27 results on '"Boiger M"'
Search Results
2. Coping with global uncertainty: Perceptions of COVID-19 psychological distress, relationship quality, and dyadic coping for romantic partners across 27 countries
- Author
-
Randall, AK, Leon, G, Basili, E, Martos, T, Boiger, M, Baldi, M, Hocker, L, Kline, K, Masturzi, A, Aryeetey, R, Bar-Kalifa, E, Boon, SD, Botella, L, Burke, T, Carnelley, KB, Carr, A, Dash, A, Fitriana, M, Gaines, SO, Galdiolo, S, Hart, CM, Joo, S, Kanth, B, Karademas, E, Karantzas, Gery, Landolt, SA, McHugh, L, Milek, A, Murphy, E, Natividade, JC, Portugal, A, Quiñones, Á, Relvas, AP, Rumondor, PCB, Rusu, P, Sallay, V, Saul, LA, Schmitt, DP, Sels, L, Shujja, S, Taylor, LK, Ozguluk, SB, Verhofstadt, L, Yoo, G, Zemp, M, Donato, S, Totenhagen, CJ, van Eickels, RL, Adil, A, Anaba, EA, Asampong, E, Beauchemin-Roy, S, Berry, A, Brassard, A, Chesterman, Susan, Ferguson, L, Fonseca, G, Gaugue, J, Geonet, M, Hermesch, N, Abdul Wahab Khan, RK, Knox, Laura, Lafontaine, MF, Lawless, N, Londero-Santos, A, Major, S, Marot, TA, Mullins, Ellie, Otermans, PCJ, Pagani, AF, Parise, M, Parvin, R, De, M, Péloquin, K, Rebelo, B, Righetti, F, Romano, Daniel, Salavati, S, Samrock, S, Serea, M, Seok, CB, Sotero, L, Stafford, O, Thomadakis, C, Topcu-Uzer, C, Ugarte, C, Low, WY, Simon-Zámbori, P, Siau, CS, Duca, DS, Filip, C, Park, H, Wearen, S, Bodenmann, G, Chiarolanza, C, Randall, AK, Leon, G, Basili, E, Martos, T, Boiger, M, Baldi, M, Hocker, L, Kline, K, Masturzi, A, Aryeetey, R, Bar-Kalifa, E, Boon, SD, Botella, L, Burke, T, Carnelley, KB, Carr, A, Dash, A, Fitriana, M, Gaines, SO, Galdiolo, S, Hart, CM, Joo, S, Kanth, B, Karademas, E, Karantzas, Gery, Landolt, SA, McHugh, L, Milek, A, Murphy, E, Natividade, JC, Portugal, A, Quiñones, Á, Relvas, AP, Rumondor, PCB, Rusu, P, Sallay, V, Saul, LA, Schmitt, DP, Sels, L, Shujja, S, Taylor, LK, Ozguluk, SB, Verhofstadt, L, Yoo, G, Zemp, M, Donato, S, Totenhagen, CJ, van Eickels, RL, Adil, A, Anaba, EA, Asampong, E, Beauchemin-Roy, S, Berry, A, Brassard, A, Chesterman, Susan, Ferguson, L, Fonseca, G, Gaugue, J, Geonet, M, Hermesch, N, Abdul Wahab Khan, RK, Knox, Laura, Lafontaine, MF, Lawless, N, Londero-Santos, A, Major, S, Marot, TA, Mullins, Ellie, Otermans, PCJ, Pagani, AF, Parise, M, Parvin, R, De, M, Péloquin, K, Rebelo, B, Righetti, F, Romano, Daniel, Salavati, S, Samrock, S, Serea, M, Seok, CB, Sotero, L, Stafford, O, Thomadakis, C, Topcu-Uzer, C, Ugarte, C, Low, WY, Simon-Zámbori, P, Siau, CS, Duca, DS, Filip, C, Park, H, Wearen, S, Bodenmann, G, and Chiarolanza, C
- Abstract
Following the global outbreak of COVID-19 in March 2020, individuals report psychological distress associated with the “new normal”—social distancing, financial hardships, and increased responsibilities while working from home. Given the interpersonal nature of stress and coping responses between romantic partners, based on the systemic transactional model this study posits that perceived partner dyadic coping may be an important moderator between experiences of COVID-19 psychological distress and relationship quality. To examine these associations, self-report data from 14,020 people across 27 countries were collected during the early phases of the COVID-19 pandemic (March–July, 2020). It was hypothesized that higher symptoms of psychological distress would be reported post-COVID-19 compared to pre-COVID-19 restrictions (Hypothesis 1), reports of post-COVID-19 psychological distress would be negatively associated with relationship quality (Hypothesis 2), and perceived partner DC would moderate these associations (Hypothesis 3). While hypotheses were generally supported, results also showed interesting between-country variability. Limitations and future directions are presented
