50 results on '"Anat Perry"'
Search Results
2. Considering the Role of Human Empathy in AI-Driven Therapy
- Author
-
Matan Rubin, Hadar Arnon, Jonathan D Huppert, and Anat Perry
- Subjects
Psychology ,BF1-990 - Abstract
Recent breakthroughs in artificial intelligence (AI) language models have elevated the vision of using conversational AI support for mental health, with a growing body of literature indicating varying degrees of efficacy. In this paper, we ask when, in therapy, it will be easier to replace humans and, conversely, in what instances, human connection will still be more valued. We suggest that empathy lies at the heart of the answer to this question. First, we define different aspects of empathy and outline the potential empathic capabilities of humans versus AI. Next, we consider what determines when these aspects are needed most in therapy, both from the perspective of therapeutic methodology and from the perspective of patient objectives. Ultimately, our goal is to prompt further investigation and dialogue, urging both practitioners and scholars engaged in AI-mediated therapy to keep these questions and considerations in mind when investigating AI implementation in mental health.
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
3. Social and moral psychology of COVID-19 across 69 countries
- Author
-
Flavio Azevedo, Tomislav Pavlović, Gabriel G. Rêgo, F. Ceren Ay, Biljana Gjoneska, Tom W. Etienne, Robert M. Ross, Philipp Schönegger, Julián C. Riaño-Moreno, Aleksandra Cichocka, Valerio Capraro, Luca Cian, Chiara Longoni, Ho Fai Chan, Jay J. Van Bavel, Hallgeir Sjåstad, John B. Nezlek, Mark Alfano, Michele J. Gelfand, Michèle D. Birtel, Aleksandra Cislak, Patricia L. Lockwood, Koen Abts, Elena Agadullina, John Jamir Benzon Aruta, Sahba Nomvula Besharati, Alexander Bor, Becky L. Choma, Charles David Crabtree, William A. Cunningham, Koustav De, Waqas Ejaz, Christian T. Elbaek, Andrej Findor, Daniel Flichtentrei, Renata Franc, June Gruber, Estrella Gualda, Yusaku Horiuchi, Toan Luu Duc Huynh, Agustin Ibanez, Mostak Ahamed Imran, Jacob Israelashvili, Katarzyna Jasko, Jaroslaw Kantorowicz, Elena Kantorowicz-Reznichenko, André Krouwel, Michael Laakasuo, Claus Lamm, Caroline Leygue, Ming-Jen Lin, Mohammad Sabbir Mansoor, Antoine Marie, Lewend Mayiwar, Honorata Mazepus, Cillian McHugh, John Paul Minda, Panagiotis Mitkidis, Andreas Olsson, Tobias Otterbring, Dominic J. Packer, Anat Perry, Michael Bang Petersen, Arathy Puthillam, Tobias Rothmund, Hernando Santamaría-García, Petra C. Schmid, Drozdstoy Stoyanov, Shruti Tewari, Bojan Todosijević, Manos Tsakiris, Hans H. Tung, Radu G. Umbres, Edmunds Vanags, Madalina Vlasceanu, Andrew Vonasch, Meltem Yucel, Yucheng Zhang, Mohcine Abad, Eli Adler, Narin Akrawi, Hamza Alaoui Mdarhri, Hanane Amara, David M. Amodio, Benedict G. Antazo, Matthew Apps, Mouhamadou Hady Ba, Sergio Barbosa, Brock Bastian, Anton Berg, Maria P. Bernal-Zárate, Michael Bernstein, Michał Białek, Ennio Bilancini, Natalia Bogatyreva, Leonardo Boncinelli, Jonathan E. Booth, Sylvie Borau, Ondrej Buchel, C. Daryl Cameron, Chrissie F. Carvalho, Tatiana Celadin, Chiara Cerami, Hom Nath Chalise, Xiaojun Cheng, Kate Cockcroft, Jane Conway, Mateo Andres Córdoba-Delgado, Chiara Crespi, Marie Crouzevialle, Jo Cutler, Marzena Cypryańska, Justyna Dabrowska, Michael A. Daniels, Victoria H. Davis, Pamala N. Dayley, Sylvain Delouvée, Ognjan Denkovski, Guillaume Dezecache, Nathan A. Dhaliwal, Alelie B. Diato, Roberto Di Paolo, Marianna Drosinou, Uwe Dulleck, Jānis Ekmanis, Arhan S. Ertan, Hapsa Hossain Farhana, Fahima Farkhari, Harry Farmer, Ali Fenwick, Kristijan Fidanovski, Terry Flew, Shona Fraser, Raymond Boadi Frempong, Jonathan A. Fugelsang, Jessica Gale, E. Begoña Garcia-Navarro, Prasad Garladinne, Oussama Ghajjou, Theofilos Gkinopoulos, Kurt Gray, Siobhán M. Griffin, Bjarki Gronfeldt, Mert Gümren, Ranju Lama Gurung, Eran Halperin, Elizabeth Harris, Volo Herzon, Matej Hruška, Guanxiong Huang, Matthias F. C. Hudecek, Ozan Isler, Simon Jangard, Frederik J. Jorgensen, Frank Kachanoff, John Kahn, Apsara Katuwal Dangol, Oleksandra Keudel, Lina Koppel, Mika Koverola, Emily Kubin, Anton Kunnari, Yordan Kutiyski, Oscar Moreda Laguna, Josh Leota, Eva Lermer, Jonathan Levy, Neil Levy, Chunyun Li, Elizabeth U. Long, Marina Maglić, Darragh McCashin, Alexander L. Metcalf, Igor Mikloušić, Soulaimane El Mimouni, Asako Miura, Juliana Molina-Paredes, César Monroy-Fonseca, Elena Morales-Marente, David Moreau, Rafał Muda, Annalisa Myer, Kyle Nash, Tarik Nesh-Nash, Jonas P. Nitschke, Matthew S. Nurse, Yohsuke Ohtsubo, Victoria Oldemburgo de Mello, Cathal O’Madagain, Michal Onderco, M. Soledad Palacios-Galvez, Jussi Palomöki, Yafeng Pan, Zsófia Papp, Philip Pärnamets, Mariola Paruzel-Czachura, Zoran Pavlović, César Payán-Gómez, Silva Perander, Michael Mark Pitman, Rajib Prasad, Joanna Pyrkosz-Pacyna, Steve Rathje, Ali Raza, Kasey Rhee, Claire E. Robertson, Iván Rodríguez-Pascual, Teemu Saikkonen, Octavio Salvador-Ginez, Gaia C. Santi, Natalia Santiago-Tovar, David Savage, Julian A. Scheffer, David T. Schultner, Enid M. Schutte, Andy Scott, Madhavi Sharma, Pujan Sharma, Ahmed Skali, David Stadelmann, Clara Alexandra Stafford, Dragan Stanojević, Anna Stefaniak, Anni Sternisko, Augustin Stoica, Kristina K. Stoyanova, Brent Strickland, Jukka Sundvall, Jeffrey P. Thomas, Gustav Tinghög, Benno Torgler, Iris J. Traast, Raffaele Tucciarelli, Michael Tyrala, Nick D. Ungson, Mete S. Uysal, Paul A. M. Van Lange, Jan-Willem van Prooijen, Dirk van Rooy, Daniel Västfjäll, Peter Verkoeijen, Joana B. Vieira, Christian von Sikorski, Alexander Cameron Walker, Jennifer Watermeyer, Erik Wetter, Ashley Whillans, Katherine White, Rishad Habib, Robin Willardt, Michael J. A. Wohl, Adrian Dominik Wójcik, Kaidi Wu, Yuki Yamada, Onurcan Yilmaz, Kumar Yogeeswaran, Carolin-Theresa Ziemer, Rolf A. Zwaan, Paulo S. Boggio, and Waldir M. Sampaio
- Subjects
Science - Abstract
Abstract The COVID-19 pandemic has affected all domains of human life, including the economic and social fabric of societies. One of the central strategies for managing public health throughout the pandemic has been through persuasive messaging and collective behaviour change. To help scholars better understand the social and moral psychology behind public health behaviour, we present a dataset comprising of 51,404 individuals from 69 countries. This dataset was collected for the International Collaboration on Social & Moral Psychology of COVID-19 project (ICSMP COVID-19). This social science survey invited participants around the world to complete a series of moral and psychological measures and public health attitudes about COVID-19 during an early phase of the COVID-19 pandemic (between April and June 2020). The survey included seven broad categories of questions: COVID-19 beliefs and compliance behaviours; identity and social attitudes; ideology; health and well-being; moral beliefs and motivation; personality traits; and demographic variables. We report both raw and cleaned data, along with all survey materials, data visualisations, and psychometric evaluations of key variables.
