19 results on '"Carter, Alex R."'
Search Results
2. Differential white matter involvement associated with distinct visuospatial deficits after right hemisphere stroke
- Author
-
Carter, Alex R., McAvoy, Mark P., Siegel, Joshua S., Hong, Xin, Astafiev, Serguei V., Rengachary, Jennifer, Zinn, Kristi, Metcalf, Nicholas V., Shulman, Gordon L., and Corbetta, Maurizio
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
3. Why use a connectivity-based approach to study stroke and recovery of function?
- Author
-
Carter, Alex R., Shulman, Gordon L., and Corbetta, Maurizio
- Published
- 2012
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
4. Large-scale changes in network interactions as a physiological signature of spatial neglect
- Author
-
Baldassarre, Antonello, Ramsey, Lenny, Hacker, Carl L., Callejas, Alicia, Astafiev, Serguei V., Metcalf, Nicholas V., Zinn, Kristi, Rengachary, Jennifer, Snyder, Abraham Z., Carter, Alex R., Shulman, Gordon L., and Corbetta, Maurizio
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
5. Rehabilitation After Stroke: Current State of the Science
- Author
-
Carter, Alex R., Connor, Lisa T., and Dromerick, Alexander W.
- Published
- 2010
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
6. Brain mapping of attention and neglect after stroke
- Author
-
Carter, Alex R., primary, Shulman, Gordon L., additional, and Corbetta, Maurizio, additional
- Published
- 2010
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
7. Topography of Spontaneous Activity Predicts Visuo-Spatial Neglect
- Author
-
Baldassarre, Antonello, Ramsey, Lenny, Hacker, Carl, Metcalf, Nick V., Rengachary, Jennifer, Zinn, Kristina L., Snyder, Abraham Z., Carter, Alex R., Shulman, Gordon L., and Corbetta, Maurizio
- Published
- 2013
8. Bimanual Dexterity and Interhemispheric Interactions in Musicians
- Author
-
Carter, Alex R., Zinn, Kristi, Rengachary, Jennifer, and Corbetta, Maurizio
- Published
- 2012
9. Behaviorally-Driven Anatomical Mapping of Hemispatial Neglect
- Author
-
Carter, Alex R., Mcavoy, Mark P., Astafiev, Serguei V., Rengachary, Jennifer, Pope, Daniel L. W., Snyder, Abraham Z., Zinn, Kristi, Metcalf, Nick, Shulman, Gordon L., and MAURIZIO CORBETTA
- Published
- 2011
10. High-Intensity, Unilateral Resistance Training of a Non-Paretic Muscle Group Increases Active Range of Motion in a Severely Paretic Upper Extremity Muscle Group after Stroke
- Author
-
Urbin, M. A., primary, Harris-Love, Michelle L., additional, Carter, Alex R., additional, and Lang, Catherine E., additional
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
11. Common Behavioral Clusters and Subcortical Anatomy in Stroke
- Author
-
Corbetta, Maurizio, primary, Ramsey, Lenny, additional, Callejas, Alicia, additional, Baldassarre, Antonello, additional, Hacker, Carl D., additional, Siegel, Joshua S., additional, Astafiev, Serguei V., additional, Rengachary, Jennifer, additional, Zinn, Kristina, additional, Lang, Catherine E., additional, Connor, Lisa Tabor, additional, Fucetola, Robert, additional, Strube, Michael, additional, Carter, Alex R., additional, and Shulman, Gordon L., additional
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
12. Role of Corticospinal Tract Damage in Somatomotor Network Functional Connectivity after Stroke
- Author
-
Carter, Alex R., Patel, Kevin R., Astafiev, Serguei V., Snyder, Abraham Z., Rengachary, Jennifer, Strube, Michael J., Pope, Daniel L. W., Shimony, Joshua S., Shulman, Gordon L., and Corbetta, Maurizio
- Published
- 2010
13. Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation in a 15-Year-Old Patient With Autism and Comorbid Depression
- Author
-