- Published
- 2022
3. Erratum to Coping with global uncertainty: Perceptions of COVID-19 psychological distress, relationship quality, and dyadic coping for romantic partners across 27 countries
- Author
-
Randall, AK, Leon, G, Basili, E, Martos, T, Boiger, M, Baldi, M, Hocker, L, Kline, K, Masturzi, A, Aryeetey, R, Bar-Kalifa, E, Boon, SD, Botella, L, Burke, T, Carnelley, KB, Carr, A, Dash, A, Fitriana, M, Gaines, SO, Galdiolo, S, Hart, CM, Joo, S, Kanth, B, Karademas, E, Karantzas, Gery, Landolt, SA, McHugh, L, Milek, A, Murphy, E, Natividade, JC, Portugal, A, Quinones, A, Relvas, AP, Rumondor, PCB, Rusu, P, Sallay, V, Saul, LA, Schmitt, DP, Sels, L, Shujja, S, Taylor, LK, Ozguluk, SB, Verhofstadt, L, Yoo, G, Zemp, M, Donato, S, Totenhagen, CJ, van Eickels, RL, Adil, A, Anaba, EA, Asampong, E, Beauchemin-Roy, S, Berry, A, Brassard, A, Chesterman, Susan, Ferguson, L, Fonseca, G, Gaugue, J, Geonet, M, Hermesch, N, Khan, RKAW, Knox, Laura, Lafontaine, M-F, Lawless, N, Londero-Santos, A, Major, S, Marot, TA, Mullins, Ellie, Otermans, PCJ, Pagani, AF, Parise, M, Parvin, R, De, M, Peloquin, K, Rebelo, B, Righetti, F, Romano, Daniel, Salavati, S, Samrock, S, Serea, M, Seok, CB, Sotero, L, Stafford, O, Thomadakis, C, Topcu-Uzer, C, Ugarte, C, Low, WY, Simon-Zambori, P, Siau, CS, Duca, D-S, Filip, C, Park, H, Wearen, S, Bodenmann, G, Chiarolanza, C, Randall, AK, Leon, G, Basili, E, Martos, T, Boiger, M, Baldi, M, Hocker, L, Kline, K, Masturzi, A, Aryeetey, R, Bar-Kalifa, E, Boon, SD, Botella, L, Burke, T, Carnelley, KB, Carr, A, Dash, A, Fitriana, M, Gaines, SO, Galdiolo, S, Hart, CM, Joo, S, Kanth, B, Karademas, E, Karantzas, Gery, Landolt, SA, McHugh, L, Milek, A, Murphy, E, Natividade, JC, Portugal, A, Quinones, A, Relvas, AP, Rumondor, PCB, Rusu, P, Sallay, V, Saul, LA, Schmitt, DP, Sels, L, Shujja, S, Taylor, LK, Ozguluk, SB, Verhofstadt, L, Yoo, G, Zemp, M, Donato, S, Totenhagen, CJ, van Eickels, RL, Adil, A, Anaba, EA, Asampong, E, Beauchemin-Roy, S, Berry, A, Brassard, A, Chesterman, Susan, Ferguson, L, Fonseca, G, Gaugue, J, Geonet, M, Hermesch, N, Khan, RKAW, Knox, Laura, Lafontaine, M-F, Lawless, N, Londero-Santos, A, Major, S, Marot, TA, Mullins, Ellie, Otermans, PCJ, Pagani, AF, Parise, M, Parvin, R, De, M, Peloquin, K, Rebelo, B, Righetti, F, Romano, Daniel, Salavati, S, Samrock, S, Serea, M, Seok, CB, Sotero, L, Stafford, O, Thomadakis, C, Topcu-Uzer, C, Ugarte, C, Low, WY, Simon-Zambori, P, Siau, CS, Duca, D-S, Filip, C, Park, H, Wearen, S, Bodenmann, G, and Chiarolanza, C
- Published
- 2022
4. Coping with global uncertainty: Perceptions of COVID-19 psychological distress, relationship quality, and dyadic coping for romantic partners across 27 countries
- Author
-