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
4. National identity predicts public health support during a global pandemic
- Author
-
Jay J. Van Bavel, Aleksandra Cichocka, Valerio Capraro, Hallgeir Sjåstad, John B. Nezlek, Tomislav Pavlović, Mark Alfano, Michele J. Gelfand, Flavio Azevedo, Michèle D. Birtel, Aleksandra Cislak, Patricia L. Lockwood, Robert Malcolm Ross, Koen Abts, Elena Agadullina, John Jamir Benzon Aruta, Sahba Nomvula Besharati, Alexander Bor, Becky L. Choma, Charles David Crabtree, William A. Cunningham, Koustav De, Waqas Ejaz, Christian T. Elbaek, Andrej Findor, Daniel Flichtentrei, Renata Franc, Biljana Gjoneska, June Gruber, Estrella Gualda, Yusaku Horiuchi, Toan Luu Duc Huynh, Augustin Ibanez, Mostak Ahamed Imran, Jacob Israelashvili, Katarzyna Jasko, Jaroslaw Kantorowicz, Elena Kantorowicz-Reznichenko, André Krouwel, Michael Laakasuo, Claus Lamm, Caroline Leygue, Ming-Jen Lin, Mohammad Sabbir Mansoor, Antoine Marie, Lewend Mayiwar, Honorata Mazepus, Cillian McHugh, John Paul Minda, Panagiotis Mitkidis, Andreas Olsson, Tobias Otterbring, Dominic J. Packer, Anat Perry, Michael Bang Petersen, Arathy Puthillam, Julián C. Riaño-Moreno, Tobias Rothmund, Hernando Santamaría-García, Petra C. Schmid, Drozdstoy Stoyanov, Shruti Tewari, Bojan Todosijević, Manos Tsakiris, Hans H. Tung, Radu G. Umbreș, Edmunds Vanags, Madalina Vlasceanu, Andrew Vonasch, Meltem Yucel, Yucheng Zhang, Mohcine Abad, Eli Adler, Narin Akrawi, Hamza Alaoui Mdarhri, Hanane Amara, David M. Amodio, Benedict G. Antazo, Matthew Apps, F. Ceren Ay, Mouhamadou Hady Ba, Sergio Barbosa, Brock Bastian, Anton Berg, Maria P. Bernal-Zárate, Michael Bernstein, Michał Białek, Ennio Bilancini, Natalia Bogatyreva, Leonardo Boncinelli, Jonathan E. Booth, Sylvie Borau, Ondrej Buchel, C. Daryl Cameron, Chrissie F. Carvalho, Tatiana Celadin, Chiara Cerami, Hom Nath Chalise, Xiaojun Cheng, Luca Cian, Kate Cockcroft, Jane Conway, Mateo Andres Córdoba-Delgado, Chiara Crespi, Marie Crouzevialle, Jo Cutler, Marzena Cypryańska, Justyna Dabrowska, Michael A. Daniels, Victoria H. Davis, Pamala N. Dayley, Sylvain Delouvee, Ognjan Denkovski, Guillaume Dezecache, Nathan A. Dhaliwal, Alelie B. Diato, Roberto Di Paolo, Marianna Drosinou, Uwe Dulleck, Jānis Ekmanis, Arhan S. Ertan, Tom W. Etienne, Hapsa Hossain Farhana, Fahima Farkhari, Harry Farmer, Ali Fenwick, Kristijan Fidanovski, Terry Flew, Shona Fraser, Raymond Boadi Frempong, Jonathan A. Fugelsang, Jessica Gale, E. Begoña Garcia-Navarro, Prasad Garladinne, Oussama Ghajjou, Theofilos Gkinopoulos, Kurt Gray, Siobhán M. Griffin, Bjarki Gronfeldt, Mert Gümren, Ranju Lama Gurung, Eran Halperin, Elizabeth Harris, Volo Herzon, Matej Hruška, Guanxiong Huang, Matthias F. C. Hudecek, Ozan Isler, Simon Jangard, Frederik J. Jørgensen, Frank Kachanoff, John Kahn, Apsara Katuwal Dangol, Oleksandra Keudel, Lina Koppel, Mika Koverola, Emily Kubin, Anton Kunnari, Yordan Kutiyski, Oscar Laguna, Josh Leota, Eva Lermer, Jonathan Levy, Neil Levy, Chunyun Li, Elizabeth U. Long, Chiara Longoni, Marina Maglić, Darragh McCashin, Alexander L. Metcalf, Igor Mikloušić, Soulaimane El Mimouni, Asako Miura, Juliana Molina-Paredes, César Monroy-Fonseca, Elena Morales-Marente, David Moreau, Rafał Muda, Annalisa Myer, Kyle Nash, Tarik Nesh-Nash, Jonas P. Nitschke, Matthew S. Nurse, Yohsuke Ohtsubo, Victoria Oldemburgo de Mello, Cathal O’Madagain, Michal Onderco, M. Soledad Palacios-Galvez, Jussi Palomäki, Yafeng Pan, Zsófia Papp, Philip Pärnamets, Mariola Paruzel-Czachura, Zoran Pavlović, César Payán-Gómez, Silva Perander, Michael Mark Pitman, Rajib Prasad, Joanna Pyrkosz-Pacyna, Steve Rathje, Ali Raza, Gabriel G. Rêgo, Kasey Rhee, Claire E. Robertson, Iván Rodríguez-Pascual, Teemu Saikkonen, Octavio Salvador-Ginez, Waldir M. Sampaio, Gaia C. Santi, Natalia Santiago-Tovar, David Savage, Julian A. Scheffer, Philipp Schönegger, David T. Schultner, Enid M. Schutte, Andy Scott, Madhavi Sharma, Pujan Sharma, Ahmed Skali, David Stadelmann, Clara Alexandra Stafford, Dragan Stanojević, Anna Stefaniak, Anni Sternisko, Augustin Stoica, Kristina K. Stoyanova, Brent Strickland, Jukka Sundvall, Jeffrey P. Thomas, Gustav Tinghög, Benno Torgler, Iris J. Traast, Raffaele Tucciarelli, Michael Tyrala, Nick D. Ungson, Mete S. Uysal, Paul A. M. Van Lange, Jan-Willem van Prooijen, Dirk van Rooy, Daniel Västfjäll, Peter Verkoeijen, Joana B. Vieira, Christian von Sikorski, Alexander Cameron Walker, Jennifer Watermeyer, Erik Wetter, Ashley Whillans, Robin Willardt, Michael J. A. Wohl, Adrian Dominik Wójcik, Kaidi Wu, Yuki Yamada, Onurcan Yilmaz, Kumar Yogeeswaran, Carolin-Theresa Ziemer, Rolf A. Zwaan, and Paulo S. Boggio
- Subjects
Science - Abstract
Understanding collective behaviour is an important aspect of managing the pandemic response. Here the authors show in a large global study that participants that reported identifying more strongly with their nation reported greater engagement in public health behaviours and support for public health policies in the context of the pandemic.