Cristancho, Pilar, primary, Akkineni, Keerthi, additional, Constantino, John N., additional, Carter, Alex R., additional, and O ’Reardon, John P., additional
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
14. Resting-State Functional Connectivity and Its Association With Multiple Domains of Upper-Extremity Function in Chronic Stroke
- Author
-
Urbin, M. A., primary, Hong, Xin, additional, Lang, Catherine E., additional, and Carter, Alex R., additional
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
15. Upstream Dysfunction of Somatomotor Functional Connectivity After Corticospinal Damage in Stroke
- Author
-
Carter, Alex R., primary, Patel, Kevin R., additional, Astafiev, Serguei V., additional, Snyder, Abraham Z., additional, Rengachary, Jennifer, additional, Strube, Michael J., additional, Pope, Anna, additional, Shimony, Joshua S., additional, Lang, Catherine E., additional, Shulman, Gordon L., additional, and Corbetta, Maurizio, additional
- Published
- 2011
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
16. Resting state inter-hemispheric fMRI connectivity predicts performance after stroke
- Author
-
Carter, Alex R., primary, Astafiev, Serguei V., additional, Lang, Catherine E., additional, Connor, Lisa T., additional, Rengachary, Jennifer, additional, Strube, Michael J., additional, Pope, Daniel L. W., additional, Shulman, Gordon L., additional, and Corbetta, Maurizio, additional
- Published
- 2009
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
17. Upstream Dysfunction of Somatomotor Functional Connectivity After Corticospinal Damage in Stroke.
- Author
-
Carter, Alex R., Patel, Kevin R., Astafiev, Serguei V., Snyder, Abraham Z., Rengachary, Jennifer, Strube, Michael J., Pope, Anna, Shimony, Joshua S., Lang, Catherine E., Shulman, Gordon L., and Corbetta, Maurizio
- Abstract
Background. Recent studies have shown that focal injuries can have remote effects on network function that affect behavior, but these network-wide repercussions are poorly understood. Objective. This study tested the hypothesis that lesions specifically to the outflow tract of a distributed network can result in upstream dysfunction in structurally intact portions of the network. In the somatomotor system, this upstream dysfunction hypothesis predicted that lesions of the corticospinal tract might be associated with functional disruption within the system. Motor impairment might then reflect the dual contribution of corticospinal damage and altered network functional connectivity. Methods. A total of 23 subacute stroke patients and 13 healthy controls participated in the study. Corticospinal tract damage was quantified using a template of the tract generated from diffusion tensor imaging in healthy controls. Somatomotor network functional integrity was determined by resting state functional connectivity magnetic resonance imaging. Results. The extent of corticospinal damage was negatively correlated with interhemispheric resting functional connectivity, in particular with connectivity between the left and right central sulcus. Although corticospinal damage accounted for much of the variance in motor performance, the behavioral impact of resting connectivity was greater in subjects with mild or moderate corticospinal damage and less in those with severe corticospinal damage. Conclusions. Our results demonstrated that dysfunction of cortical functional connectivity can occur after interruption of corticospinal outflow tracts and can contribute to impaired motor performance. Recognition of these secondary effects from a focal lesion is essential for understanding brain–behavior relationships after injury, and they may have important implications for neurorehabilitation. [ABSTRACT FROM PUBLISHER]