Randall, A. K., Leon, G., Basili, E., Martos, T., Boiger, M., Baldi, M., Hocker, L., Kline, K., Masturzi, A., Aryeetey, R., Bar-Kalifa, E., Boon, S. D., Botella, L., Burke, T., Carnelley, K., Carr, A., Dash, A., Fitriana, M., Gaines, S. O., Galdiolo, S., Claire M, H., Joo, S., Kanth, B., Karademas, E., Karantzas, G., Landolt, S. A., Mchugh, L., Milek, A., Murphy, E., Natividade, J. C., Portugal, A., Quinones, A., Relvas, A. P., Rumondor, P. C. B., Rusu, P., Sallay, V., Saul, L. A., Schmitt, D. P., Sels, L., Shujja, S., Taylor, L. K., Ozguluk, S. B., Verhofstadt, L., Yoo, G., Zemp, M., Donato, Silvia, Totenhagen, C. J., van Eickels, R. L., Anaba, E. A., Beauchemin-Roy, S., Berry, A., Brassard, A., Chesterman, S., Ferguson, L., Fonseca, G., Gaugue, J., Geonet, M., Hermesch, N., Knox, L., Lafontaine, M. -F., Lawless, N., Londero-Santos, A., Major, S., Marot, T. A., Mullins, E., Otermans, P. C. J., Ariela F, P., Parise, Miriam, Parvin, R., De, M., Peloquin, K., Rebelo, B., Righetti, F., Romano, D., Salavati, S., Samrock, S., Serea, M., Seok, C. B., Sotero, L., Stafford, O., Thomadakis, C., Topcu-Uzer, C., Ugarte, C., Yun, L. W., Simon-Zambori, P., Siau, C. S., Duca, D. -S., Filip, C., Park, H., Wearen, S., Bodenmann, G., Chiarolanza, C., Donato S. (ORCID:0000-0002-8406-4604), Parise M. (ORCID:0000-0003-2150-6636), Randall, A. K., Leon, G., Basili, E., Martos, T., Boiger, M., Baldi, M., Hocker, L., Kline, K., Masturzi, A., Aryeetey, R., Bar-Kalifa, E., Boon, S. D., Botella, L., Burke, T., Carnelley, K., Carr, A., Dash, A., Fitriana, M., Gaines, S. O., Galdiolo, S., Claire M, H., Joo, S., Kanth, B., Karademas, E., Karantzas, G., Landolt, S. A., Mchugh, L., Milek, A., Murphy, E., Natividade, J. C., Portugal, A., Quinones, A., Relvas, A. P., Rumondor, P. C. B., Rusu, P., Sallay, V., Saul, L. A., Schmitt, D. P., Sels, L., Shujja, S., Taylor, L. K., Ozguluk, S. B., Verhofstadt, L., Yoo, G., Zemp, M., Donato, Silvia, Totenhagen, C. J., van Eickels, R. L., Anaba, E. A., Beauchemin-Roy, S., Berry, A., Brassard, A., Chesterman, S., Ferguson, L., Fonseca, G., Gaugue, J., Geonet, M., Hermesch, N., Knox, L., Lafontaine, M. -F., Lawless, N., Londero-Santos, A., Major, S., Marot, T. A., Mullins, E., Otermans, P. C. J., Ariela F, P., Parise, Miriam, Parvin, R., De, M., Peloquin, K., Rebelo, B., Righetti, F., Romano, D., Salavati, S., Samrock, S., Serea, M., Seok, C. B., Sotero, L., Stafford, O., Thomadakis, C., Topcu-Uzer, C., Ugarte, C., Yun, L. W., Simon-Zambori, P., Siau, C. S., Duca, D. -S., Filip, C., Park, H., Wearen, S., Bodenmann, G., Chiarolanza, C., Donato S. (ORCID:0000-0002-8406-4604), and Parise M. (ORCID:0000-0003-2150-6636)
- Abstract
Following the global outbreak of COVID-19 in March 2020, individuals report psychological distress associated with the “new normal”—social distancing, financial hardships, and increased responsibilities while working from home. Given the interpersonal nature of stress and coping responses between romantic partners, based on the systemic transactional model this study posits that perceived partner dyadic coping may be an important moderator between experiences of COVID-19 psychological distress and relationship quality. To examine these associations, self-report data from 14,020 people across 27 countries were collected during the early phases of the COVID-19 pandemic (March–July, 2020). It was hypothesized that higher symptoms of psychological distress would be reported post-COVID-19 compared to pre-COVID-19 restrictions (Hypothesis 1), reports of post-COVID-19 psychological distress would be negatively associated with relationship quality (Hypothesis 2), and perceived partner DC would moderate these associations (Hypothesis 3). While hypotheses were generally supported, results also showed interesting between-country variability. Limitations and future directions are presented.
