. An Introduction to Network Psychometrics: Relating Ising Network Models to Item Response Theory Models.
- Author
-
Marsman, M., Borsboom, D., Kruis, J., Epskamp, S., van Bork, R., Waldorp, L. J., Maas, H. L. J. van der, and Maris, G.
- Subjects
PSYCHOMETRICS ,ITEM response theory ,RASCH models - Abstract
In recent years, network models have been proposed as an alternative representation of psychometric constructs such as depression. In such models, the covariance between observables (e.g., symptoms like depressed mood, feelings of worthlessness, and guilt) is explained in terms of a pattern of causal interactions between these observables, which contrasts with classical interpretations in which the observables are conceptualized as the effects of a reflective latent variable. However, few investigations have been directed at the question how these different models relate to each other. To shed light on this issue, the current paper explores the relation between one of the most important network models—the Ising model from physics—and one of the most important latent variable models—the Item Response Theory (IRT) model from psychometrics. The Ising model describes the interaction between states of particles that are connected in a network, whereas the IRT model describes the probability distribution associated with item responses in a psychometric test as a function of a latent variable. Despite the divergent backgrounds of the models, we show a broad equivalence between them and also illustrate several opportunities that arise from this connection. [ABSTRACT FROM PUBLISHER]
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
40. Dual role of cAMP and involvement of both G-proteins and ras in regulation of ERK2 in Dictyostelium discoideum.
- Author
-
Knetsch, M. L., primary, Epskamp, S. J., additional, Schenk, P. W., additional, Wang, Y., additional, Segall, J. E., additional, and Snaar-Jagalska, B. E., additional
- Published
- 1996
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
41. Reply to ‘Critiques of network analysis of multivariate data in psychological science’
- Author
-
Denny Borsboom, Marie K. Deserno, Mijke Rhemtulla, Sacha Epskamp, Eiko I. Fried, Richard J. McNally, Donald J. Robinaugh, Marco Perugini, Jonas Dalege, Giulio Costantini, Adela-Maria Isvoranu, Anna C. Wysocki, Claudia D. van Borkulo, Riet van Bork, Lourens J. Waldorp, Psychologische Methodenleer (Psychologie, FMG), Klinische Psychologie (Psychologie, FMG), Urban Mental Health, Psychology Other Research (FMG), Borsboom, D, Deserno, M, Rhemtulla, M, Epskamp, S, Fried, E, Mcnally, R, Robinaugh, D, Perugini, M, Dalege, J, Costantini, G, Isvoranu, A, Wysocki, A, van Borkulo, C, van Bork, R, and Waldorp, L
- Subjects
model selection ,reliability ,psychometric ,network analysi ,General Medicine ,General Chemistry - Published
- 2022
42. Stability and variability of personality networks. A tutorial on recent developments in network psychometrics
- Author
-
Emanuele Preti, Sacha Epskamp, Giulio Costantini, Erica Casini, Marco Perugini, Juliette Richetin, Psychologische Methodenleer (Psychologie, FMG), Costantini, G, Richetin, J, Preti, E, Casini, E, Epskamp, S, and Perugini, M
- Subjects
Theoretical computer science ,Fused Graphical Lasso ,Psychometrics ,media_common.quotation_subject ,05 social sciences ,Stability (learning theory) ,050109 social psychology ,Network analysi ,ENCODE ,050105 experimental psychology ,Communion ,Agency ,Lasso (statistics) ,Statistics ,Centrality ,Personality ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Big Five personality traits ,Psychology ,General Psychology ,Network analysis ,media_common - Abstract
Networks have been recently proposed for modeling dynamics in several kinds of psychological phenomena, such as personality and psychopathology. In this work, we introduce techniques that allow disentangling between-subject networks, which encode dynamics that involve stable individual differences, from within-subject networks, which encode dynamics that involve momentary levels of certain individual characteristics. Furthermore, we show how networks can be simultaneously estimated in separate groups of individuals, using a technique called the Fused Graphical Lasso. This technique allows also performing meaningful comparisons among groups. The unique properties of each kind of network are discussed. A tutorial to implement these techniques in the “R” statistical software is presented, together with an example of application.
