Lung cancer is the second most common form of cancer but the leading cause of cancer death in both men and women (Siegel, Naishadham, & Jemal, 2013). In 2013, an estimated 228,190 individuals will be diagnosed with lung cancer and 159,480 will die from the disease (American Cancer Society [ACS], 2013). Although advances in knowledge about cancer biology and improvements in early diagnosis and treatment have increased the opportunities for long-term survival, the prognosis remains poor with a five-year survival rate of only 16% (ACS, 2013). As such, the impact of the disease and treatment on patients’ symptoms and quality of life (QOL) requires exploration. The vast majority of patients with lung cancer are diagnosed with advanced disease, a high burden of symptoms (i.e., dyspnea, hemoptysis, cough, and chest pain), poorer QOL, and shortened survival (Buchanan, Milroy, Baker, Thompson, & Levack, 2009; Hopwood & Stephens, 1995, 2000; Siegel et al., 2013; Steinberg et al., 2009). Research has demonstrated that early integration of symptom management leads to meaningful improvements in QOL, mood, and survival in patients with lung cancer (Temel et al., 2010). However, few studies have focused on sleep as a factor influencing symptom burden and QOL. Individuals with lung cancer are reported as having the highest or second highest level of sleep disturbances relative to other cancers and noncancer controls, and so may be a particularly at-risk population for sleep problems (Davidson, MacLean, Brundage, & Schulze, 2002; Palesh et al., 2010). A Canadian study using well-established diagnostic criteria and involving 982 outpatients revealed that those with lung cancer (n = 114) had a higher prevalence of sleep problems, including excessive daytime sleepiness, sleeping more than usual, severe fatigue, and using sleeping pills more often than patients with other solid tumors (Davidson et al., 2002). Only patients with breast cancer had a higher prevalence of insomnia than patients with lung cancer (Davidson et al., 2002). The authors found that patients with lung cancer were diagnosed and treated earlier than other patients with cancer. The median time from diagnosis for all cancers was 34 months, whereas the median time from diagnosis for patients with lung cancer was only 11 months. This suggests that earlier treatment interferes with sleep (Davidson et al., 2002). Palesh et al. (2010) used the Hamilton Depression Inventory, which included six questions assessing frequency and duration of sleep, to study 823 patients undergoing chemotherapy, and found that patients with lung cancer (n = 120) had the highest prevalence of insomnia syndrome when compared to patients with breast, gynecologic, hematologic, or alimentary tract cancers. Insomnia syndrome is defined as difficulty falling asleep, difficulty staying asleep (waking up in the middle of the night), and/or early morning awakenings for at least three days a week for two weeks, with each episode lasting at least 30 minutes. Patients with insomnia syndrome had significantly more fatigue and depression than patients without insomnia syndrome (Palesh et al., 2010). These previous studies provide evidence for the increased prevalence of self-reported sleep disturbances in patients during treatment for lung cancer. Disturbances of mood have an impact on sleep disturbances in patients with lung cancer. Previous estimates have suggested that 15%–40% of individuals with lung cancer have a clinically significant level of anxiety, meeting the criteria for referral (Greer, Pirl, Park, Lynch, & Temel, 2008; Savard, Villa, Ivers, Simard, & Morin, 2009), and 21%–44% are depressed (Hopwood & Stephens, 2000; Turner, Muers, Haward, & Mulley, 2007). Sleep disturbances are included in two of the six categories for anxiety disorders in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (American Psychiatric Association, 2000), suggesting that sleep disturbance and anxiety are clinically linked. Sleep requires diminished cortical arousal, whereas anxiety manifests with heightened cortical and peripheral arousal (Stein & Mellman, 2005). A review of sleep electroencephalogram (EEG) studies found that sleep homeostasis was impaired and slow wave sleep activity was reduced in depression (Armitage, 2007). Silberfarb, Hauri, Oxman, and Schnurr (1993) screened for depression and anxiety and reported that patients with lung cancer scored within normal limits, but slept as poorly as insomniacs. Only 21% of people with cancer who complained of insomnia had concurrent psychopathology (Ginsburg, Quirt, Ginsburg, & MacKillop, 1995)—a rate similar to overlap rates seen in the general older adult population (Foley et al., 1995). Still, some of the ascribed causes of severe insomnia were related to anxiety, depression, and guilt (Ginsburg et al., 1995). Sleep disturbances in patients with lung cancer have significant adverse effects on symptoms and QOL (Degner & Sloan, 1995; Gooneratne et al., 2007). Pain was significantly worse in patients with lung cancer with insomnia compared to patients without insomnia (Rumble, Keefe, Edinger, Porter, & Garst, 2005). Pain, fatigue, and dyspnea all were significantly correlated with sleep disturbance and, after controlling for all covariates, sleep disturbance was significantly associated with impaired cognitive function and poorer functional status in 115 patients receiving chemotherapy for lung cancer (Chen, Yu, & Yang, 2008). QOL in lung cancer survivors (n = 76) was not significantly different from non-cancer controls (n = 78), but lung cancer survivors with poorer sleep quality had significantly worse QOL than those with good sleep quality (Gooneratne et al., 2007). These studies suggest that newly diagnosed patients and those receiving treatment for lung cancer have increased symptoms and significantly worse QOL compared to controls. In addition, lung cancer survivors with poor quality sleep have significantly worse QOL than those with good sleep quality. Much of the previous research on sleep in patients with lung cancer has focused on self-reported sleep measures. Few studies have used both subjective and objective measures to evaluate sleep. The purpose of this study was to characterize and compare relationships among sleep, mood, and QOL using both subjective and objective measures of sleep in patients receiving treatment for lung cancer. The authors hypothesized that some discrepancies would exist between objective and subjective measures (Schlesinger, Hering-Hanit, & Dagan, 2001; Vitiello, Larsen, & Moe, 2004), but associations also would exist between objective and subjective measures and that mood disorders and poorer QOL would be highly associated with self-reported sleep disturbance. This study was guided by the Two Process Model of Sleep Regulation, one of the leading theories of sleep regulation (Borbely, 1982; Borbely & Achermann, 2005). This model suggests that sleep occurs when the homeostatic drive to sleep (Process S) approaches an upper threshold, and awakening from sleep occurs when Process S reaches a lower threshold. The need to sleep increases as the amount of prior wakefulness increases. In other words, an increase in sleep pressure occurs as wakefulness is prolonged and for a recovery process during sleep. The second process, Process C, signifies the circadian control of sleep propensity or chance for sleep. The circadian system communicates time of day (light and dark cues from the retina) and thereby regulates the change from wakefulness to sleep and the change from sleep to wakefulness. The interaction between these two processes forms the basis for a consistent period of sleep at night and a well-ordered period of wakefulness during the day.