- Published
- 2022
5. What has culture got to do with emotions?
- Author
-
De Leersnyder, J., Mesquita, B., Boiger, M., Gelfand, M.J., Chiu, C.-Y., Hong, Y.-Y., and Sociale Psychologie (Psychologie, FMG)
- Abstract
Emotions are relationship engagements that are dynamically and socioculturally constructed. Starting from the historic context in which the current research program originated, this chapter develops a theory in which cultural differences in emotion can be understood from the cultural context’s valued model of self and relating. It presents evidence for a “cultural logic” to emotion in the prevalence and content of emotion as well as to which experiences are associated with positive outcomes and well-being. Furthermore, it shows how a myriad of processes co-constitute the alignment of culture and emotion—processes that can be situated at the personal, interpersonal, and collective levels and that are highlighted when emotions are studied in acculturating individuals or biculturals. In concluding, this chapter presents a dynamic and sociocultural model of emotion in which people collectively construct their experiences in line with the prevalent meanings and practices of their sociocultural context.
- Published
- 2021
6. Hoe emoties verschillen tussen culturen
- Author
-
Van Acker, K., Boiger, M., De Leersnyder, J., Mesquita, B., de Jong, J., van Dijk, R., and Sociale Psychologie (Psychologie, FMG)
- Published
- 2020
7. Interpersonal Emotion Regulation in Couples: Cultural Differences and Similarities
- Author
-
Boiger, M. and Sociale Psychologie (Psychologie, FMG)
- Published
- 2019
8. A cultural psychological perspective on close relationships
- Author
-
Boiger, M., Schoebi, D., Campos, B., and Sociale Psychologie (Psychologie, FMG)
- Subjects
Relationship formation ,Process (engineering) ,Close relationship ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Perspective (graphical) ,Foregrounding ,East Asia ,Sociology ,Cultural psychology ,Independence ,media_common ,Epistemology - Abstract
People in different cultures have different objectives and ideals in close relationships. This chapter explores the different ideas, values, and practices that underlie people’s striving for different kinds of close relationships across cultures. It focuses on a comparison between “Western” and East Asian contexts. The chapter looks at a few select relational processes for which cross-cultural data are available: Attachment, relationship formation, conflict, and emotion. It proposes that some of the core ideas of cultural psychology are useful for understanding cultural variation in these relational process: there is meaningful cultural variation in each of these that can be understood from the relative foregrounding of either independence or interdependence in the respective cultural contexts. The chapter shows that there is at least one other way of relating that is systematically different from the independent model that has largely been assumed to be universal in close relationship research.
- Published
- 2019
9. Innovative Assembly Concepts with Reel-To-Reel Transfer
- Author
-
Feldmann, K., Boiger, M., Bigl, T., and Zolleiss, B.
- Published
- 2004
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
10. Foreword: A Cultural Perspective on Shame
- Author
-
Boiger, M., Vanderheiden, E., Mayer, C.-H., and Sociale Psychologie (Psychologie, FMG)
- Published
- 2017
11. The cultural psychology of emotion
- Author
-
Mesquita, B., De Leersnyder, J., Boiger, M., Feldman Barrett, L., Lewis, M., Haviland-Jones, J.M., Psychology Other Research (FMG), and Sociale Psychologie (Psychologie, FMG)
- Published
- 2016
12. Emotions in 'the world': cultural practices, products, and meanings of anger and shame in two individualist cultures
- Author
-
Boiger, M, De Deyne, S, Mesquita, B, Boiger, M, De Deyne, S, and Mesquita, B
- Abstract
Three studies tested the idea that people's cultural worlds are structured in ways that promote and highlight emotions and emotional responses that are beneficial in achieving central goals in their culture. Based on the idea that U.S. Americans strive for competitive individualism, while (Dutch-speaking) Belgians favor a more egalitarian variant of individualism, we predicted that anger and shame, as well as their associated responses, would be beneficial to different extents in these two cultural contexts. A questionnaire study found that cultural practices promote beneficial emotions (anger in the United States, shame in Belgium) and avoid harmful emotions (shame in the United States): emotional interactions were perceived to occur more or less frequently to the extent that they elicited culturally beneficial or harmful emotions. Similarly, a cultural product analysis showed that popular children's books from the United States and Belgium tend to portray culturally beneficial emotions more than culturally harmful emotions. Finally, a word-association study of the shared cultural meanings surrounding anger and shame provided commensurate evidence at the level of the associated response. In each language network, anger and shame were imbued with meanings that reflected the cultural significance of the emotion: while culturally consistent emotions carried relatively stronger connotations of emotional yielding (e.g., giving in to anger and aggressing against the offender in the United States), culturally inconsistent emotions carried relatively stronger connotations of emotional containment (e.g., a stronger emphasis on suppressing or transforming shame in the United States).