- Published
- 2019
43. Network analysis of multivariate data in psychological science
- Author
-
Marco Perugini, Lourens J. Waldorp, Anna C. Wysocki, Riet van Bork, Denny Borsboom, Mijke Rhemtulla, Eiko I. Fried, Giulio Costantini, Richard J. McNally, Sacha Epskamp, Marie K. Deserno, Donald J. Robinaugh, Adela-Maria Isvoranu, Jonas Dalege, Claudia D. van Borkulo, Borsboom, D, Deserno, M, Rhemtulla, M, Epskamp, S, Fried, E, Mcnally, R, Robinaugh, D, Perugini, M, Dalege, J, Costantini, G, Isvoranu, A, Wysocki, A, van Borkulo, C, van Bork, R, Waldorp, L, Psychologische Methodenleer (Psychologie, FMG), Klinische Psychologie (Psychologie, FMG), and Urban Mental Health
- Subjects
Estimation ,Multivariate statistics ,Computer science ,Psychological research ,psychometric ,personality psychology ,05 social sciences ,050109 social psychology ,General Medicine ,centrality ,computer.software_genre ,Data structure ,050105 experimental psychology ,General Biochemistry, Genetics and Molecular Biology ,Data set ,Robustness (computer science) ,network analysi ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Pairwise comparison ,Data mining ,computer ,Network analysis - Abstract
In recent years, network analysis has been applied to identify and analyse patterns of statistical association in multivariate psychological data. In these approaches, network nodes represent variables in a data set, and edges represent pairwise conditional associations between variables in the data, while conditioning on the remaining variables. This Primer provides an anatomy of these techniques, describes the current state of the art and discusses open problems. We identify relevant data structures in which network analysis may be applied: cross-sectional data, repeated measures and intensive longitudinal data. We then discuss the estimation of network structures in each of these cases, as well as assessment techniques to evaluate network robustness and replicability. Successful applications of the technique in different research areas are highlighted. Finally, we discuss limitations and challenges for future research. Network analysis allows the investigation of complex patterns and relationships by examining nodes and the edges connecting them. Borsboom et al. discuss the adoption of network analysis in psychological research.
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
44. Descriptive, predictive and explanatory personality research: Different goals, different approaches, but a shared need to move beyond the Big Few traits
- Author
-
René Mõttus, Dustin Wood, David M Condon, Mitja Back, Anna Baumert, Giulio Costantini, Sacha Epskamp, Samuel Greiff, Wendy Johnson, Aaron Lukaszewski, Aja Louise Murray, William Revelle, Aidan G.C. Wright, Tal Yarkoni, Matthias Ziegler, Johannes Zimmermann, Psychologische Methodenleer (Psychologie, FMG), Mottus, R, Wood, D, Condon, D, Back, M, Baumert, A, Costantini, G, Epskamp, S, Greiff, S, Johnson, W, Lukaszewski, A, Murray, A, Revelle, W, Wright, A, Yarkoni, T, Ziegler, M, and Zimmermann, J
- Subjects
ddc:150 ,personality ,150 Psychologie ,hierarchy ,prediction ,explanation ,cause - Abstract
We argue that it is useful to distinguish between three key goals of personality science—description, prediction and explanation—and that attaining them often requires different priorities and methodological approaches. We put forward specific recommendations such as publishing findings with minimum a priori aggregation and exploring the limits of predictive models without being constrained by parsimony and intuitiveness but instead maximizing out-of-sample predictive accuracy. We argue that naturally occurring variance in many decontextualized and multidetermined constructs that interest personality scientists may not have individual causes, at least as this term is generally understood and in ways that are human-interpretable, never mind intervenable. If so, useful explanations are narratives that summarize many pieces of descriptive findings rather