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
5. Author Correction: National identity predicts public health support during a global pandemic
- Author
-
Jay J. Van Bavel, Aleksandra Cichocka, Valerio Capraro, Hallgeir Sjåstad, John B. Nezlek, Tomislav Pavlović, Mark Alfano, Michele J. Gelfand, Flavio Azevedo, Michèle D. Birtel, Aleksandra Cislak, Patricia L. Lockwood, Robert Malcolm Ross, Koen Abts, Elena Agadullina, John Jamir Benzon Aruta, Sahba Nomvula Besharati, Alexander Bor, Becky L. Choma, Charles David Crabtree, William A. Cunningham, Koustav De, Waqas Ejaz, Christian T. Elbaek, Andrej Findor, Daniel Flichtentrei, Renata Franc, Biljana Gjoneska, June Gruber, Estrella Gualda, Yusaku Horiuchi, Toan Luu Duc Huynh, Agustin Ibanez, Mostak Ahamed Imran, Jacob Israelashvili, Katarzyna Jasko, Jaroslaw Kantorowicz, Elena Kantorowicz-Reznichenko, André Krouwel, Michael Laakasuo, Claus Lamm, Caroline Leygue, Ming-Jen Lin, Mohammad Sabbir Mansoor, Antoine Marie, Lewend Mayiwar, Honorata Mazepus, Cillian McHugh, John Paul Minda, Panagiotis Mitkidis, Andreas Olsson, Tobias Otterbring, Dominic J. Packer, Anat Perry, Michael Bang Petersen, Arathy Puthillam, Julián C. Riaño-Moreno, Tobias Rothmund, Hernando Santamaría-García, Petra C. Schmid, Drozdstoy Stoyanov, Shruti Tewari, Bojan Todosijević, Manos Tsakiris, Hans H. Tung, Radu G. Umbreș, Edmunds Vanags, Madalina Vlasceanu, Andrew Vonasch, Meltem Yucel, Yucheng Zhang, Mohcine Abad, Eli Adler, Narin Akrawi, Hamza Alaoui Mdarhri, Hanane Amara, David M. Amodio, Benedict G. Antazo, Matthew Apps, F. Ceren Ay, Mouhamadou Hady Ba, Sergio Barbosa, Brock Bastian, Anton Berg, Maria P. Bernal-Zárate, Michael Bernstein, Michał Białek, Ennio Bilancini, Natalia Bogatyreva, Leonardo Boncinelli, Jonathan E. Booth, Sylvie Borau, Ondrej Buchel, C. Daryl Cameron, Chrissie F. Carvalho, Tatiana Celadin, Chiara Cerami, Hom Nath Chalise, Xiaojun Cheng, Luca Cian, Kate Cockcroft, Jane Conway, Mateo Andres Córdoba-Delgado, Chiara Crespi, Marie Crouzevialle, Jo Cutler, Marzena Cypryańska, Justyna Dabrowska, Michael A. Daniels, Victoria H. Davis, Pamala N. Dayley, Sylvain Delouvee, Ognjan Denkovski, Guillaume Dezecache, Nathan A. Dhaliwal, Alelie B. Diato, Roberto Di Paolo, Marianna Drosinou, Uwe Dulleck, Jānis Ekmanis, Arhan S. Ertan, Tom W. Etienne, Hapsa Hossain Farhana, Fahima Farkhari, Harry Farmer, Ali Fenwick, Kristijan Fidanovski, Terry Flew, Shona Fraser, Raymond Boadi Frempong, Jonathan A. Fugelsang, Jessica Gale, E. Begoña Garcia-Navarro, Prasad Garladinne, Oussama Ghajjou, Theofilos Gkinopoulos, Kurt Gray, Siobhán M. Griffin, Bjarki Gronfeldt, Mert Gümren, Ranju Lama Gurung, Eran Halperin, Elizabeth Harris, Volo Herzon, Matej Hruška, Guanxiong Huang, Matthias F. C. Hudecek, Ozan Isler, Simon Jangard, Frederik J. Jørgensen, Frank Kachanoff, John Kahn, Apsara Katuwal Dangol, Oleksandra Keudel, Lina Koppel, Mika Koverola, Emily Kubin, Anton Kunnari, Yordan Kutiyski, Oscar Laguna, Josh Leota, Eva Lermer, Jonathan Levy, Neil Levy, Chunyun Li, Elizabeth U. Long, Chiara Longoni, Marina Maglić, Darragh McCashin, Alexander L. Metcalf, Igor Mikloušić, Soulaimane El Mimouni, Asako Miura, Juliana Molina-Paredes, César Monroy-Fonseca, Elena Morales-Marente, David Moreau, Rafał Muda, Annalisa Myer, Kyle Nash, Tarik Nesh-Nash, Jonas P. Nitschke, Matthew S. Nurse, Yohsuke Ohtsubo, Victoria Oldemburgo de Mello, Cathal O’Madagain, Michal Onderco, M. Soledad Palacios-Galvez, Jussi Palomäki, Yafeng Pan, Zsófia Papp, Philip Pärnamets, Mariola Paruzel-Czachura, Zoran Pavlović, César Payán-Gómez, Silva Perander, Michael Mark Pitman, Rajib Prasad, Joanna Pyrkosz-Pacyna, Steve Rathje, Ali Raza, Gabriel G. Rêgo, Kasey Rhee, Claire E. Robertson, Iván Rodríguez-Pascual, Teemu Saikkonen, Octavio Salvador-Ginez, Waldir M. Sampaio, Gaia C. Santi, Natalia Santiago-Tovar, David Savage, Julian A. Scheffer, Philipp Schönegger, David T. Schultner, Enid M. Schutte, Andy Scott, Madhavi Sharma, Pujan Sharma, Ahmed Skali, David Stadelmann, Clara Alexandra Stafford, Dragan Stanojević, Anna Stefaniak, Anni Sternisko, Augustin Stoica, Kristina K. Stoyanova, Brent Strickland, Jukka Sundvall, Jeffrey P. Thomas, Gustav Tinghög, Benno Torgler, Iris J. Traast, Raffaele Tucciarelli, Michael Tyrala, Nick D. Ungson, Mete S. Uysal, Paul A. M. Van Lange, Jan-Willem van Prooijen, Dirk van Rooy, Daniel Västfjäll, Peter Verkoeijen, Joana B. Vieira, Christian von Sikorski, Alexander Cameron Walker, Jennifer Watermeyer, Erik Wetter, Ashley Whillans, Robin Willardt, Michael J. A. Wohl, Adrian Dominik Wójcik, Kaidi Wu, Yuki Yamada, Onurcan Yilmaz, Kumar Yogeeswaran, Carolin-Theresa Ziemer, Rolf A. Zwaan, and Paulo S. Boggio
- Subjects
Science - Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
6. The influence of anger on empathy and theory of mind.
- Author
-
Ronja Weiblen, Noam Mairon, Sören Krach, Macià Buades-Rotger, Mor Nahum, Philipp Kanske, Anat Perry, and Ulrike M Krämer
- Subjects
Medicine ,Science - Abstract
Social cognition allows humans to understand and predict other people's behavior by inferring or sharing their emotions, intentions and beliefs. Few studies have investigated the impact of one's own emotional state on understanding others. Here, we tested the effect of being in an angry state on empathy and theory of mind (ToM). In a between-groups design we manipulated anger status with different paradigms in three studies (autobiographical recall (N = 45), negative feedback (N = 49), frustration (N = 46)) and checked how this manipulation affected empathic accuracy and performance in the EmpaToM. All paradigms were successful in inducing mild anger. We did not find the expected effect of anger on empathy or ToM performance but observed small behavioral changes. Together, our results validate the use of three different anger induction paradigms and speak for rather weak behavioral effects of mild state anger on empathy and ToM.
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
7. Different neural activations for an approaching friend versus stranger: Linking personal space to numerical cognition
- Author
-
Orly Rubinsten, Nachshon Korem, Anat Perry, Miri Goldberg, and Simone Shamay‐Tsoory
- Subjects
dyscalculia ,event‐related potential ,social space ,spatial processing ,Neurosciences. Biological psychiatry. Neuropsychiatry ,RC321-571 - Abstract
Abstract Introduction Typically, humans place themselves at a preferred distance from others. This distance is known to characterize human spatial behavior. Here, we focused on neurocognitive conditions that may affect interpersonal distances. The current study investigated whether neurocognitive deficiencies in numerical and spatial knowledge may affect social perception and modulate personal space. Method In an event‐related potential (ERP) study, university students with developmental dyscalculia (DD) and typically developing control participants were given a computerized version of the comfortable interpersonal distance task, in which participants were instructed to press the spacebar when they began to feel uncomfortable by the approach of a virtual protagonist. Results Results showed that students with deficiencies in numerical and spatial skills (i.e., DD) demonstrated reduced variability in their preferred distance from an approaching friend. Importantly, DD showed decreased amplitude of the N1 wave in the friend condition. Conclusion These results suggest that people coping with deficiencies in spatial cognition have a less efficient allocation of spatial attention in the service of processing personal distances. Accordingly, the study highlights the fundamental role of spatial neurocognition in organizing social space.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
8. Empathic Accuracy in Clinical Populations
- Author
-
Yonat Rum and Anat Perry
- Subjects
empathic accuracy ,autism ,schizophrenia ,psychopathy ,depression ,anxiety ,Psychiatry ,RC435-571 - Abstract
Empathy, broadly defined as the ability to understand the other and to share others’ emotions, motivates prosocial behavior and underlies successful interpersonal relations. Dysfunctions in this ability may cause fundamental difficulties in social communication. Empathy has been measured in various ways, from self-report questionnaires to laboratory objective performance tests. Empathic accuracy (EA), i.e., the ability to accurately empathize, is measured using more complex and ecological paradigms, such as asking participants to infer filmed interactions, or having people narrate personal emotional stories then assessing the correspondence between the perceiver and the target of empathy as the criteria for empathic ability. This measure is particularly useful in the study of clinical populations, where deconstructing the multifaceted concept of empathy may contribute to a more complete understanding of specific clinical profiles. This paper presents a scoping review of the literature on EA in clinical populations, and on EA and clinical traits and states in nonclinical or high-risk populations. Following an exhaustive literature search, 34 studies were found eligible to be included in this review. The largest category was studies focused on EA in people with schizophrenia (31%; 11 papers), followed by studies focused on EA in autism spectrum disorders (ASD) and autistic traits in a nonclinical population (22%; 8 papers). Studies were also found on EA and depression tendencies, psychopathy, social anxiety, behavior disorders, and personality disorders, and a few other clinical conditions. The included studies varied on research aims, designs, sample sizes, and male:female ratios. The overall synthesized results suggest that EA is reduced in schizophrenia and ASD. In other clinical populations, the number of studies was very limited. We urge researchers to further examine EA in these less-studied populations. The review reveals a general underrepresentation of female participants in studies on EA in clinical populations. We suggest that future research address understudied clinical populations, such as those diagnosed with psychopathy. Subject, target, and situational variables should also be considered, with special attention to gender differences (and similarities), the association between EA abilities and adaptive functioning, and the study of individuals with clinical conditions as targets, not just observers, in EA tasks.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
9. To Feel and Talk in a Language of Conflict: Distinct Emotional Experience and Expression of Bilinguals among Disadvantaged Minority Members
- Author
-
Nur Kassem, Yonat Rum, and Anat Perry
- Abstract
Research conducted on emotionality in bilinguals suggests that language use modulates emotional expression. The current study examines bilingual disadvantaged minority members' emotional experience and expression as shaped by the group relations in a conflict area. We hypothesised that, in general, greater emotionality will be found in one's native language. Moreover, since the second language is imposed and acquired in a negative context, there may be differential effects on negative and positive language. A novel ecological paradigm was used: Twenty-eight Palestinian citizens of Israel were videotaped while recounting emotional stories in both Arabic (L1) and Hebrew (L2), resulting in 212 videos. Two studies followed: In Study 1 we compared participants' emotional ratings (1a) and analyzed the content of emotional expression (1b). In Study 2, American participants rated emotional expressiveness. In Study 1, an interaction effect was found between language and valence, with less positive emotions and expressions in L2. In Study 2, a general difference in expressiveness was found in favour of L1. These studies show an effect of power disparities on the emotional load of the second language, thus highlighting the emotional costs of using a second language acquired in a conflict area.