- Published
- 2013
13. Optimization of SMD assembly systems regarding dynamical and thermal behaviors.
- Author
-
Feldmann, K., Krimi, S., Boiger, M., and Zolleiss, B.
- Published
- 2001
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
14. Optimization of SMD assembly systems regarding dynamical and thermal behaviors
- Author
-
Feldmann, K., primary, Krimi, S., additional, Boiger, M., additional, and Zolleiss, B., additional
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
15. The Relationality of Feelings: A cultural comparison of affective patterns in Western and East-Asian relationships : Gevoelens in relatie: een cultureel vergelijkende studie van affectieve patronen in Westerse en Oost Aziatische relaties
- Author
-
Kirchner-Hausler, A, Boiger, M, Kuppens, P, and Gomes de Mesquita, B
- Abstract
Western and East-Asian cultures appear to emphasize different ideas about how to build and maintain relationships with others around us: Close relationships in Western cultures are marked by a strong focus on the individual, and are supposed to offer partners a space to foster self-esteem and meet their personal needs. In contrast, close relationships in East-Asian cultures are marked by an emphasis on the relatedness and social harmony between individuals, highlighting the importance of first and foremost anticipating and adjusting to the needs and expectations of others (Kitayama & Markus, 2000; Rothbaum, Pott, Azuma, Miyake, & Weisz, 2000). These cultural differences in relationship practices have implications for the feelings that are likely to arise in close relationships. Our emotions play a critical role when it comes to forming and maintaining relationships with others: they reflect and communicate our values and needs towards others, and help us to negotiate and shape relationships in ways that are appropriate and fulfilling (Mesquita, 2010). Accordingly, as relationship practices and goals vary between cultures, so should the most prominent feelings that emerge in these relationships, supporting partners in pursuing these central cultural relationship goals and in maintaining relationships in culturally appropriate ways. Previous cross-cultural studies have indeed shown meaningful cultural differences in the emotion norms and experiences of individuals (Miyamoto, Ma, & Wilken, 2017), but our understanding is still lacking when it comes to whether these cultural differences reflect cultural relationship models, and how these differences in emotions play out in actual relationships. The present dissertation therefore set out to study if and how emotions in close relationship vary (systematically) across Western and East-Asian cultures. We hypothesized that (1) close relationships in Western and East-Asian cultures would each be characterized relatively more by those feelings that are consistent with, and support, the respective cultural relationship goals, that (2) close relationships in the two cultures are marked relatively more by different affective temporal dynamics that are geared towards the experience of emotions that are consistent with the respective cultural relationship goals, and that (3) both the emotions, and the affective dynamics, that support cultural relationship goals are associated with better relational wellbeing in the respective cultures. In examining these hypotheses, we specifically focused on the emotional experience in social, interpersonal situations, where relationships are repeatedly negotiated, and on the experience of positive and negative feelings as cross-culturally reliable indicators of affective experience. My thesis addresses the research aims in three different studies, utilizing different methodological (e.g. daily diary, standardized interactions, situation vignettes) and analytical approaches (e.g. multilevel modeling, structural equation modeling, sequential analysis). My dissertation starts with studies describing cultural differences in specific emotions of individuals from a Western (United States) and an East-Asian cultural context (Japan). Following this, a large portion of this dissertation builds upon the experience of positive and negative affect in relationships, drawn from a cross-cultural, multi-method study conducted with romantic couples from Belgium and Japan. Results converge to support the idea that culture systematically and meaningfully shapes emotions and emotional patterns in close relationships and social situations. status: published
- Published
- 2019
16. Couple conflict observed: Emotions in Belgium and Japan.
- Author
-
Schouten A, Boiger M, Uchida A, Verstaen A, Paillé C, Uchida Y, and Mesquita B
- Subjects
- Humans, Belgium, Female, Male, Adult, Japan ethnology, Interpersonal Relations, Conflict, Psychological, Personal Satisfaction, Family Characteristics ethnology, Spouses psychology, Emotions, Cross-Cultural Comparison
- Abstract
In the present study, we examined cultural variation in couples' emotions during disagreement. We coded the emotions of 58 Belgian and 80 Japanese couples using the Specific Affect Coding System. We observed more anger and domineering, but less fear/tension and other-validation in Belgian than in Japanese couples. Moreover, in Japanese couples, culturally typical emotions were associated with higher conflict resolution and relationship satisfaction. The findings suggest meaningful cultural differences in couples' observed emotions during disagreement, as they can be understood from the prevailing relationship ideals in each culture. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