than models that target individual cause–effect associations. By meticulously studying specific and contextualized behaviours, thoughts, feelings and goals, however, individual causes of variance may ultimately be identifiable, although such causal explanations will likely be far more complex, phenomenon-specific and person-specific than anticipated thus far. Progress in all three areas—description, prediction and explanation—requires higher dimensional models than the currently dominant ‘Big Few’ and supplementing subjective trait-ratings with alternative sources of information such as informant-reports and behavioural measurements. Developing a new generation of psychometric tools thus provides many immediate research opportunities. © 2020 European Association of Personality Psychology
- Published
- 2020
45. The dynamics of Borderline Personality Disorder
- Author
-
Costantini, Giulio, Alì, Paolo Alessandro, Di Pierro, Rossella, Richetin, Juliette, Preti, Emanuele, Epskamp, Sacha., Costantini, G, Alì, P, Di Pierro, R, Richetin, J, Preti, E, and Epskamp, S
- Subjects
M-PSI/03 - PSICOMETRIA ,Network analysis, Borderline Personality Disorder, multilevel vector autoregression, ecological momentary assessment - Abstract
Recent reasoning in psychopathology sees mental disorders as phenomena that emerge, at least in part, from complex patterns of symptom-symptom interactions. Borderline Personality Disorder (BPD) is characterized by instability of self-image, interpersonal relationships, and affects, as well as by marked impulsivity. In this study, we investigated the dynamical interplay of BPD symptoms over time. We employed an Ecological Momentary Assessment (EMA) protocol assessing BPD symptoms five times a day for 31 days in a sample of 156 participants. Participants also completed a comprehensive assessment of personality, including BPD, before the EMA, after the EMA, and one month later. We examined BPD symptoms at two timescales: The short scale of EMA and the long scale offered by the three questionnaires. At the short timescale, we employed a multilevel vector auto-regressive network model, which provided insights into the contemporaneous and cross-lagged relationships among symptoms. At the long timescale, we observed a decrease in all BPD symptoms immediately after EMA and after one month, suggesting beneficial effects of the self-reflection induced by EMA. We discuss how each analysis gave unique insights into the dynamics of BPD.
- Published
- 2019
46. State of the aRt personality research: A tutorial on network analysis of personality data in R
- Author
-
Denny Borsboom, Sacha Epskamp, René Mõttus, Angélique O. J. Cramer, Giulio Costantini, Lourens J. Waldorp, Marco Perugini, Psychologische Methodenleer (Psychologie, FMG), Costantini, G, Epskamp, S, Borsboom, D, Perugini, M, Mõttus, R, Waldorp, L, and Cramer, A
- Subjects
Psychology (all) ,Social Psychology ,Psychometrics ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Network analysi ,Data science ,Clustering ,Data set ,HEXACO ,Latent variable ,Centrality ,Personality ,Big Five personality traits ,Personality trait ,Construct (philosophy) ,Cluster analysis ,Psychology ,Social psychology ,Psychometric ,General Psychology ,Network analysis ,media_common - Abstract
Network analysis represents a novel theoretical approach to personality. Network approaches motivate alternative ways of analyzing data, and suggest new ways of modeling and simulating personality processes. In the present paper, we provide an overview of network analysis strategies as they apply to personality data. We discuss different ways to construct networks from typical personality data, show how to compute and interpret important measures of centrality and clustering, and illustrate how one can simulate on networks to mimic personality processes. All analyses are illustrated using a data set on the commonly used HEXACO questionnaire using elementary R-code that readers may easily adapt to apply to their own data.