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
10. Does it matter if empathic AI has no empathy?
- Author
-
Garriy Shteynberg, Jodi Halpern, Amir Sadovnik, Jon Garthoff, Anat Perry, Jessica Hay, Carlos Montemayor, Michael A. Olson, Tim L. Hulsey, and Abrol Fairweather
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
11. Mu rhythm suppression over sensorimotor regions is associated with greater empathic accuracy
- Author
-
Shir Genzer, Desmond C. Ong, jamil zaki, and Anat Perry
- Subjects
business.industry ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,Emotions ,Brain ,Electroencephalography ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,General Medicine ,Empathic accuracy ,bepress|Life Sciences|Neuroscience and Neurobiology ,PsyArXiv|Neuroscience|Cognitive Neuroscience ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences ,Text mining ,Rhythm ,PsyArXiv|Neuroscience ,bepress|Life Sciences|Neuroscience and Neurobiology|Behavioral Neurobiology ,bepress|Social and Behavioral Sciences ,Humans ,Empathy ,bepress|Life Sciences|Neuroscience and Neurobiology|Cognitive Neuroscience ,business ,Psychology ,PsyArXiv|Neuroscience|Behavioral Neuroscience ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Emotion ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
When people encounter others emotions, they engage multiple brain systems, including parts of sensorimotor cortex associated with motor simulation. Simulation-related brain activity is commonly described as a "low-level" component of empathy and social cognition. As such, it has been studied predominantly using simple, non-naturalistic tasks, such as viewing facial expressions or biological motion. This leaves unclear whether and how sensorimotor simulation contributes to more complex empathic judgments. Here we explore this phenomenon by combining a naturalistic social paradigm with a reliable index of sensorimotor cortex-based simulation: EEG suppression of oscillatory activity in the mu (8-13 Hz) frequency band. We recruited participants to watch naturalistic video clips of people ("targets") describing emotional events in their lives. Participants viewed these clips (i) with both video and sound, (ii) with only video, or (iii) with only sound. Participants provided continuous ratings of how they believed the target in the video felt. We calculated participants empathic accuracy as the correlation between their inferences and targets self-report. Across all conditions, right-lateralized mu suppression tracked empathic accuracy. This was true when examining accuracy for videos as a whole, as well as when examining accuracy over 3-second intervals within each video. Our results provide novel evidence that motor representations—as measured through mu suppression—play an important role not only in low-level motor simulation, but also in higher-level inferences about others emotions.
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
12. Intonation Units in spontaneous speech evoke a neural response
- Author
-
Maya Inbar, Shir Genzer, Anat Perry, Eitan Grossman, and Ayelet N. Landau
- Abstract
Spontaneous speech is produced in chunks called Intonation Units (IUs). IUs are defined by a set of prosodic cues and occur in all human languages. Linguistic theory suggests that IUs pace the flow of information and serve as a window onto the dynamic focus of attention in speech processing. IUs provide a promising and hitherto unexplored theoretical framework for studying the neural mechanisms of communication, thanks to their universality and their consistent temporal structure across different grammatical and socio-cultural conditions. In this article, we identify a neural response unique to the boundary defined by the IU. We measured the EEG of participants who listened to different speakers recounting an emotional life event. We analyzed the speech stimuli linguistically, and modeled the EEG response at word offset using a GLM approach. We find that the EEG response to IU-final words differs from the response to IU-nonfinal words when acoustic boundary strength is held constant. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first time this is demonstrated in spontaneous speech under naturalistic listening conditions, and under a theoretical framework that connects the prosodic chunking of speech, on the one hand, with the flow of information during communication, on the other. Finally, we relate our findings to the body of research on rhythmic brain mechanism in speech processing by comparing the topographical distributions of neural speech tracking in model-predicted and empirical EEG. This qualitative comparison suggests that IU-related neural activity contributes to the previously characterized delta-band neural speech tracking.
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
13. Effects of social presence on behavioural, neural and physiological aspects of empathy for pain
- Author
-
Pauline Petereit, Ronja Weiblen, Anat Perry, and Ulrike M. Krämer
- Abstract
In mediated interactions (e.g. video calls), less information is available about the other. To investigate how this affects our empathy for one another, we conducted an EEG study, in which thirty human participants observed one of 5 targets undergoing painful electric stimulation, once in a direct interaction and once in a live, video-mediated interaction. We found that observers were as accurate in judging others’ pain and showed as much affective empathy via video as in a direct encounter. While mu suppression, a common neural marker of empathy, was not sensitive to others’ pain, theta responses to others’ pain as well as skin conductance coupling between participants were reduced in the video-mediated condition. We conclude that physical proximity with its rich social cues is important for nuanced physiological resonance with the other’s experience. More studies are warranted to confirm these results and to understand their behavioural significance for remote social interactions.
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
14. Nuancing Perspective
- Author
-
Anat Perry and Jacob Israelashvili
- Subjects
Sociology and Political Science ,Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,Social Psychology ,Perspective (graphical) ,Psychology ,Social psychology ,General Psychology ,Epistemology - Abstract
Two experiments manipulated participants’ familiarity with another person and examined their performance in future understanding of that person’s emotions. To gain familiarity, participants watched several videos of the target sharing experiences and rated her emotions. In the Feedback condition, perceivers learned about the actual emotions the target felt. In the Control condition, perceivers completed identical recognition tasks but did not know the target’s own emotion ratings. Studies ( Ntotal= 398; one preregistered) found that the Feedback group was more accurate than the Control in future understanding of the target’s emotions. Results provide a proof-of-concept demonstration that brief preliminary learning about past emotional experiences of another person can give one a more accurate understanding of the person in the future.
