15 results on '"C. Porcherot"'
Search Results
2. Contributors
- Author
-
Kathryn Ambroze, Gastón Ares, Lisa Feldman Barrett, Lina Cárdenas Bayona, Moustafa Bensafi, Gary G. Berntson, Hélène Beuchat, Armand V. Cardello, I. Cayeux, D. Cereghetti, Rafah Chaudhry, Carolina Chaya, Yulia E. Chentsova Dutton, Toby Coates, Géraldine Coppin, S. Delplanque, Pieter M.A. Desmet, Hans De Steur, René A. de Wijk, John S.A. Edwards, Charis Eisen, Kelly Faig, C. Ferdenzi, Steven F. Fokkinga, Arnaud Fournel, N. Gaudreau, Allan Geliebter, Xavier Gellynck, Agnes Giboreau, Loris Grandjean, Daniel Grühn, Heather J. Hartwell, Hyisung C. Hwang, Keiko Ishii, Rubén Jacob-Dazarola, Sara R. Jaeger, Gerry Jager, Harry R. Kissileff, Rebecca R. Klatzkin, Kelly A. Knowles, Ueli Kramer, Stefanie Kremer, Samuel H. Lyons, Marylou Mantel, David Matsumoto, Saif M. Mohammad, Elizabeth Necka, Michelle Murphy Niedziela, Laurence J. Nolan, Lucas P.J.J. Noldus, Greg J. Norman, Anna Ogarkova, Bunmi O. Olatunji, Juan Carlos Ortíz Nicolás, Deger Ozkaramanli, Eva R. Pool, C. Porcherot, Catherine Rouby, David Sander, Joachim J. Schouteten, Neika Sharifian, Sara Spinelli, David M.H. Thomson, Louise den Uijl, Hannelize van Zyl, Ruut Veenhoven, Megan Viar-Paxton, Leticia Vidal, Christiana Westlin, and JungKyoon Yoon
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
3. Contributors
- Author
-
Susan E.P. Bastian, Jacqueline H. Beckley, France Bellisle, Rebecca N. Bleibaum, David Blumenthal, Christina Bodin Danielsson, Ünal Ö. Boya, Armand V. Cardello, Edith Classen, Lukas Danner, Nicolas Darcel, Julien Delarue, Anestis Dougkas, John S.A. Edwards, Lawrence L. Garber, Davide Giacalone, Agnès Giboreau, I. Goisbault, Christina Hartmann, Heather J. Hartwell, Patrick Hehn, Nathalie Herbeth, Suzanne Higgs, Lotte Holm, Eva M. Hyatt, Sara R. Jaeger, Trent E. Johnson, Martin J. Kern, Carla Lynn Kuesten, Thierry Lageat, Jacob Lahne, Monica Laureati, René Lion, Thomas Bøker Lund, Dariah Lutsch, Johanna Mäkelä, Herbert L. Meiselman, Jun Niimi, Mari Niva, Ella Pagliarini, Frank Pessel, Uyen Thuy Xuan Phan, Betina Piqueras-Fiszman, Maartje P. Poelman, C. Porcherot, Sarah Price, Katelijn Quartier, Renata Ristic, Helen Ruddock, Laure Saulais, Michael Siegrist, Sara Spinelli, Ingrid H.M. Steenhuis, Heather Thomas, Jan Vanrie, M.C. Vignon-Mares, Brian Wansink, and Elizabeth H. Zandstra
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
4. Personal and home care products in context
- Author
-
M.C. Vignon-Mares, C. Porcherot, and I. Goisbault
- Subjects
Future studies ,Cultural diversity ,Control (management) ,Natural (music) ,Context (language use) ,Product (category theory) ,Marketing ,Psychology ,Test (assessment) ,Consumer experience - Abstract
Contextual factors are key when experiencing personal and home care products, as they interact with the fragranced product and person-related factors in determining use, enjoyment, and emotional responses. This chapter first addresses the importance of capturing the whole consumer experience by evaluating the fragrance under natural conditions. However, context is generally neglected and absent in many consumer tests that are conducted in central location tests (CLTs). Another approach is to implement consumer tests in natural conditions, or in home use tests (HUTs). Tests conducted at home, however, present many practical challenges in addition to time and cost constraints, because of the lack of control of individual and cultural differences in habits and rituals and the different contexts of use. In addition, these tests do not foster product discrimination and qualitative description in comparison with CLTs. The chapter next raises the question of whether intermediate approaches between CLTs and HUTs could be created by adding contextual variables in CLTs. Different test alternatives, such as evoking or mimicking/simulating contexts in CLTs, are therefore considered. These approaches retain the idea of a controlled setup, while immersing consumers in a given context. Studies illustrating these approaches are presented herein and show better levels of discrimination to those observed with CLTs with good qualitative descriptions allowing the reworking of perfume. The chapter ends with recommendations and perspectives for future studies dedicated to the use of immersive techniques (this discussion could be completed with Chapter 16 from Hartmann and Siegrist and Chapter 23 from Hehn et al., dealing with Virtual Reality).