17. Different bumps in the road: The emotional dynamics of couple disagreements in Belgium and Japan.
- Author
-
Boiger M, Kirchner-Häusler A, Schouten A, Uchida Y, and Mesquita B
- Subjects
- Anger, Belgium, Cross-Cultural Comparison, Humans, Japan, Emotions, Empathy, Interpersonal Relations
- Abstract
In the present study, we propose that the emotional "bumps" that couples experience during relationship disagreements differ systematically among cultures. We predicted that self-assertive emotions such as anger or strength play a central role in Belgium, where they are instrumental for relational independence. In comparison, other-focused emotions such as shame or empathy for the partner should play a central role in Japan, where they support relational interdependence. Romantic couples from Belgium ( n = 58) and Japan ( n = 80) discussed relationship disagreements in the lab, which were video-recorded. After the interaction, participants separately rated their emotional experience during video-mediated recall. We identified the emotions that played a central role during the interactions in terms of attractors; these are the emotions around which couples stabilize and that likely play a central role in realizing different relationship ideals. In line with our predictions, attractors reflected states of the interpersonal emotional system that support independence in Belgium (e.g., angry or strong feelings) and interdependence (e.g., empathy) in Japan. Moreover, we found that-at least in Belgium-having more culturally typical interactions was associated with a stronger endorsement of culturally valued relationship ideals and, in turn, better relational functioning. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
18. Cultural Differences in Emotion Suppression in Belgian and Japanese Couples: A Social Functional Model.
- Author
-
Schouten A, Boiger M, Kirchner-Häusler A, Uchida Y, and Mesquita B
- Abstract
Emotion suppression has been found to have negative psychological and social consequences in Western cultural contexts. Yet, in some other cultural contexts, emotion suppression is less likely to have negative consequences; relatedly, emotion suppression is also more common in those East-Asian cultural contexts. In a dyadic conflict study, we aim to (a) conceptually replicate cultural differences found in previous research with respect to the prevalence and consequences of emotion suppression, and (b) extend previous research by testing whether cultural differences are larger for some than for other types of negative emotions. We postulate that cultural differences in suppression are less pronounced for socially engaging emotions (e.g., guilt) than socially disengaging emotions (e.g., anger), because the former foster the relationship, whereas the latter emphasize individual goals. Belgian ( N = 58) and Japanese ( N = 80) couples engaged in a 10-min conflict interaction followed by video-mediated recall, during which participants rated their emotions and emotion suppression every 30 s. As predicted, Japanese participants reported more suppression than their Belgian counterparts, but the cultural difference was more pronounced when participants experienced more socially disengaging emotions than when they experienced more socially engaging emotions. These results suggest that the type of emotion should be considered when describing cultural differences in emotion suppression. Finally, and consistent with previous research, emotion suppression was negatively associated with interaction outcomes (i.e., conflict resolution) in Belgian couples, but not in Japanese couples., (Copyright © 2020 Schouten, Boiger, Kirchner-Häusler, Uchida and Mesquita.)
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
19. When Holding in Prevents From Reaching Out: Emotion Suppression and Social Support-Seeking in Multicultural Groups.
- Author
-
Boroş S, van Gorp L, and Boiger M
- Abstract
Members of multicultural groups benefit from developing diverse social support networks. Engaging openly with people who have a different worldview (i.e., given by a different cultural background) broadens one's cognitive horizons, facilitates one's adaptation to new contexts, decreases stereotyping and discrimination and generally improves individual and group performance. However, if this social connection is hindered (either by limiting the number of people one reaches out to or in terms of preferring to connect to similar others), then the diversity advantage is lost - both for the individuals and for the groups. Through two case studies of professional groups with varying cultural diversity (moderate and superdiverse), we investigate the evolution of their members' social support networks (i.e., to what extent and to whom they reach out for support) depending on (1) individuals' habitual emotion suppression and (2) cultural orientation on the individualism-collectivism dimension. Results show that individualistic cultures suffer a double-whammy: when suppressing, their members seek less support (i.e., don't reach out so much to ask for support) and tend to seek culturally similar others for it when they do. Suppressing collectivists are less affected in absolute levels of connectedness, but still prefer culturally similar others as sources of support. Our study offers an emotion-based view of why people stick together with similar others in diverse groups and how learning to better cope with emotions can make us more open-minded toward diversity in professional settings., (Copyright © 2019 Boroş, van Gorp and Boiger.)