- Published
- 2012
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
18. Resting interhemispheric functional magnetic resonance imaging connectivity predicts performance after stroke.
- Author
-
Carter, Alex R., Astafiev, Serguei V., Lang, Catherine E., Connor, Lisa T., Rengachary, Jennifer, Strube, Michael J., Pope, Daniel L. W., Shulman, Gordon L., and Corbetta, Maurizio
- Abstract
Objective Focal brain lesions can have important remote effects on the function of distant brain regions. The resulting network dysfunction may contribute significantly to behavioral deficits observed after stroke. This study investigates the behavioral significance of changes in the coherence of spontaneous activity in distributed networks after stroke by measuring resting state functional connectivity (FC) using functional magnetic resonance imaging. Methods In acute stroke patients, we measured FC in a dorsal attention network and an arm somatomotor network, and determined the correlation of FC with performance obtained in a separate session on tests of attention and motor function. In particular, we compared the behavioral correlation with intrahemispheric FC to the behavioral correlation with interhemispheric FC. Results In the attention network, disruption of interhemispheric FC was significantly correlated with abnormal detection of visual stimuli (Pearson r with field effect = −0.624, p = 0.002). In the somatomotor network, disruption of interhemispheric FC was significantly correlated with upper extremity impairment (Pearson r with contralesional Action Research Arm Test = 0.527, p = 0.036). In contrast, intrahemispheric FC within the normal or damaged hemispheres was not correlated with performance in either network. Quantitative lesion analysis demonstrated that our results could not be explained by structural damage alone. Interpretation These results suggest that lesions cause state changes in the spontaneous functional architecture of the brain, and constrain behavioral output. Clinically, these results validate using FC for assessing the health of brain networks, with implications for prognosis and recovery from stroke, and underscore the importance of interhemispheric interactions. ANN NEUROL 2010;67:365-375 [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2010
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
19. Agraphia
- Author
-
Tiu JB and Carter AR
- Abstract
Agraphia is an impairment or loss of a previous ability to write. Agraphia can occur in isolation, although it often occurs concurrently with other neurologic deficits such as alexia, apraxia, or hemispatial neglect. Clinically, agraphia can be divided into “central” agraphia (also called “linguistic” or “aphasic“ agraphia), and “peripheral” agraphia (also called “nonlinguistic” or “nonaphasic” agraphia).[1] To perform the act of writing, an individual produces a series of “graphemes” to communicate meaningful information. In the English language, graphemes consist of the letters of the Latin alphabet. To write properly, one must first have knowledge of the letters themselves, and then one must know how to organize letters to form proper words and grammatically correct sentences. Lesions disrupting these processes result in central agraphia. The individual must then have the knowledge of the set of coordinated movements to correctly draw out letters (praxis), the ability to “mentally queue up” a sequence of letters to make an entire word (motor programming), the visuospatial ability to guide a writing implement on a writing surface, and finally, the motor system to carry out these tasks.[1][2] Impairment of these latter steps involved in the motor planning or motor action of writing leads to peripheral agraphia. Note that “peripheral” agraphia can localize to the central nervous system when it does not directly involve linguistic centers, such as in the case of motor agraphia due to a motor cortex lesion leading to the paresis of the writing limb. There exist both clinical neurologic and neuropsychological classification schema to categorize the agraphias, and a clinical neurologic classification is used in this review. “Pure agraphia” is a term that is used to refer to an isolated impairment of writing without an associated relevant impairment in either language ability or praxis. While certain authors equate “pure agraphia” with apraxic agraphia, there is an effort by other authors to distinguish between “pure linguistic (or aphasic) agraphia” and “pure apraxic agraphia”.[3] This review agrees with making this distinction on both a clinical and anatomic basis, and the evidence for supporting this distinction is discussed throughout this article.It must also be noted that the term “aphasia” is used inconsistently in literature. Aphasia most commonly refers to an acquired impairment of just spoken language, but it is also frequently used to encompass impairments of both written and spoken language.