- Published
- 2015
47. Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science
- Author
-
Yoram K. Kunkels, Dylan Selterman, Denise J. Humphries, Kristina G. Brown, David G. Dobolyi, David J. Johnson, Mark A. Roebke, Andy T. Woods, John Hodsoll, Marije van der Hulst, Alexander A. Aarts, Kim Kelso, Erin C. Westgate, James A. Grange, Jesse Chandler, Jenelle Feather, Annick Bosch, Olivia Devitt, Benjamin T. Brown, Megan M. Kyc, Štěpán Bahník, Alissa Melinger, Michael Conn, Rebecca S. Frazier, Marc Jekel, Sara Bowman, Michael J. Wood, Erica Baranski, Sining Wu, Milan Valášek, Anna E. Van't Veer, Jeanine L. M. Skorinko, Joeri Wissink, Sara Steegen, Michael C. Pitts, Douglas Gazarian, Steve N.H. Tsang, Matthew W. Kirkhart, Jennifer S. Beer, Nathali Immelman, Elizabeth Chagnon, Robbie C. M. van Aert, Maya B. Mathur, Magnus Johannesson, Joshua D. Foster, Frank J. Farach, Gandalf Nicolas, Ian S. Penton-Voak, Rebecca M. Goldberg, Sarah L. Thomas, Kathleen Schmidt, Stephanie C. Lin, Linda Cillessen, Belén Fernández-Castilla, Taru Flagan, René Schlegelmilch, Joanneke Weerdmeester, Cyril Pernet, Andreas Cordes, Onur Sahin, Jolanda J. Kossakowski, Samuel Shaki, David Santos, Sabine Scholz, Jeremy R. Gray, Frank Renkewitz, Key Jung Lee, Gillian M. Sandstrom, Marie K. Deserno, Melissa Vazquez, Ed Cremata, Rebecca Saxe, Manuela Thomae, Johannes M. Meixner, Emma Heikensten, Sylvia Eboigbe, Carmel A. Levitan, Natalia Vélez, James G. Field, Riet van Bork, Vivien Estel, Michèle B. Nuijten, Lin Lin, Kate M. Johnson, Bobby Den Bezemer, Jennifer A. Joy-Gaba, Francis Tuerlinckx, Frits Traets, Ilse Luteijn, Christopher R. Chartier, Denise C. Marigold, Denny Borsboom, Elizabeth Gilbert, Jeff Galak, Shannon P. Callahan, E. J. Masicampo, Thomas Talhelm, Chris H.J. Hartgerink, Patrick T. Goodbourn, Stephanie M. Müller, Taylor Nervi, Marcus Möschl, Katherine Moore, Wolf Vanpaemel, Seung K. Kim, Elizabeth Bartmess, Heather N. Mainard, Martin Voracek, Gea Hoogendoorn, Sean P. Mackinnon, Ryan Donohue, Kate A. Ratliff, Jin X. Goh, Anastasia E. Rigney, Andreas Glöckner, Marieke Vermue, Angela S. Attwood, Michelle A. DeGaetano, Nick Spencer, Heather Bentley, Nina Strohminger, Geneva T. Dodson, R. Nathan Pipitone, Hayley M. D. Cleary, Matt Motyl, Amanda L. Forest, Marcus R. Munafò, Marcel Zeelenberg, Susann Fiedler, Ann Calhoun-Sauls, Mallorie Miller, Anondah R. Saide, Ljiljana B. Lazarević, Hilmar Brohmer, Mallory C. Kidwell, Pranjal H. Mehta, Jessie Gorges, Russ Clay, Jeffrey R. Spies, Joanna E. Anderson, Johnny van Doorn, Ashley A. Ricker, Elizabeth W. Dunn, Erin L Braswell, Jamie DeCoster, Larissa Seibel, Matthias Lippold, Lutz Ostkamp, William B. Simpson, Cathy On-Ying Hung, Carina Sonnleitner, Emily M. Wright, Laura Dewitte, Koen Ilja Neijenhuijs, Tim Kuhlmann, Job Krijnen, Leah Beyan, Jesse Graham, Andrew M Rivers, Sacha Epskamp, Aamir Laique, Christopher J. Anderson, Peter Raymond Attridge, Eric-Jan Wagenmakers, Agnieszka Slowik, Michael C. Frank, Bryan Gorges, Alejandro Vásquez Echeverría, Gina Vuu, Giulio Costantini, Eskil Forsell, Michelangelo Vianello, Don van den Bergh, Anna Fedor, Courtney K. Soderberg, M. Brent Donnellan, Kayleigh E Easey, Shauna Gordon-McKeon, Raoul Bell, William J. Johnston, Brian A. Nosek, Ashlee Welsh, Melissa Lewis, Anna Dreber, Simon Columbus, Frank A. Bosco, Pia Tio, Joshua K. Hartshorne, Lars Goellner, Elisa Maria Galliani, Etienne P. Le Bel, Kellylynn Zuni, Olivia Perna, Kristi M. Lemm, Marco Perugini, Anniek M. te Dorsthorst, Hedderik van Rijn, Timothy M. Errington, Bennett Kleinberg, Vanessa C. Irsik, Frank Jäkel, Timothy Hayes, Mark Verschoor, Mark D. Cloud, Bethany Lassetter, Justin Goss, Paul J. Turchan, Gavin Brent Sullivan, Darren Loureiro, Jo Embley, Robert S. Ryan, Jovita Brüning, Jan Crusius, Joel S. Snyder, Larissa Gabrielle Johnson, Nicolás Delia Penna, Grace Binion, Calvin K. Lai, Gustav Nilsonne, Heather M. Fuchs, Angela Rachael Dorrough, Michelle