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
20. Beyond essentialism: Cultural differences in emotions revisited.
- Author
-
Boiger M, Ceulemans E, De Leersnyder J, Uchida Y, Norasakkunkit V, and Mesquita B
- Subjects
- Adult, Female, Humans, Male, Shame, Young Adult, Anger physiology, Culture, Emotions physiology
- Abstract
The current research offers an alternative to essentialism for studying cultural variation in emotional experience. Rather than assuming that individuals always experience an emotion in the same way, our starting point was that the experience of an emotion like anger or shame may vary from one instance to another. We expected to find different anger and shame experience types, that is, groups of people who differ in the instances of anger and shame that they experience. We proposed that studying cultural differences in emotional experience means studying differences in the distribution of these types across cultural contexts: There should be systematic differences in the types that are most common in each culture. Students from the United States, Japan, and Belgium (N = 928) indicated their emotional experiences in terms of appraisals and action tendencies in response to 15 hypothetical anger or shame situations. Using an inductive clustering approach, we identified anger and shame types who were characterized by different patterns of anger and shame experience. As expected, we found that the distribution of these types differed across the three cultural contexts: Of the two anger types, one was common in Japan and one in the United States and Belgium; the three shame types were each most prevalent in a different cultural context. Participants' anger and shame types were primarily predicted by their culture of origin (with an accuracy of 72.3% for anger and 74.0% for shame) and not, or much less, by their ethnic origin, socioeconomic status, gender, self-construal, or personality. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2018 APA, all rights reserved).
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
21. Humiliated fury is not universal: the co-occurrence of anger and shame in the United States and Japan.
- Author
-
Kirchner A, Boiger M, Uchida Y, Norasakkunkit V, Verduyn P, and Mesquita B
- Subjects
- Female, Humans, Japan, Male, United States, White People psychology, Young Adult, Anger, Asian People psychology, Embarrassment, Shame
- Abstract
It has been widely believed that individuals transform high-intensity shame into anger because shame is unbearably painful. This phenomenon was first coined "humiliated fury," and it has since received empirical support. The current research tests the novel hypothesis that shame-related anger is not universal, yet hinges on the cultural meanings of anger and shame. Two studies compared the occurrence of shame-related anger in North American cultural contexts (where shame is devalued and anger is valued) to its occurrence in Japanese contexts (where shame is valued and anger is devalued). In a daily-diary study, participants rated anger and shame feelings during shame situations that occurred over one week. In a vignette study, participants rated anger and shame in response to standardised shame vignettes that were generated in previous research by either U.S. or Japanese respondents. Across the two studies, and in line with previous research on humiliated fury, shame predicted anger for U.S., Participants: Yet, neither in the daily diary study nor for the Japanese-origin vignettes, did we find shame-related anger in Japanese participants. Only when presented with U.S.-origin vignettes, did Japanese respondents in the vignette study report shame-related anger. The findings suggest that shame-related anger is a culture-specific phenomenon.
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
22. The cultural construction of emotions.
- Author
-
Mesquita B, Boiger M, and De Leersnyder J
- Abstract
A large body of anthropological and psychological research on emotions has yielded significant evidence that emotional experience is culturally constructed: people more commonly experience those emotions that help them to be a good and typical person in their culture. Moreover, experiencing these culturally normative emotions is associated with greater well-being. In this review, we summarize recent research showing how emotions are actively constructed to meet the demands of the respective cultural environment; we discuss collective as well as individual processes of construction. By focusing on cultural construction of emotion, we shift the focus toward how people from different cultures 'do' emotions and away from which emotions they 'have'., (Copyright © 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.)
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
23. Defending honour, keeping face: Interpersonal affordances of anger and shame in Turkey and Japan.
- Author
-
Boiger M, Güngör D, Karasawa M, and Mesquita B
- Subjects
- Cross-Cultural Comparison, Female, Humans, Japan, Male, Surveys and Questionnaires, Turkey, Young Adult, Anger, Asian People psychology, Interpersonal Relations, Shame
- Abstract
In the present study, we tested the idea that emotions are afforded to the extent that they benefit central cultural concerns. We predicted that emotions that are beneficial for the Turkish concern for defending honour (both anger and shame) are afforded frequently in Turkey, whereas emotions that are beneficial for the Japanese concern for keeping face (shame but not anger) are afforded frequently in Japan. N=563 students from Turkey and Japan indicated how frequently people in their culture experience a range of interpersonal anger and shame situations, and how intense their emotions would be. As predicted, participants perceived emotional interactions to occur frequently to the extent that they elicited culturally beneficial emotions. Moreover, the affordance of culturally beneficial emotions differed in predictable ways not only between cultures but also within cultures between situations with close vs. distant others and male vs. female protagonists.