[2][4][5] This article uses the term “aphasia” to refer to an isolated impairment of spoken language. The peripheral agraphias can be viewed as analogous to “dysarthrias” of written language, in the sense that “dysarthria” denotes an impairment in the motor output of verbal language. Furthermore, in central agraphia, it is possible for there to be a dissociation between written and spoken language ability; in some cases, there is even a difference in the type of aphasia manifested in the same patient, such as verbal nonfluent aphasia accompanied by written fluent aphasia.[6][7] The qualifier term “linguistic” is used in this review to encompass issues of both written and spoken language. Agraphia is distinguished from illiteracy, in which case the ability to write is never obtained. The term “dysgraphia” is used most commonly to denote handwriting impairment as part of a primary learning disability, but it is also sometimes used to denote an incomplete acquired writing impairment and, in this case, is synonymous with agraphia.[8] “Paragraphia” refers to a specific writing error, and they are analogous to the paraphasic errors of speech. The impairment of typing on a keyboard--dystypia, or dystextia in the case of mobile phone use--often accompanies agraphia, but cases of isolated dystypia or dystextia suggest these may represent distinct impairments.[9] Central (Linguistic) Agraphia: Agraphia with Nonfluent Aphasia: Agraphia, with nonfluent aphasia, typically reflects features of Broca’s aphasia, also referred to as nonfluent or motor aphasia. Letter and word output are lower than expected, with spelling errors due to letter omission. There may also be agrammatism marked by a paucity of prepositions and other grammatical elements, and calligraphy is poor. Dissociation may be present between written language and verbal language abilities, and written language may be inferior to verbal language. Agraphia with Fluent Aphasia [10]: Agraphia, with fluent aphasia, typically reflects the impairments in Wernicke’s aphasia, also referred to as fluent or sensory aphasia. Patients characteristically produce a normal quantity of words with normal calligraphy, but the content is nonsensical. There may be neologisms (“jargonagraphia”), as well as the nonsensical use or placement of proper words. Grammatical elements may be overused in relation to nouns. Written language ability may be superior to verbal language—sometimes markedly so.[11]. Agraphia with Conduction Aphasia [12]: Agraphia with conduction aphasia has been rarely described. The hallmark of conduction aphasia is a characteristic impairment of repetition known as the “conduit d’approche”: the patient will incorrectly repeat a word, but will sequentially iterate through phonologically proximate variations until the correct pronunciation is reached. Written analogs of the “conduit d’approche” have been described in patients with conduction aphasia. Alexia with Agraphia [13]: Alexia with agraphia simply refers to an impairment of both writing and reading ability, which can occur with or without aphasia. Patterns of written language impairment may follow those seen in the above categories. Pure Linguistic Agraphia vs. Pure Apraxic Agraphia: An isolated impairment of writing that is not accompanied by any other language impairment or impairment in praxis can be considered “pure” agraphia. However, effort should be made to distinguish isolated agraphia due to a linguistic deficit from isolated agraphia due to writing apraxia. Pure Linguistic Agraphia [14][15][16][17][18]: Pure linguistic agraphia is present when an isolated impairment in written language occurs as a result of a disruption to central linguistic processes involved in writing. Verbal language and reading abilities are intact. There may be semantic and/or orthographic errors in writing. Calligraphy quality is often normal. Case-based evidence suggests that pure linguistic agraphia may be further subdivided into phonological and lexical subtypes.[11][19] In phonological agraphia, there is an inability to spell pronounceable nonwords, and a retained ability to spell irregular words, which are exception words where spelling is dissociated from pronunciation--compare “steak” with “beak.” The reverse is found in lexical agraphia. Dyscravia is a more recently described disorder of phoneme-to-grapheme conversion in which written words are misspelled using proximate sounds, such as substituting a “t” for a “d”; dyscravia classifies as a subtype of phonological agraphia.[20]. Pure Apraxic Agraphia [17][21][22][23][24][25]: Pure apraxic agraphia is task-specific apraxia.