Dugas, Johanna Cohoon, Minha Lee, Robert Krause, David Reinhard, Goran Knežević, Jason M. Prenoveau, Kristin A. Lane, Stanka A. Fitneva, Rima-Maria Rahal, Mathijs Van De Ven, Anup Gampa, Marcel A.L.M. van Assen, Jordan Axt, Felix Henninger, Misha Pavel, Daniel Lakens, Jeremy K. Miller, Sara García, Leslie Cramblet Alvarez, Colleen Osborne, Kai J. Jonas, Taylor Holubar, Stefan Stieger, Heather Barry Kappes, Felix Cheung, Daan R. van Renswoude, Catherine Olsson, Roel van Dooren, Tylar Martinez, Megan Tapia, Philip A. Gable, Cody D. Christopherson, Franziska Plessow, Roger Giner-Sorolla, Abraham M. Rutchick, Michael Barnett-Cowan, Mark J. Brandt, Rebecca A. Dore, Michael May, H. Colleen Sinclair, Georg Jahn, Daniel P. Martin, Fred Hasselman, Casey Eggleston, Nicole Mechin, Joshua J. Matacotta, Molly Babel, Franziska Maria Kolorz, Social & Organizational Psychology, IBBA, Clinical Psychology, EMGO+ - Mental Health, Social Networks, Solidarity and Inequality, Department of Social Psychology, Department of Methodology and Statistics, Aarts, A, Anderson, J, Anderson, C, Attridge, P, Attwood, A, Axt, J, Babel, M, Bahník, Š, Baranski, E, Barnett Cowan, M, Bartmess, E, Beer, J, Bell, R, Bentley, H, Beyan, L, Binion, G, Borsboom, D, Bosch, A, Bosco, F, Bowman, S, Brandt, M, Braswell, E, Brohmer, H, Brown, B, Brown, K, Brüning, J, Calhoun Sauls, A, Callahan, S, Chagnon, E, Chandler, J, Chartier, C, Cheung, C, Cd, Cillessen, L, Clay, R, Cleary, H, Cloud, M, Cohn, M, Cohoon, J, Columbus, S, Cordes, A, Costantini, G, Cramblet Alvarez, L, Cremata, E, Crusius, J, Decoster, J, Degaetano, M, Della Penna, N, den Bezemer, B, Deserno, M, Devitt, O, Dewitte, L, Dobolyi, D, Dodson, G, Donnellan, M, Donohue, R, Dore, R, Dorrough, A, Dreber, A, Dugas, M, Dunn, E, Easey, K, Eboigbe, S, Eggleston, C, Embley, J, Epskamp, S, Errington, T, Estel, V, Farach, F, Feather, J, Fedor, A, Fernández Castilla, B, Fiedler, S, Field, J, Fitneva, S, Flagan, T, Forest, A, Forsell, E, Foster, J, Frank, M, Frazier, R, Fuchs, H, Gable, P, Galak, J, Galliani, E, Gampa, A, Garcia, S, Gazarian, D, Gilbert, E, Giner Sorolla, R, Glöckner, A, Goellner, L, Goh, J, Goldberg, R, Goodbourn, P, Gordon McKeon, S, Gorges, B, Gorges, J, Goss, J, Graham, J, Grange, J, Gray, J, Hartgerink, C, Hartshorne, J, Hasselman, F, Hayes, T, Heikensten, E, Henninger, F, Hodsoll, J, Holubar, T, Hoogendoorn, G, Humphries, D, Hung, C, Immelman, N, Irsik, V, Jahn, G, Jäkel, F, Jekel, M, Johannesson, M, Johnson, L, Johnson, D, Johnson, K, Johnston, W, Jonas, K, Joy Gaba, J, Kappes, H, Kelso, K, Kidwell, M, Kim, S, Kirkhart, M, Kleinberg, B, Kneževic, G, Kolorz, F, Kossakowski, J, Krause, R, Krijnen, J, Kuhlmann, T, Kunkels, Y, Kyc, M, Lai, C, Laique, A, Lakens, D, Lane, K, Lassetter, B, Lazarevic, L, Lebel, E, Lee, K, Lee, M, Lemm, K, Levitan, C, Lewis, M, Lin, L, Lin, S, Lippold, M, Loureiro, D, Luteijn, I, Mackinnon, S, Mainard, H, Marigold, D, Martin, D, Martinez, T, Masicampo, E, Matacotta, J, Mathur, M, May, M, Mechin, N, Mehta, P, Meixner, J, Melinger, A, Miller, J, Miller, M, Moore, K, Möschl, M, Motyl, M, Müller, S, Munafo, M, Neijenhuijs, K, Nervi, T, Nicolas, G, Nilsonne, G, Nosek, B, Nuijten, M, Olsson, C, Osborne, C, Ostkamp, L, Pavel, M, Penton Voak, I, Perna, O, Pernet, C, Perugini, M, Pipitone, N, Pitts, M, Plessow, F, Prenoveau, J, Rahal, R, Ratliff, K, Reinhard, D, Renkewitz, F, Ricker, A, Rigney, A, Rivers, A, Roebke, M, Rutchick, A, Ryan, R, Sahin, O, Saide, A, Sandstrom, G, Santos, D, Saxe, R, Schlegelmilch, R, Schmidt, K, Scholz, S, Seibel, L, Selterman, D, Shaki, S, Simpson, E, Sinclair, H, Skorinko, J, Slowik, A, Snyder, J, Soderberg, C, Sonnleitner, C, Spencer, N, Spies, J, Steegen, S, Stieger, S, Strohminger, N, Sullivan, G, Talhelm, T, Tapia, M, te Dorsthorst, A, Thomae, M, Thomas, S, Tio, P, Traets, F, Tsang, S, Tuerlinckx, F, Turchan, P, Valášek, M, van 't Veer, A, Van Aert, R, van Assen, M, van Bork, R, van de Ven, M, van den Bergh, D, van der Hulst, M, van Dooren, R, van Doorn, J, van Renswoude, D, van Rijn, H, Vanpaemel, W, Vásquez Echeverría, A, Vazquez, M, Velez, N, Vermue, M, Verschoor, M, Vianello, M, Voracek, M, Vuu, G, Wagenmakers, E, Weerdmeester, J, Welsh, A, Westgate, E, Wissink, J, Wood, M, Woods, A, Wright, E, Wu, S, Zeelenberg, M, Zuni, K, Sociology/ICS, Experimental Psychology, Human Technology Interaction, Sociale Psychologie (Psychologie, FMG), Ontwikkelingspsychologie (Psychologie, FMG), and Brein en Cognitie (Psychologie, FMG)