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
15. Gender bias in academia: A lifetime problem that needs solutions
- Author
-
Adrienne L. Fairhall, Rosa Dominguez-Faus, Nancy Kopell, Ilina Bhaya-Grossman, Brooke Staveland, Katarina Slama, Mark A. Gorenstein, Robert T. Knight, Richard Jimenez, Joni D. Wallis, Athina Tzovara, Pedro Pinheiro-Chagas, Jack J. Lin, Ignacio Saez, Dylan Riley, Maria Ivanova, Yvonne M. Fonken, Ivan Skelin, Chris Holdgraf, Déborah Marciano, Julia W. Y. Kam, Celeste Kidd, Danielle S. Bassett, Nicholas E. Myers, Adeen Flinker, Zachariah R. Cross, Soyeon Jun, Anaïs Llorens, Enitan T Marcelle, Anna C. Nobre, Anat Perry, Sabine Kastner, Stephanie Martin, Shlomit Yuval-Greenberg, Karita E. Ojala, Nina F. Dronkers, Laura J. Kray, Colin W. Hoy, Stephanie K. Riès, William K. Chang, Aurélie Bidet-Caulet, Ludovic Bellier, Xiao Jing Wang, Anne Kristin Solbakk, Elizabeth A. Buffalo, University of California [Berkeley] (UC Berkeley), University of California (UC), University of Bern, Centre de recherche en neurosciences de Lyon - Lyon Neuroscience Research Center (CRNL), Université Claude Bernard Lyon 1 (UCBL), Université de Lyon-Université de Lyon-Université Jean Monnet - Saint-Étienne (UJM)-Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM)-Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), University of South Australia [Adelaide], University of California [Davis] (UC Davis), New York University School of Medicine (NYU), New York University School of Medicine, NYU System (NYU)-NYU System (NYU), University of Oxford, Seoul National University [Seoul] (SNU), Universitaetsklinikum Hamburg-Eppendorf = University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf [Hamburg] (UKE), The Hebrew University of Jerusalem (HUJ), Stanford University, San Diego State University (SDSU), University of California [Irvine] (UC Irvine), University of Pennsylvania, University of Washington [Seattle], Boston University [Boston] (BU), University of Oslo (UiO), New York University [New York] (NYU), NYU System (NYU), Tel Aviv University (TAU), Princeton University, Otten, Lisa, Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM), Institut de Neurosciences des Systèmes (INS), Aix Marseille Université (AMU)-Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM), Llorens, Anaïs, Tzovara, Athina, Bellier, Ludovic, Bhaya-Grossman, Ilina, Cross, Zachariah R, and Dronkers, Nina F
- Subjects
Gender Equity ,Male ,academic institutions ,Gender equity ,Universities ,Inequality ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Sexism ,MEDLINE ,gender equity ,Article ,Gender bias ,Psychology ,Humans ,000 Informatik, Wissen, Systeme ,ComputingMilieux_MISCELLANEOUS ,media_common ,Neurology & Neurosurgery ,Research ,[SCCO.NEUR]Cognitive science/Neuroscience ,General Neuroscience ,[SCCO.NEUR] Cognitive science/Neuroscience ,Neurosciences ,510 Mathematik ,[SHS.GENRE] Humanities and Social Sciences/Gender studies ,Mental health ,Research Personnel ,Brain Disorders ,Mental Health ,Good Health and Well Being ,Balance (accounting) ,Cognitive Sciences ,Female ,Demographic economics ,Generic health relevance ,610 Medizin und Gesundheit ,[SHS.GENRE]Humanities and Social Sciences/Gender studies ,Diversity (politics) - Abstract
International audience; Despite increased awareness of the lack of gender equity in academia and a growing number of initiatives to address issues of diversity, change is slow, and inequalities remain. A major source of inequity is gender bias, which has a substantial negative impact on the careers, work-life balance, and mental health of underrepresented groups in science. Here, we argue that gender bias is not a single problem but manifests as a collection of distinct issues that impact researchers’ lives. We disentangle these facets and propose concrete solutions that can be adopted by individuals, academic institutions, and society
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
16. Mirror-neuron system recruitment by action observation: Effects of focal brain damage on mu suppression.
- Author
-
Silvi Frenkel-Toledo, Shlomo Bentin, Anat Perry, Dario G. Liebermann, and Nachum Soroker
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
17. Don't stand so close to me: A behavioral and ERP study of preferred interpersonal distance.
- Author
-
Anat Perry, Orly Rubinsten, Leehe Peled, and Simone G. Shamay-Tsoory
- Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
18. Physicians prescribe fewer analgesics during night shifts than day shifts
- Author
-
Shoham Choshen-Hillel, Ido Sadras, Tom Gordon-Hecker, Shir Genzer, David Rekhtman, Eugene M. Caruso, Koby L. Clements, Adrienne Ohler, David Gozal, Salomon Israel, Anat Perry, and Alex Gileles-Hillel
- Subjects
Analgesics ,Physician-Patient Relations ,Multidisciplinary ,Pain Research ,Neurosciences ,Pain ,Datasets as Topic ,Shift Work Schedule ,Drug Prescriptions ,sleep deprivation ,United States ,shift work ,pain management ,Clinical Research ,Physicians ,Sleep Deprivation ,Humans ,Generic health relevance ,Empathy ,Israel ,Chronic Pain - Abstract
Adequate pain management is one of the biggest challenges of the modern healthcare system. Physician perception of patient subjective pain, which is crucial to pain management, is susceptible to a host of potential biases. Here we explore the timing of physicians’ work as a previously unrecognized source of systematic bias in pain management. We hypothesized that during night shifts, sleep deprivation, fatigue, and stress would reduce physicians’ empathy for others’ pain, leading to underprescription of analgesics for patient pain relief. In study 1, 67 resident physicians, either following a night shift or not, performed empathy for pain assessment tasks and simulated patient scenarios in laboratory conditions. As predicted, following a night shift, physicians showed reduced empathy for pain. In study 2, we explored this phenomenon in medical decisions in the field. We analyzed three emergency department datasets from Israel and the United States that included discharge notes of patients arriving with pain complaints during 2013 to 2020 ( n = 13,482). Across all datasets, physicians were less likely to prescribe an analgesic during night shifts (compared to daytime shifts) and prescribed fewer analgesics than generally recommended by the World Health Organization. This effect remained significant after adjusting for patient, physician, type of complaint, and emergency department characteristics. Underprescription for pain during night shifts was particularly prominent for opioids. We conclude that night shift work is an important and previously unrecognized source of bias in pain management, likely stemming from impaired perception of pain. We consider the implications for hospitals and other organizations employing night shifts.
- Published
- 2022
19. Predicting attitudinal and behavioral responses to COVID-19 pandemic using machine learning
- Author
-