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
5. List of Contributors
- Author
-
Lisa Feldman Barrett, Lina Cárdenas Bayona, Moustafa Bensafi, Gary G. Berntson, Armand V. Cardello, I. Cayeux, Yulia E. Chentsova-Dutton, Géraldine Coppin, S. Delplanque, Louise den Uijl, Pieter M.A. Desmet, John S.A. Edwards, Charis Eisen, Panteleimon Ekkekakis, C. Ferdenzi, Steven F. Fokkinga, Arnaud Fournel, N. Gaudreau, Agnes Giboreau, Daniel Grühn, Heather J. Hartwell, Hyisung C. Hwang, Keiko Ishii, Rubén Jacob-Dazarola, Sara R. Jaeger, Gerry Jager, Silvia C. King, Ueli Kramer, Stefanie Kremer, Jaime L. Kurtz, Samuel H. Lyons, David Matsumoto, Herbert L. Meiselman, Saif M. Mohammad, Elizabeth Necka, Juan Carlos Ortíz Nicolás, Greg J. Norman, Anna Ogarkova, Bunmi O. Olatunji, Deger Ozkaramanli, C. Porcherot, Catherine Rouby, David Sander, Neika Sharifian, David M.H. Thomson, Hannelize van Zyl, Megan Viar-Paxton, Cheryl A. Welch, JungKyoon Yoon, and Zachary Zenko
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
6. Exogenous capture of visual spatial attention by olfactory-trigeminal stimuli.
- Author
-
Ischer M, Coppin G, De Marles A, Essellier M, Porcherot C, Cayeux I, Margot C, Sander D, and Delplanque S
- Subjects
- Adult, Carbon Dioxide analysis, Cues, Eucalyptol analysis, Humans, Olfactory Perception, Photic Stimulation, Reaction Time physiology, Young Adult, Attention physiology, Odorants analysis, Olfactory Bulb physiology, Spatial Navigation physiology, Trigeminal Nuclei physiology
- Abstract
The extent to which a nasal whiff of scent can exogenously orient visual spatial attention remains poorly understood in humans. In a series of seven studies, we investigated the existence of an exogenous capture of visual spatial attention by purely trigeminal (i.e., CO2) and both olfactory and trigeminal stimuli (i.e., eucalyptol). We chose these stimuli because they activate the trigeminal system which can be considered as an alert system and are thus supposedly relevant for the individual, and thus prone to capture attention. We used them as lateralized cues in a variant of a visual spatial cueing paradigm. In valid trials, trigeminal cues and visual targets were presented on the same side whereas in invalid trials they were presented on opposite sides. To characterize the dynamics of the cross-modal attentional capture, we manipulated the interval between the onset of the trigeminal cues and the visual targets (from 580 to 1870 ms). Reaction times in trigeminal valid trials were shorter than all other trials, but only when this interval was around 680 or 1170 ms for CO2 and around 610 ms for eucalyptol. This result reflects that both pure trigeminal and olfactory-trigeminal stimuli can exogenously capture humans' spatial visual attention. We discuss the importance of considering the dynamics of this cross-modal attentional capture., Competing Interests: The authors declared no potential conflicts of interests with respect to the authorship and/or publication of this article. The commercial affiliation of CP, IC & CM does not alter our adherence to PLOS ONE policies on sharing data and materials.