- Subjects
Research design ,Department Psychologie ,BF Psychology ,media_common.quotation_subject ,POWER ,Learning and Plasticity ,Reproducibility Project ,Q1 ,Experimental Psychopathology and Treatment ,Replication (statistics) ,Statistics ,TRUTH ,Psychology ,General ,Mathematics ,media_common ,Selection bias ,Replication crisis ,Behaviour Change and Well-being ,Multidisciplinary ,PUBLICATION ,Publication bias ,Reproducibility ,Confidence interval ,INCENTIVES ,PREVALENCE ,Meta-analysis ,REPLICABILITY ,REPLICATION ,Developmental Psychopathology ,FALSE - Abstract
IntroductionReproducibility is a defining feature of science, but the extent to which it characterizes current research is unknown. Scientific claims should not gain credence because of the status or authority of their originator but by the replicability of their supporting evidence. Even research of exemplary quality may have irreproducible empirical findings because of random or systematic error.RationaleThere is concern about the rate and predictors of reproducibility, but limited evidence. Potentially problematic practices include selective reporting, selective analysis, and insufficient specification of the conditions necessary or sufficient to obtain the results. Direct replication is the attempt to recreate the conditions believed sufficient for obtaining a previously observed finding and is the means of establishing reproducibility of a finding with new data. We conducted a large-scale, collaborative effort to obtain an initial estimate of the reproducibility of psychological science.ResultsWe conducted replications of 100 experimental and correlational studies published in three psychology journals using high-powered designs and original materials when available. There is no single standard for evaluating replication success. Here, we evaluated reproducibility using significance and P values, effect sizes, subjective assessments of replication teams, and meta-analysis of effect sizes. The mean effect size (r) of the replication effects (Mr = 0.197, SD = 0.257) was half the magnitude of the mean effect size of the original effects (Mr = 0.403, SD = 0.188), representing a substantial decline. Ninety-seven percent of original studies had significant results (P < .05). Thirty-six percent of replications had significant results; 47% of original effect sizes were in the 95% confidence interval of the replication effect size; 39% of effects were subjectively rated to have replicated the original result; and if no bias in original results is assumed, combining original and replication results left 68% with statistically significant effects. Correlational tests suggest that replication success was better predicted by the strength of original evidence than by characteristics of the original and replication teams.ConclusionNo single indicator sufficiently describes replication success, and the five indicators examined here are not the only ways to evaluate reproducibility. Nonetheless, collectively these results offer a clear conclusion: A large portion of replications produced weaker evidence for the original findings despite using materials provided by the original authors, review in advance for methodological fidelity, and high statistical power to detect the original effect sizes. Moreover, correlational evidence is consistent with the conclusion that variation in