[26] As with pure linguistic agraphia, in pure apraxic agraphia, there is an isolated impairment in written language in the presence of intact verbal language and reading ability. However, the writing impairment here stems from disruption to processes involved in the motor output of writing, downstream of linguistic processes. As such, impaired calligraphy is often considered a hallmark of apraxic agraphia. There should be no other manifestation of ideomotor or ideational apraxia for apraxic agraphia to be considered “pure.” In some cases of pure apraxic agraphia, findings may be as subtle as a selective impairment in font, such as cursive script (“allographic agraphia”), or case (such as an isolated inability to write in upper-case).[27][28]. Distinguishing between pure linguistic and pure apraxic agraphia: The distinction between pure linguistic apraxia and pure apraxic agraphia has been viewed as a significant diagnostic challenge. Certain techniques may help distinguish these two entities:[3]. Calligraphy (writing quality): Calligraphy is often impaired in pure apraxic agraphia. As such, several authors have suggested that the presence of intact calligraphy points to pure linguistic agraphia. However, it remains unclear if one can have an isolated impairment of calligraphy involving linguistic processes affected in central agraphia with nonfluent aphasia. Spelling: Oral spelling, which is considered a non-motor modality of “writing,” may be affected in linguistic agraphia, but intact in apraxic agraphia. Copying: Writing that improves with copying written text may be useful in identifying pure linguistic agraphia, on the grounds that writing praxis is intact. Non-motor writing: Besides oral spelling, other non-motor modalities to “write,” such as keyboard typing and mobile phone texting, may be preserved in pure apraxic agraphia and impaired in pure linguistic agraphia. Pseudowords and irregular words: The writing of pseudowords and irregular words are used in the clinical neuropsychologic assessment of writing to localize disruptions in “phonological” and “lexical” processes, respectively.[11][29][30] Such techniques may help identify pure linguistic agraphias if writing impairments are limited to these specific linguistic features. Peripheral (Nonlinguistic or Nonaphasic) Agraphia: Apraxic Agraphia [1][16]: In apraxic agraphia, there is an impairment in the motor planning of writing. Apraxic agraphia can occur with or without other forms of apraxia; when isolated, it can be designated as pure apraxic agraphia. Apraxic agraphia is most commonly a form of ideomotor apraxia, in which case the individual has a desire to write but is unable to do so. However, ideational agraphia--in which the knowledge of the concept of writing itself is lost--has also been described.[31]. Motor Agraphia: Motor agraphia is broad and occurs due to the disruption of the motor system downstream of praxis, anywhere from the motor cortex to the peripheral nerve and muscle. Paretic agraphia is a result of paresis regardless of localization. Micrographia is hypokinetic motor agraphia most commonly associated with Parkinsonism and attributed to bradykinesia; it is marked by a progressive decrement of handwriting size. Hyperkinetic agraphia can result from a number of movement disorders such as tremor or chorea. Writer’s cramp is a task-specific focal dystonia of the forearm and hand muscles activated by writing. Writer’s cramp is considered simple when isolated to writing, and complex (or dystonic) when it carries over into other non-writing activities.[32]. Reiterative Agraphia: The reiterative agraphias include features such as perseveration, echographia (rewriting of phrases produced by the examiner), or paligraphia (rewriting of the same phrase produced by the patient). Visuospatial Agraphia [33]: Visuospatial agraphia is writing impairment due to errors of orientation to the writing instrument and/or writing surface. One common cause of visuospatial agraphia is hemispatial neglect, where one is unaware of half of the page--most frequently this manifests with a blank left-hand side of a page. Other features suggesting visuospatial agraphia include errors of word spacing or word grouping, as well as specific letter-writing errors such as incorrectly repeating strokes, or even repeatedly writing the same letter multiple times. Letters or words may be superimposed. In these cases, writing errors are attributed to constructional apraxia. Note that the term “constructional apraxia” is a visuospatial disorder and does not refer to true apraxia (i.e., an error of a skilled motor task), and is not to be confused with apraxic agraphia.[34] Visuospatial agraphia can also be due to optic ataxia, in which there are errors of visual depth perception or oculomotor deficits.[35]. Functional (Psychogenic) Agraphia: Functional agraphia can also occur as a manifestation of conversion syndrome. As with other functional neurologic disorders, framing functional agraphia within the model of traditional neuroanatomic localization can be challenging., (Copyright © 2022, StatPearls Publishing LLC.)
- Published
- 2022
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.