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
7. Sensitivity of Physiological Emotional Measures to Odors Depends on the Product and the Pleasantness Ranges Used.
- Author
-
Pichon AM, Coppin G, Cayeux I, Porcherot C, Sander D, and Delplanque S
- Abstract
Emotions are characterized by synchronized changes in several components of an organism. Among them, physiological variations provide energy support for the expression of approach/avoid action tendencies induced by relevant stimuli, while self-reported subjective pleasantness feelings integrate all other emotional components and are plastic. Consequently, emotional responses evoked by odors should be highly differentiated when they are linked to different functions of olfaction (e.g., avoiding environmental hazards). As this differentiation has been observed for contrasted odors (very pleasant or unpleasant), we questioned whether subjective and physiological emotional response indicators could still disentangle subtle affective variations when no clear functional distinction is made (mildly pleasant or unpleasant fragrances). Here, we compared the sensitivity of behavioral and physiological [respiration, skin conductance, facial electromyography (EMG), and heart rate] indicators in differentiating odor-elicited emotions in two situations: when a wide range of odor families was presented (e.g., fruity, animal), covering different functional meanings; or in response to a restricted range of products in one particular family (fragrances). Results show clear differences in physiological indicators to odors that display a wide range of reported pleasantness, but these differences almost entirely vanish when fragrances are used even though their subjective pleasantness still differed. Taken together, these results provide valuable information concerning the ability of classic verbal and psychophysiological measures to investigate subtle differences in emotional reactions to a restricted range of similar olfactory stimuli.
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
8. How incorporation of scents could enhance immersive virtual experiences.
- Author
-
Ischer M, Baron N, Mermoud C, Cayeux I, Porcherot C, Sander D, and Delplanque S
- Abstract
Under normal everyday conditions, senses all work together to create experiences that fill a typical person's life. Unfortunately for behavioral and cognitive researchers who investigate such experiences, standard laboratory tests are usually conducted in a nondescript room in front of a computer screen. They are very far from replicating the complexity of real world experiences. Recently, immersive virtual reality (IVR) environments became promising methods to immerse people into an almost real environment that involves more senses. IVR environments provide many similarities to the complexity of the real world and at the same time allow experimenters to constrain experimental parameters to obtain empirical data. This can eventually lead to better treatment options and/or new mechanistic hypotheses. The idea that increasing sensory modalities improve the realism of IVR environments has been empirically supported, but the senses used did not usually include olfaction. In this technology report, we will present an odor delivery system applied to a state-of-the-art IVR technology. The platform provides a three-dimensional, immersive, and fully interactive visualization environment called "Brain and Behavioral Laboratory-Immersive System" (BBL-IS). The solution we propose can reliably deliver various complex scents during different virtual scenarios, at a precise time and space and without contamination of the environment. The main features of this platform are: (i) the limited cross-contamination between odorant streams with a fast odor delivery (< 500 ms), (ii) the ease of use and control, and (iii) the possibility to synchronize the delivery of the odorant with pictures, videos or sounds. How this unique technology could be used to investigate typical research questions in olfaction (e.g., emotional elicitation, memory encoding or attentional capture by scents) will also be addressed.
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
9. Sweet reward increases implicit discrimination of similar odors.
- Author
-
Pool E, Delplanque S, Porcherot C, Jenkins T, Cayeux I, and Sander D
- Abstract
Stimuli associated with emotional events signal the presence of potentially relevant situations, thus learning to rapidly identify this kind of stimuli can be highly beneficial. It has been demonstrated that individuals acquire a better perceptual representation of stimuli associated with negative and threatening emotional events. Here we investigated whether the same process occurs for stimuli associated with positive and rewarding emotional events. We used an appetitive Pavlovian conditioning paradigm during which one of two perceptually non-distinguishable odors was associated with a rewarding taste (i.e., chocolate). We investigated whether appetitive conditioning could improve the recognition of the odor associated with the reward, rendering it discriminable from its similar version that was never associated with the reward. Results revealed a dissociation between explicit perception and physiological reactions. Although participants were not able to explicitly perceive a difference, they reacted faster, inhaled more and had higher skin conductance responses when confronted with the reward-associated odor compared to its similar version that was never associated with the reward. Our findings have demonstrated that positive emotional associations can improve the implicit perceptual representation of odors, by triggering different physiological responses to odors that do not seem to be otherwise distinguishable.