the strength of initial evidence (such as original P value) was more predictive of replication success than variation in the characteristics of the teams conducting the research (such as experience and expertise). The latter factors certainly can influence replication success, but they did not appear to do so here. Reproducibility is not well understood because the incentives for individual scientists prioritize novelty over replication. Innovation is the engine of discovery and is vital for a productive, effective scientific enterprise. However, innovative ideas become old news fast. Journal reviewers and editors may dismiss a new test of a published idea as unoriginal. The claim that “we already know this” belies the uncertainty of scientific evidence. Innovation points out paths that are possible; replication points out paths that are likely; progress relies on both. Replication can increase certainty when findings are reproduced and promote innovation when they are not. This project provides accumulating evidence for many findings in psychological research and suggests that there is still more work to do to verify whether we know what we think we know.
- Published
- 2015
48. Polygenic Scores and Networks of Psychopathology Symptoms.
- Author
-
Piazza GG, Allegrini AG, Eley TC, Epskamp S, Fried E, Isvoranu AM, Roiser JP, and Pingault JB
- Subjects
- Humans, Female, Child, Male, Cross-Sectional Studies, Longitudinal Studies, Psychopathology, Phenotype, Multifactorial Inheritance genetics, Mental Disorders genetics, Mental Disorders epidemiology, Mental Disorders psychology
- Abstract
Importance: Studies on polygenic risk for psychiatric traits commonly use a disorder-level approach to phenotyping, implicitly considering disorders as homogeneous constructs; however, symptom heterogeneity is ubiquitous, with many possible combinations of symptoms falling under the same disorder umbrella. Focusing on individual symptoms may shed light on the role of polygenic risk in psychopathology., Objective: To determine whether polygenic scores are associated with all symptoms of psychiatric disorders or with a subset of indicators and whether polygenic scores are associated with comorbid phenotypes via specific sets of relevant symptoms., Design, Setting, and Participants: Data from 2 population-based cohort studies were used in this cross-sectional study. Data from children in the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) were included in the primary analysis, and data from children in the Twins Early Development Study (TEDS) were included in confirmatory analyses. Data analysis was conducted from October 2021 to January 2024. Pregnant women based in the Southwest of England due to deliver in 1991 to 1992 were recruited in ALSPAC. Twins born in 1994 to 1996 were recruited in TEDS from population-based records. Participants with available genetic data and whose mothers completed the Short Mood and Feelings Questionnaire and the Strength and Difficulties Questionnaire when children were 11 years of age were included., Main Outcomes and Measures: Psychopathology relevant symptoms, such as hyperactivity, prosociality, depression, anxiety, and peer and conduct problems at age 11 years. Psychological networks were constructed including individual symptoms and polygenic scores for depression, anxiety, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), body mass index (BMI), and educational attainment in ALSPAC. Following a preregistered confirmatory analysis, network models were cross-validated in TEDS., Results: Included were 5521 participants from ALSPAC (mean [SD] age, 11.8 [0.14] years; 2777 [50.3%] female) and 4625 participants from TEDS (mean [SD] age, 11.27 [0.69] years; 2460 [53.2%] female). Polygenic scores were preferentially associated with restricted subsets of core symptoms and indirectly associated with other, more distal symptoms of psychopathology (network edges ranged between r = -0.074 and r = 0.073). Psychiatric polygenic scores were associated with specific cross-disorder symptoms, and nonpsychiatric polygenic scores were associated with a variety of indicators across disorders, suggesting a potential contribution of nonpsychiatric traits to comorbidity. For example, the polygenic score for ADHD was associated with a core ADHD symptom, being easily distracted (r = 0.07), and the polygenic score for BMI was associated with symptoms across disorders, including being bullied (r = 0.053) and not thinking things out (r = 0.041)., Conclusions and Relevance: Genetic associations observed at the disorder level may hide symptom-level heterogeneity. A symptom-level approach may enable a better understanding of the role of polygenic risk in shaping psychopathology and comorbidity.
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
49. Network analysis: An overview for mental health research.
- Author
-
Briganti G, Scutari M, Epskamp S, Borsboom D, Hoekstra RHA, Golino HF, Christensen AP, Morvan Y, Ebrahimi OV, Costantini G, Heeren A, Ron J, Bringmann LF, Huth K, Haslbeck JMB, Isvoranu AM, Marsman M, Blanken T, Gilbert A, Henry TR, Fried EI, and McNally RJ
- Subjects
- Humans, Biomedical Research methods, Bayes Theorem, Mental Health, Mental Disorders diagnosis, Mental Disorders therapy
- Abstract
Network approaches to psychopathology have become increasingly common in mental health research, with many theoretical and methodological developments quickly gaining traction. This article illustrates contemporary practices in applying network analytical tools, bridging the gap between network concepts and their empirical applications. We explain how we can use graphs to construct networks representing complex associations among observable psychological variables. We then discuss key network models, including dynamic networks, time-varying networks, network models derived from panel data, network intervention analysis, latent networks, and moderated models. In addition, we discuss Bayesian networks and their role in causal inference with a focus on cross-sectional data. After presenting the different methods, we discuss how network models and psychopathology theories can meaningfully inform each other. We conclude with a discussion that summarizes the insights each technique can provide in mental health research., (© 2024 The Author(s). International Journal of Methods in Psychiatric Research published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd.)
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
50. The urban desirability paradox: U.K. urban-rural differences in well-being, social satisfaction, and economic satisfaction.
- Author
-
Finnemann A, Huth K, Borsboom D, Epskamp S, and van der Maas H
- Subjects
- Humans, Male, Adult, Female, United Kingdom, Middle Aged, Cities, Socioeconomic Factors, Aged, Urban Population, Rural Population, Personal Satisfaction
- Abstract
As the majority of the global population resides in cities, it is imperative to understand urban well-being. While cities offer concentrated social and economic opportunities, the question arises whether these benefits translate to equitable levels of satisfaction in these domains. Using a robust and objective measure of urbanicity on a sample of 156,000 U.K. residents aged 40 and up, we find that urban living is associated with lower scores across seven dimensions of well-being, social satisfaction, and economic satisfaction. In addition, these scores exhibit greater variability within urban areas, revealing increased inequality. Last, we identify optimal distances in the hinterlands of cities with the highest satisfaction and the least variation. Our findings raise concern for the psychological well-being of urban residents and show the importance of nonlinear methods in urban research.
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.