Tomislav Pavlović, Flavio Azevedo, Koustav De, Julián C Riaño-Moreno, Marina Maglić, Theofilos Gkinopoulos, Patricio Andreas Donnelly-Kehoe, César Payán-Gómez, Guanxiong Huang, Jaroslaw Kantorowicz, Michèle D Birtel, Philipp Schönegger, Valerio Capraro, Hernando Santamaría-García, Meltem Yucel, Agustin Ibanez, Steve Rathje, Erik Wetter, Dragan Stanojević, Jan-Willem van Prooijen, Eugenia Hesse, Christian T Elbaek, Renata Franc, Zoran Pavlović, Panagiotis Mitkidis, Aleksandra Cichocka, Michele Gelfand, Mark Alfano, Robert M Ross, Hallgeir Sjåstad, John B Nezlek, Aleksandra Cislak, Patricia Lockwood, Koen Abts, Elena Agadullina, David M Amodio, Matthew A J Apps, John Jamir Benzon Aruta, Sahba Besharati, Alexander Bor, Becky Choma, William Cunningham, Waqas Ejaz, Harry Farmer, Andrej Findor, Biljana Gjoneska, Estrella Gualda, Toan L D Huynh, Mostak Ahamed Imran, Jacob Israelashvili, Elena Kantorowicz-Reznichenko, André Krouwel, Yordan Kutiyski, Michael Laakasuo, Claus Lamm, Jonathan Levy, Caroline Leygue, Ming-Jen Lin, Mohammad Sabbir Mansoor, Antoine Marie, Lewend Mayiwar, Honorata Mazepus, Cillian McHugh, Andreas Olsson, Tobias Otterbring, Dominic Packer, Jussi Palomäki, Anat Perry, Michael Bang Petersen, Arathy Puthillam, Tobias Rothmund, Petra C Schmid, David Stadelmann, Augustin Stoica, Drozdstoy Stoyanov, Kristina Stoyanova, Shruti Tewari, Bojan Todosijević, Benno Torgler, Manos Tsakiris, Hans H Tung, Radu Gabriel Umbreș, Edmunds Vanags, Madalina Vlasceanu, Andrew J Vonasch, Yucheng Zhang, Mohcine Abad, Eli Adler, Hamza Alaoui Mdarhri, Benedict Antazo, F Ceren Ay, Mouhamadou El Hady Ba, Sergio Barbosa, Brock Bastian, Anton Berg, Michał Białek, Ennio Bilancini, Natalia Bogatyreva, Leonardo Boncinelli, Jonathan E Booth, Sylvie Borau, Ondrej Buchel, Chrissie Ferreira de Carvalho, Tatiana Celadin, Chiara Cerami, Hom Nath Chalise, Xiaojun Cheng, Luca Cian, Kate Cockcroft, Jane Conway, Mateo A Córdoba-Delgado, Chiara Crespi, Marie Crouzevialle, Jo Cutler, Marzena Cypryańska, Justyna Dabrowska, Victoria H Davis, John Paul Minda, Pamala N Dayley, Sylvain Delouvée, Ognjan Denkovski, Guillaume Dezecache, Nathan A Dhaliwal, Alelie Diato, Roberto Di Paolo, Uwe Dulleck, Jānis Ekmanis, Tom W Etienne, Hapsa Hossain Farhana, Fahima Farkhari, Kristijan Fidanovski, Terry Flew, Shona Fraser, Raymond Boadi Frempong, Jonathan Fugelsang, Jessica Gale, E Begoña García-Navarro, Prasad Garladinne, Kurt Gray, Siobhán M Griffin, Bjarki Gronfeldt, June Gruber, Eran Halperin, Volo Herzon, Matej Hruška, Matthias F C Hudecek, Ozan Isler, Simon Jangard, Frederik Jørgensen, Oleksandra Keudel, Lina Koppel, Mika Koverola, Anton Kunnari, Josh Leota, Eva Lermer, Chunyun Li, Chiara Longoni, Darragh McCashin, Igor Mikloušić, Juliana Molina-Paredes, César Monroy-Fonseca, Elena Morales-Marente, David Moreau, Rafał Muda, Annalisa Myer, Kyle Nash, Jonas P Nitschke, Matthew S Nurse, Victoria Oldemburgo de Mello, M Soledad Palacios-Galvez, Yafeng Pan, Zsófia Papp, Philip Pärnamets, Mariola Paruzel-Czachura, Silva Perander, Michael Pitman, Ali Raza, Gabriel Gaudencio Rêgo, Claire Robertson, Iván Rodríguez-Pascual, Teemu Saikkonen, Octavio Salvador-Ginez, Waldir M Sampaio, Gaia Chiara Santi, David Schultner, Enid Schutte, Andy Scott, Ahmed Skali, Anna Stefaniak, Anni Sternisko, Brent Strickland, Jeffrey P Thomas, Gustav Tinghög, Iris J Traast, Raffaele Tucciarelli, Michael Tyrala, Nick D Ungson, Mete Sefa Uysal, Dirk Van Rooy, Daniel Västfjäll, Joana B Vieira, Christian von Sikorski, Alexander C Walker, Jennifer Watermeyer, Robin Willardt, Michael J A Wohl, Adrian Dominik Wójcik, Kaidi Wu, Yuki Yamada, Onurcan Yilmaz, Kumar Yogeeswaran, Carolin-Theresa Ziemer, Rolf A Zwaan, Paulo Sergio Boggio, Ashley Whillans, Paul A M Van Lange, Rajib Prasad, Michal Onderco, Cathal O'Madagain, Tarik Nesh-Nash, Oscar Moreda Laguna, Emily Kubin, Mert Gümren, Ali Fenwick, Arhan S Ertan, Michael J Bernstein, Hanane Amara, Jay Joseph Van Bavel, Brain and Cognition, Public Administration, Pavlovic, Tomislav, Avezedo, Flávio, De, Koustav, Maglić, Marina, Donnelly-Kehoe, Patricio Andreas, Payán-Gómez, César, Van Bavel, Jay J., Schmid, Petra Claudia, Crouzevialle, Marie, Willardt, Robin, et al., Ivo Pilar Institute of Social Sciences, University of Cambridge [UK] (CAM), University of Kentucky (UK), Universidad El Bosque [Bogota], University of Crete [Heraklion] (UOC), Universidad del Rosario [Bogota], Universidad Nacional de Colombia [Bogotà] (UNAL), City University of Hong Kong [Hong Kong] (CUHK), Universiteit Leiden, University of Greenwich, University of St Andrews [Scotland], Middlesex University [London], Pontifical Xavierian University, Duke University [Durham], Universidad Adolfo Ibáñez [Santiago], University of California (UC), Trinity College Dublin, Stockholm School of Economics (SSE), University of Belgrade [Belgrade], Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam [Amsterdam] (VU), Universidad Mayor de San Andrés (UMSA), Aarhus University [Aarhus], University of Kent [Canterbury], Stanford University, Macquarie University, Norwegian School of Economics and Business Administration, SWPS University of Social Sciences and Humanities (SWPS), University of Birmingham [Birmingham], University of Oxford, Catholic University of Leuven - Katholieke Universiteit Leuven (KU Leuven), Vysšaja škola èkonomiki = National Research University Higher School of Economics [Moscow] (HSE), University of Amsterdam [Amsterdam] (UvA), Sunway University [Malaysia], University of the Witwatersrand [Johannesburg] (WITS), University of Toronto, National University of Sciences and Technology [Islamabad] (NUST), Comenius University in Bratislava, Macedonian Academy of Sciences and Arts [Skopje, North Macedonia] (MASA), Universidad de Huelva, University of Southampton, BRAC University, The Hebrew University of Jerusalem (HUJ), Erasmus University Rotterdam, Kieskompas, Helsingin yliopisto = Helsingfors universitet = University of Helsinki, University of Vienna [Vienna], Reichman University [Herzliya], Aalto University, Universidad Nacional Autónoma de México = National Autonomous University of Mexico (UNAM), National Taiwan University [Taiwan] (NTU), HEMS School, BI Norwegian Business School [Oslo], University of Limerick (UL), Karolinska Institutet [Stockholm], University of Agder (UIA), Lehigh University [Bethlehem], Monk Prayogshala, Friedrich-Schiller-Universität = Friedrich Schiller University Jena [Jena, Germany], Eidgenössische Technische Hochschule Zürich - Swiss Federal Institute of Technology in Zürich (ETH Zurïch), University of Bayreuth, National School of Political and Administrative Studies (SNSPA), Plovdiv University 'Paisii Hilendarski', Indian Institute of Management Indore (IMM Indore), Institute of Social Sciences Belgrade, Queensland University of Technology [Brisbane] (QUT), Royal Holloway [University of London] (RHUL), University of London [London], National University of Political Studies and Public Administration, University of Latvia (LU), New York University [New York] (NYU), NYU System (NYU), University of Canterbury [Christchurch], Hebei University of Technology [Tianjin], Université Mohammed VI Polytechnique [Ben Guerir] (UM6P), AnsuR Technologies [Fornebu], Université Cheikh Anta Diop [Dakar, Sénégal] (UCAD), Victoria University [Melbourne], University of Wrocław [Poland] (UWr), IMT Institute for Advanced Studies [Lucca], Université de Florence, Università degli Studi di Firenze = University of Florence (UniFI), London School of Economics and Political Science (LSE), TBS Education, Slovak Academy of Science [Bratislava] (SAS), Santa Catarina State University (UDESC), University of Bologna/Università di Bologna, Tribhuvan University, Shenzhen Univerisity [Shenzhen], University of Virginia, Université Toulouse Capitole (UT Capitole), Université de Toulouse (UT), University of Western Ontario (UWO), Université de Rennes 2 (UR2), Laboratoire de Psychologie Sociale et Cognitive (LAPSCO), Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS)-Université Clermont Auvergne (UCA), University of British Columbia (UBC), University of Queensland [Brisbane], Pennsylvania State University (Penn State), Penn State System, Westfälische Wilhelms-Universität Münster = University of Münster (WWU), The University of Sydney, University of Waterloo [Waterloo], University of North Carolina [Chapel Hill] (UNC), University of North Carolina System (UNC), University of Colorado [Boulder], University of Regensburg, Kyiv School of economic (KSE), Linköping University (LIU), Monash University [Clayton], Ansbach University of Applied Sciences, Ludwig Maximilian University [Munich] (LMU), Department of Management - London School of Economics and Political Science (LSE), Questrom School of Business, Boston University [Boston] (BU), Dublin City University [Dublin] (DCU), University of Auckland [Auckland], Maria