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
10. Choice both affects and reflects preferences.
- Author
-
Coppin G, Delplanque S, Bernard C, Cekic S, Porcherot C, Cayeux I, and Sander D
- Subjects
- Adolescent, Adult, Analysis of Variance, Female, Humans, Male, Odorants, Photic Stimulation, Young Adult, Affect physiology, Choice Behavior physiology, Cognitive Dissonance
- Abstract
The free-choice paradigm is a widely used paradigm in psychology. It has been used to show that after a choice between two similarly pleasant stimuli, the pleasantness of the chosen one tends to increase, whereas the pleasantness of the rejected one tends to decrease-a spreading of alternatives. However, the methodological validity of the free-choice paradigm to study choice-induced preference change has recently been seriously questioned [Chen, K. M., & Risen, J. L. (2010). How choice affects and reflects preferences: Revisiting the free-choice paradigm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99, 573-594. doi:10.1037/a0020217]. According to this criticism, the classically reported spreading of alternatives between the first and second rating sessions cannot be unambiguously interpreted to reflect a true change in preferences and can be observed even for completely static preferences. Here, we used two measurement sequences, a classical Rating 1-choice-Rating 2 sequence and a control Rating 1-Rating 2-choice sequence, to disentangle the spreading of alternatives driven by the effect of choice from the artefactual effect highlighted by Chen and Risen. In two studies using different stimulus material (faces and odours), we find that choice has a robust modulatory impact on preferences for rejected odours, but not for chosen odours and not for faces.
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
11. Variability of affective responses to odors: culture, gender, and olfactory knowledge.
- Author
-
Ferdenzi C, Roberts SC, Schirmer A, Delplanque S, Cekic S, Porcherot C, Cayeux I, Sander D, and Grandjean D
- Subjects
- Adult, Culture, Female, Humans, Male, Middle Aged, Recognition, Psychology, Sex Factors, Singapore, Switzerland, United Kingdom, Young Adult, Emotions, Odorants analysis, Olfactory Perception
- Abstract
Emotion and odor scales (EOS) measuring odor-related affective feelings were recently developed for three different countries (Switzerland, United Kingdom, and Singapore). The first aim of this study was to investigate gender and cultural differences in verbal affective response to odors, measured with EOS and the usual pleasantness scale. To better understand this variability, the second aim was to investigate the link between affective reports and olfactory knowledge (familiarity and identification). Responses of 772 participants smelling 56-59 odors were collected in the three countries. Women rated odors as more intense and identified them better in all countries, but no reliable sex differences were found for verbal affective responses to odors. Disgust-related feelings revealed odor-dependent sex differences, due to sex differences in identification and categorization. Further, increased odor knowledge was related to more positive affects as reported with pleasantness and odor-related feeling evaluations, which can be related to top-down influences on odor representation. These top-down influences were thought, for example, to relate to beliefs about odor properties or to categorization (edible vs. nonedible). Finally, the link between odor knowledge and olfactory affect was generally asymmetrical and significant only for pleasant odors, not for unpleasant ones that seemed to be more resistant to cognitive influences. This study, for the first time using emotional scales that are appropriate to the olfactory domain, brings new insights into the variability of affective responses to odors and its relationship to odor knowledge.
- Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
12. When flexibility is stable: implicit long-term shaping of olfactory preferences.
- Author
-
Coppin G, Delplanque S, Porcherot C, Cayeux I, and Sander D
- Subjects
- Adult, Female, Humans, Male, Pliability, Young Adult, Choice Behavior, Cognitive Dissonance, Discrimination, Psychological, Learning, Odorants, Olfactory Perception
- Abstract
Preferences are traditionally assumed to be stable. However, empirical evidence such as preference modulation following choices calls this assumption into question. The evolution of such postchoice preference over long time spans, even when choices have been explicitly forgotten, has so far not been studied. In two experiments, we investigated this question by using a variant of the free choice paradigm: In a first session, participants evaluated the pleasantness of a number of odors. We then formed pairs of similarly rated odors, and asked participants to choose their favorite, for each pair. Participants were then presented with all odors again, and asked for another pleasantness rating. In a second session 1 week later, a third pleasantness rating was obtained, and participants were again asked to choose between the same options. Results suggested postchoice preference modulation immediately and 1 week after choice for both chosen and rejected options, even when choices were not explicitly remembered. A third experiment, using another paradigm, confirmed that choice can have a modulatory impact on preferences, and that this modulation can be long-lasting. Taken together, these findings suggest that although preferences appear to be flexible because they are modulated by choices, this modulation also appears to be stable over time and even without explicit recollection of the choice. These results bring a new argument to the idea that postchoice preference modulation could rely on implicit mechanisms, and are consistent with the recent proposal that cognitive dissonance reduction could to some extent be implicit.