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
24. Emotions in "the world": cultural practices, products, and meanings of anger and shame in two individualist cultures.
- Author
-
Boiger M, Deyne SD, and Mesquita B
- Abstract
Three studies tested the idea that people's cultural worlds are structured in ways that promote and highlight emotions and emotional responses that are beneficial in achieving central goals in their culture. Based on the idea that U.S. Americans strive for competitive individualism, while (Dutch-speaking) Belgians favor a more egalitarian variant of individualism, we predicted that anger and shame, as well as their associated responses, would be beneficial to different extents in these two cultural contexts. A questionnaire study found that cultural practices promote beneficial emotions (anger in the United States, shame in Belgium) and avoid harmful emotions (shame in the United States): emotional interactions were perceived to occur more or less frequently to the extent that they elicited culturally beneficial or harmful emotions. Similarly, a cultural product analysis showed that popular children's books from the United States and Belgium tend to portray culturally beneficial emotions more than culturally harmful emotions. Finally, a word-association study of the shared cultural meanings surrounding anger and shame provided commensurate evidence at the level of the associated response. In each language network, anger and shame were imbued with meanings that reflected the cultural significance of the emotion: while culturally consistent emotions carried relatively stronger connotations of emotional yielding (e.g., giving in to anger and aggressing against the offender in the United States), culturally inconsistent emotions carried relatively stronger connotations of emotional containment (e.g., a stronger emphasis on suppressing or transforming shame in the United States).
- Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
25. Condoned or condemned: the situational affordance of anger and shame in the United States and Japan.
- Author
-
Boiger M, Mesquita B, Uchida Y, and Feldman Barrett L
- Subjects
- Adult, Culture, Emotions, Female, Guilt, Humans, Interviews as Topic, Japan, Male, Students psychology, Surveys and Questionnaires, United States, Young Adult, Anger, Cross-Cultural Comparison, Shame, Social Environment
- Abstract
Two studies tested the idea that the situations that people encounter frequently and the situations that they associate most strongly with an emotion differ across cultures in ways that can be understood from what a culture condones or condemns. In a questionnaire study, N = 163 students from the United States and Japan perceived situations as more frequent to the extent that they elicited condoned emotions (anger in the United States, shame in Japan), and they perceived situations as less frequent to the extent that they elicited condemned emotions (shame in the United States, anger in Japan). In a second study, N = 160 students from the United States and Japan free-sorted the same situations. For each emotion, the situations could be organized along two cross-culturally common dimensions. Those situations that touched upon central cultural concerns were perceived to elicit stronger emotions. The largest cultural differences were found for shame; smaller, yet meaningful, differences were found for anger.
- Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
26. Cultural regulation of emotion: individual, relational, and structural sources.
- Author
-
De Leersnyder J, Boiger M, and Mesquita B
- Abstract
The most prevalent and intense emotional experiences differ across cultures. These differences in emotional experience can be understood as the outcomes of emotion regulation, because emotions that fit the valued relationships within a culture tend to be most common and intense. We review evidence suggesting that emotion regulation underlying cultural differences in emotional experience often takes place at the point of emotion elicitation through the promotion of situations and appraisals that are consistent with culturally valued relationships. These regulatory processes depend on individual tendencies, but are also co-regulated within relationships-close others shape people's environment and help them appraise events in culturally valued ways-and are afforded by structural conditions-people's daily lives "limit" the opportunities for emotion, and afford certain appraisals. The combined evidence suggests that cultural differences in emotion regulation go well beyond the effortful regulation based on display rules.
- Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
27. Influencing and adjusting in daily emotional situations: a comparison of European and Asian American action styles.
- Author
-
Boiger M, Mesquita B, Tsai AY, and Markus H
- Subjects
- Adult, Female, Humans, Male, Asian psychology, Cross-Cultural Comparison, Emotions, Leadership, Social Adjustment, White People psychology
- Abstract
Emotions are for action, but action styles in emotional episodes may vary across cultural contexts. Based on culturally different models of agency, we expected that those who engage in European-American contexts will use more influence in emotional situations, while those who engage in East-Asian contexts will use more adjustment. European-American (N=60) and Asian-American (N=44) college students reported their action style during emotional episodes four times a day during a week. Asian Americans adjusted more than European Americans, whereas both used influence to a similar extent. These cultural differences in action style varied across types of emotion experienced. Moreover, influencing was associated with life satisfaction for European Americans, but not for Asian Americans.
- Published
- 2012
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.