- Published
- 2012
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
13. How to map the affective semantic space of scents.
- Author
-
Delplanque S, Chrea C, Grandjean D, Ferdenzi C, Cayeux I, Porcherot C, Le Calvé B, Sander D, and Scherer KR
- Subjects
- Adult, Female, Humans, Male, Observer Variation, Odorants, Psychometrics, Surveys and Questionnaires, Affect, Models, Psychological, Olfactory Perception, Semantics
- Abstract
The investigation of the semantic space associated with subjective affective experiences or feelings linked to odour perception has recently emerged. Because of the specificity of the emotional effects of odours, the terms derived from traditional models of emotion are unlikely to optimally account for odour-associated feelings. In this study, sets of terms derived from two traditional models, basic emotions and valence by arousal by dominance dimensional emotions, were compared with a recently elaborated olfaction-specific set of terms (Geneva Emotion and Odour Scale; GEOS). Three main criteria were considered: (1) the feeling's intensity reported in response to odours; (2) the inter-rater agreement concerning the reported feelings; and (3) the power to discriminate feelings evoked by various odorous substances. The evidence strongly suggested that the set of terms proposed by GEOS outperformed the terms derived from the two classical models in measuring the subjective affective experience elicited by odours. These results are interpreted with respect to a good correspondence between the functions of olfaction and the meaning conveyed by GEOS terms.
- Published
- 2012
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
14. Affective dimensions of odor perception: a comparison between Swiss, British, and Singaporean populations.
- Author
-
Ferdenzi C, Schirmer A, Roberts SC, Delplanque S, Porcherot C, Cayeux I, Velazco MI, Sander D, Scherer KR, and Grandjean D
- Subjects
- Adult, Female, Humans, Male, Odorants, Singapore, Switzerland, United Kingdom, Young Adult, Affect, Cross-Cultural Comparison, Olfactory Perception
- Abstract
Do affective responses to odors vary as a function of culture? To address this question, we developed two self-report scales in the United Kingdom (Liverpool: LEOS) and in Singapore (city of Singapore: SEOS), following the same procedure as used in the past to develop the Geneva Emotion and Odor Scale (GEOS: Chrea, Grandjean, Delplanque et al., 2009). The final scales were obtained by a three-step reduction of an initial pool of 480 affective terms, retaining only the most relevant terms to describe odor-related subjective affective states and comprised of six (GEOS) or seven affective dimensions (LEOS and SEOS). These included dimensions that were common to the three cultures (Disgust, Happiness Well-being, Sensuality Desire, and Energy), common to the two European samples (Soothing Peacefulness), and dimensions that were culture specific (Sensory Pleasure in Geneva; Nostalgia and Hunger Thirst in Liverpool; Intellectual Stimulation, Spirituality, and Negative Feelings in Singapore). A comparative approach showed that the dimensional organization of odor-related affective terms in a given culture better explained data variability for that culture than data variability for the other cultures, thus highlighting the importance of culture-specific tools in the investigation of odor-related affect.
- Published
- 2011
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
15. I'm no longer torn after choice: how explicit choices implicitly shape preferences of odors.
- Author
-
Coppin G, Delplanque S, Cayeux I, Porcherot C, and Sander D
- Subjects
- Awareness, Cognitive Dissonance, Discrimination, Psychological, Female, Humans, Male, Mental Recall, Pleasure, Psychophysics, Young Adult, Attitude, Choice Behavior, Decision Making, Odorants, Smell
- Abstract
Several studies have shown that preferences can be strongly modulated by cognitive processes such as decision making and choices. However, it is still unclear whether choices can influence preferences of sensory stimuli implicitly. This question was addressed here by asking participants to evaluate odors, to choose their preferred odors within pairs, to reevaluate the odors, and to perform an unexpected memory test. Results revealed, for the first time in the study of olfaction, the existence of postchoice preference changes, in the sense of an overvaluation of chosen odors and a devaluation of rejected ones, even when choices were forgotten. These results suggest that chemosensory preferences can be modulated by explicit choices and that such modulation might rely on implicit mechanisms. This finding rules out any explanation of postchoice preference changes in terms of experimental demand and strongly challenges the classical cognitive-dissonance-reduction account of such preference changes.
- Published
- 2010
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.