Curie-Sklodowska University (UMCS), City University of New York [New York] (CUNY), University of Alberta, Australian National University (ANU), University of Silesia in Katowice, Universidad Complutense de Madrid = Complutense University of Madrid [Madrid] (UCM), Zhejiang University, Mackenzie Presbyterian University [São Paulo] (UPM), NYU Department of Psychology [New-York University], NYU System (NYU)-NYU System (NYU), University of Turku, City College of New York [CUNY] (CCNY), University of Groningen [Groningen], Carleton University, Département d'Etudes Cognitives - ENS Paris (DEC), École normale supérieure - Paris (ENS-PSL), Université Paris sciences et lettres (PSL)-Université Paris sciences et lettres (PSL), Faculté de Gouvernance, Sciences Économiques et Sociales [UM6P] (FGSES), University College of London [London] (UCL), Susquehanna University, University of Antwerp (UA), University of Exeter, University of Koblenz-Landau, Nicolaus Copernicus University [Toruń], University of California [San Diego] (UC San Diego), Kyushu University, Kadir Has University (KHAS), Harvard Business School, Harvard University, Vidyasagar College For Women, Koç University, Hult International Business School, Boǧaziçi üniversitesi = Boğaziçi University [Istanbul], Penn State Abington, Pennsylvania Commonwealth System of Higher Education (PCSHE)-Penn State System, Communication Science, Network Institute, Communication Choices, Content and Consequences (CCCC), Social Psychology, IBBA, A-LAB, University of St Andrews. Philosophy, University of St Andrews. School of Psychology and Neuroscience, Pavlović, Tomislav [0000-0002-4470-3715], Azevedo, Flavio [0000-0001-9000-8513], De, Koustav [0000-0001-9562-0672], Riaño-Moreno, Julián C [0000-0003-4182-0550], Maglić, Marina [0000-0002-6851-4601], Gkinopoulos, Theofilos [0000-0003-1070-6245], Donnelly-Kehoe, Patricio Andreas [0000-0002-3738-9537], Payán-Gómez, César [0000-0002-0633-1332], Huang, Guanxiong [0000-0002-8588-1454], Kantorowicz, Jaroslaw [0000-0002-1186-5427], Birtel, Michèle D [0000-0002-2383-9197], Schönegger, Philipp [0000-0001-9930-487X], Capraro, Valerio [0000-0002-0579-0166], Santamaría-García, Hernando [0000-0001-9422-3579], Yucel, Meltem [0000-0002-7274-5971], Ibanez, Agustin [0000-0001-6758-5101], Rathje, Steve [0000-0001-6727-571X], Wetter, Erik [0000-0002-5821-6651], Stanojević, Dragan [0000-0002-3667-2461], van Prooijen, Jan-Willem [0000-0001-6236-0819], Hesse, Eugenia [0000-0002-9077-9833], Elbaek, Christian T [0000-0002-7039-4565], Franc, Renata [0000-0002-1909-2393], Pavlović, Zoran [0000-0002-9231-5100], Mitkidis, Panagiotis [0000-0002-9495-7369], Cichocka, Aleksandra [0000-0003-1703-1586], Alfano, Mark [0000-0001-5879-8033], Ross, Robert M [0000-0001-8711-1675], Sjåstad, Hallgeir [0000-0002-8730-1038], Nezlek, John B [0000-0003-4963-3637], Cislak, Aleksandra [0000-0002-9880-6947], Lockwood, Patricia [0000-0001-7195-9559], Abts, Koen [0000-0001-8546-8347], Agadullina, Elena [0000-0002-1505-1412], Amodio, David M [0000-0001-7746-0150], Apps, Matthew AJ [0000-0001-5793-2202], Aruta, John Jamir Benzon [0000-0003-4155-1063], Besharati, Sahba [0000-0003-2836-7982], Bor, Alexander [0000-0002-2624-9221], Choma, Becky [0000-0002-8286-8983], Ejaz, Waqas [0000-0002-2492-4115], Farmer, Harry [0000-0002-3684-0605], Findor, Andrej [0000-0002-5896-6989], Gjoneska, Biljana [0000-0003-1200-6672], Gualda, Estrella [0000-0003-0220-2135], Huynh, Toan LD [0000-0002-6653-7447], Imran, Mostak Ahamed [0000-0002-5101-3149], Israelashvili, Jacob [0000-0003-1289-223X], Kantorowicz-Reznichenko, Elena [0000-0001-8128-7290], Krouwel, André [0000-0003-0952-6028], Kutiyski, Yordan [0000-0003-0889-4232], Laakasuo, Michael [0000-0003-2826-6073], Lamm, Claus [0000-0002-5422-0653], Levy, Jonathan [0000-0003-1007-2393], Leygue, Caroline [0000-0002-0355-1030], Lin, Ming-Jen [0000-0002-7174-2226], Mansoor, Mohammad Sabbir [0000-0002-6541-3506], Marie, Antoine [0000-0002-7958-0153], Mayiwar, Lewend [0000-0003-1404-4779], Mazepus, Honorata [0000-0002-5005-9399], McHugh, Cillian [0000-0002-9701-3232], Olsson, Andreas [0000-0001-5272-7744], Otterbring, Tobias [0000-0002-0283-8777], Packer, Dominic [0000-0001-8420-6548], Palomäki, Jussi [0000-0001-6063-0926], Perry, Anat [0000-0003-2329-856X], Petersen, Michael Bang [0000-0002-6782-5635], Puthillam, Arathy [0000-0003-2426-8362], Rothmund, Tobias [0000-0003-2979-5129], Schmid, Petra C [0000-0002-9990-5445], Stadelmann, David [0000-0002-1211-9936], Stoica, Augustin [0000-0003-0585-1114], Stoyanov, Drozdstoy [0000-0002-9975-3680], Stoyanova, Kristina [0000-0001-8362-6444], Tewari, Shruti [0000-0003-1903-7252], Todosijević, Bojan [0000-0002-6116-993X], Torgler, Benno [0000-0002-9809-963X], Tsakiris, Manos [0000-0001-7753-7576], Tung, Hans H [0000-0001-5332-7582], Umbreș, Radu Gabriel [0000-0002-6121-4518], Vanags, Edmunds [0000-0003-1932-936X], Vlasceanu, Madalina [0000-0003-2138-1968], Vonasch, Andrew J [0000-0002-2784-5420], Zhang, Yucheng [0000-0001-9435-6734], Abad, Mohcine [0000-0002-4964-5411], Adler, Eli [0000-0002-9005-5536], Mdarhri, Hamza Alaoui [0000-0001-9831-6561], Antazo, Benedict [0000-0001-9993-8960], Ay, F Ceren [0000-0003-4444-2268], Ba, Mouhamadou El Hady [0000-0002-2707-1242], Barbosa, Sergio [0000-0003-1989-158X], Bastian, Brock [0000-0003-4619-3322], Berg, Anton [0000-0001-7143-762X], Białek, Michał [0000-0002-5062-5733], Bilancini, Ennio [0000-0002-2027-3992], Bogatyreva, Natalia [0000-0002-6024-2322], Boncinelli, Leonardo [0000-0003-0626-5133], Booth, Jonathan E [0000-0002-8563-4613], Borau, Sylvie [0000-0003-1564-0695], Buchel, Ondrej [0000-0002-0139-5513], de Carvalho, Chrissie Ferreira [0000-0002-1369-6188], Celadin, Tatiana [0000-0002-7743-3117], Cerami, Chiara [0000-0003-1974-3421], Chalise, Hom Nath [0000-0002-9301-6890], Cian, Luca [0000-0002-8051-1366], Cockcroft, Kate [0000-0002-6166-8050], Conway, Jane [0000-0003-3883-349X], Córdoba-Delgado, Mateo A [0000-0002-2264-7388], Crespi, Chiara [0000-0002-7851-9887], Crouzevialle, Marie [0000-0002-5538-6030], Cutler, Jo [0000-0003-1073-764X], Cypryańska, Marzena [0000-0001-9234-562X], Dabrowska, Justyna [0000-0002-8821-7161], Davis, Victoria H [0000-0002-7207-4629], Minda, John Paul [0000-0002-4081-010X], Dayley, Pamala N [0000-0001-8955-9502], Delouvée, Sylvain [0000-0002-4029-597X], Denkovski, Ognjan [0000-0002-9739-4100], Dezecache, Guillaume [0000-0002-9366-6287], Dhaliwal, Nathan A [0000-0002-4667-0689], Di Paolo, Roberto [0000-0002-6081-6656], Dulleck, Uwe [0000-0002-0953-5963], Ekmanis, Jānis [0000-0003-1781-1785], Etienne, Tom W [0000-0002-4299-6593], Farkhari, Fahima [0000-0002-8484-5128], Fidanovski, Kristijan [0000-0002-9006-0140], Flew, Terry [0000-0003-4485-9338], Fraser, Shona [0000-0003-1505-0652], Frempong, Raymond Boadi [0000-0002-4603-5570], Fugelsang, Jonathan [0000-0002-6342-7023], Gale, Jessica [0000-0001-5677-8629], García-Navarro, E Begoña [0000-0001-6913-8882], Gray, Kurt [0000-0001-5816-2676], Griffin, Siobhán M [0000-0002-3613-2844], Gronfeldt, Bjarki [0000-0001-9941-7903], Gruber, June [0000-0002-7789-1353], Halperin, Eran [0000-0002-3379-2935], Herzon, Volo [0000-0001-7781-1651], Hruška, Matej [0000-0002-4521-3697], Hudecek, Matthias FC [0000-0002-7696-766X], Isler, Ozan [0000-0002-4638-2230], Jangard, Simon [0000-0002-7876-4161], Jørgensen, Frederik [0000-0002-5461-912X], Keudel, Oleksandra [0000-0001-6322-3103], Koppel, Lina [0000-0002-6302-0047], Koverola, Mika [0000-0001-8227-6120], Kunnari, Anton [0000-0002-2951-6399], Leota, Josh [0000-0002-7714-4630], Lermer, Eva [0000-0002-6600-9580], Li, Chunyun [0000-0001-5909-0889], Longoni, Chiara [0000-0002-4945-4957], McCashin, Darragh [0000-0003-2686-2111], Mikloušić, Igor [0000-0002-6539-2901], Monroy-Fonseca, César [0000-0003-4696-8159], Morales-Marente, Elena [0000-0002-1227-9606], Moreau, David [0000-0002-1957-1941], Muda, Rafał [0000-0003-4953-6642], Myer, Annalisa [0000-0002-2363-4757], Nash, Kyle [0000-0003-0461-3835], Nitschke, Jonas P [0000-0002-3244-8585], Nurse, Matthew S [0000-0003-1787-5914], de Mello, Victoria Oldemburgo [0000-0003-2867-8529], Palacios-Galvez, Maria Soledad [0000-0002-6802-6202], Pan, Yafeng [0000-0002-5633-8313], Papp, Zsófia [0000-0001-6257-0568], Pärnamets, Philip [0000-0001-8360-9097], Paruzel-Czachura, Mariola [0000-0002-8716-9778], Perander, Silva [0000-0001-6711-8079], Pitman, Michael [0000-0001-5532-5388], Raza, Ali [0000-0002-2438-6054], Rêgo, Gabriel Gaudencio [0000-0003-3304-4723], Robertson, Claire [0000-0001-8403-6358], Rodríguez-Pascual, Iván [0000-0002-5385-3643], Saikkonen, Teemu [0000-0001-9619-3270], Salvador-Ginez, Octavio [0000-0002-1652-8788], Sampaio, Waldir M [0000-0002-6066-4314], Santi, Gaia Chiara [0000-0002-9253-6122], Schultner, David [0000-0003-2253-4065], Schutte, Enid [0000-0002-6653-5409], Scott, Andy [0000-0002-3294-0078], Skali, Ahmed [0000-0002-4753-3280], Stefaniak, Anna [0000-0002-1706-7784], Sternisko, Anni [0000-0002-2507-3076], Strickland, Brent [0000-0002-7573-3722], Thomas, Jeffrey P [0000-0002-5939-5559], Tinghög, Gustav [0000-0002-8159-1249], Tucciarelli, Raffaele [0000-0002-0342-308X], Tyrala, Michael [0000-0001-5268-8319], Ungson, Nick D [0000-0003-2494-4498], Uysal, Mete Sefa [0000-0002-8698-9213], Van Rooy, Dirk [0000-0003-2525-5408], Västfjäll, Daniel [0000-0003-2873-4500], Vieira, Joana B [0000-0001-7335-4588], von Sikorski, Christian [0000-0002-3787-8277], Walker, Alexander C [0000-0003-1431-6770], Watermeyer, Jennifer [0000-0001-7918-8832], Willardt, Robin [0000-0002-2495-3450], Wohl, Michael JA [0000-0001-6945-5562], Wójcik, Adrian Dominik [0000-0002-7073-6019], Wu, Kaidi [0000-0001-6881-7437], Yamada, Yuki [0000-0003-1431-568X], Yilmaz, Onurcan [0000-0002-6094-7162], Yogeeswaran, Kumar [0000-0002-1978-5077], Ziemer, Carolin-Theresa [0000-0002-0794-7702], Zwaan, Rolf A [0000-0001-9967-7879], Boggio, Paulo Sergio [0000-0002-6109-0447], Van Lange, Paul AM [0000-0001-7774-6984], Prasad, Rajib [0000-0003-0913-1624], Onderco, Michal [0000-0001-9911-3782], O'Madagain, Cathal [0000-0002-4086-524X], Nesh-Nash, Tarik [0000-0002-5532-9095], Kubin, Emily [0000-0003-0606-8594], Gümren, Mert [0000-0002-3298-6295], Fenwick, Ali [0000-0002-5412-9745], Ertan, Arhan S [0000-0001-9730-8391], Bernstein, Michael J [0000-0002-8083-9839], Amara, Hanane [0000-0003-0732-2320], Van Bavel, Jay Joseph [0000-0002-2520-0442], Apollo - University of Cambridge Repository, Sociale Psychologie (Psychologie, FMG), ASCoR Other Research (FMG), Department of Neuroscience and Biomedical Engineering, Aalto-yliopisto, Department of Digital Humanities, Department of Psychology and Logopedics, Cognitive Science, High Performance Cognition group, Mind and Matter, and Department of Computer Science
- Subjects
Hälso- och sjukvårdsorganisation, hälsopolitik och hälsoekonomi ,Economics ,COVID-19 ,social distancing ,hygiene ,policy support ,public health measures ,J Political Science ,[SHS.PSY]Humanities and Social Sciences/Psychology ,psychology ,Settore SECS-P/02 - Politica Economica ,Q1 ,H Social Sciences ,[STAT.ML]Statistics [stat]/Machine Learning [stat.ML] ,SDG 3 - Good Health and Well-being ,RA0421 ,RA0421 Public health. Hygiene. Preventive Medicine ,Machine learning ,Psychology ,Nationalekonomi ,Settore SECS-P/01 - Economia Politica ,MCC ,Computer. Automation ,Psykologi (exklusive tillämpad psykologi) ,DAS ,Health Care Service and Management, Health Policy and Services and Health Economy ,[SHS.SCIPO]Humanities and Social Sciences/Political science ,Psychology (excluding Applied Psychology) ,machine learning ,Settore SECS-P/03 - Scienza delle Finanze ,5171 Political Science ,Human medicine ,COVID-19, social distancing, hygiene, policy support, public health measures - Abstract
At the beginning of 2020, COVID-19 became a global problem. Despite all the efforts to emphasize the relevance of preventive measures, not everyone adhered to them. Thus, learning more about the characteristics determining attitudinal and behavioral responses to the pandemic is crucial to improving future interventions. In this study, we applied machine learning on the multinational data collected by the International Collaboration on the Social and Moral Psychology of COVID-19 (N = 51,404) to test the predictive efficacy of constructs from social, moral, cognitive, and personality psychology, as well as socio-demographic factors, in the attitudinal and behavioral responses to the pandemic. The results point to several valuable insights. Internalized moral identity provided the most consistent predictive contribution—individuals perceiving moral traits as central to their self-concept reported higher adherence to preventive measures. Similar results were found for morality as cooperation, symbolized moral identity, self-control, open-mindedness, and collective narcissism, while the inverse relationship was evident for the endorsement of conspiracy theories. However, we also found a non-neglible variability in the explained variance and predictive contributions with respect to macro-level factors such as the pandemic stage or cultural region. Overall, the results underscore the importance of morality-related and contextual factors in understanding adherence to public health recommendations during the pandemic., PNAS Nexus, 1 (3), ISSN:2752-6542
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
20. Motor and attentional mechanisms involved in social interaction - Evidence from mu and alpha EEG suppression.
- Author
-
Anat Perry, Libi Stein, and Shlomo Bentin
- Published
- 2011
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
21. Are there positive effects of having a sibling with special needs? Empathy and prosociality of twins of children with non-typical development
- Author
-
Yonat Rum, Shir Genzer, Noam Markovitch, Jennifer Jenkins, Anat Perry, and Ariel Knafo‐Noam
- Subjects
Male ,Siblings ,Pediatrics, Perinatology and Child Health ,Emotions ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,Twins ,Humans ,Female ,Empathy ,Child ,Education - Abstract
This study examined whether typically developing (TD) twins of non-TD children demonstrate enhanced empathy and prosociality. Of 778 Hebrew-speaking Israeli families who participated in a twin study, 63 were identified to have a non-TD child with a TD twin, and 404 as having both twins TD. TD twins of non-TD children (27% males) were compared to the rest of the cohort of TD children (46% males) on measures of empathy and prosociality. Participants were 11 years old. TD twins of non-TD children scored significantly higher than TD twins of TD children in a measure of cognitive empathy (d = .43). No differences were found in emotional empathy and prosociality. The specificity of the positive effect on cognitive empathy is discussed.
- Published
- 2022
22. Impaired empathic accuracy following damage to the left hemisphere
- Author
-
Karine Jospe, Shir Genzer, Lihi Mansano, Desmond Ong, Jamil Zaki, Nachum Soroker, and Anat Perry
- Subjects
Facial Expression ,Stroke ,Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology ,General Neuroscience ,Emotions ,Humans ,Cues ,Empathy ,Functional Laterality ,Aged - Abstract
Failing to understand others accurately can be extremely costly. Unfortunately, events such as strokes can lead to a decline in emotional understanding. Such impairments have been documented in stroke patients and are widely hypothesized to be related to right-hemisphere lesions, as well as to the amygdala, and are thought to be driven in part by attentional biases, for example, less fixation on the eyes. Notably, most of the previous research relied on measurements of emotional understanding from simplified cues, such as facial expressions or prosody. We hypothesize that chronic damage to the left hemisphere could hinder empathic accuracy and emotion recognition in naturalistic social settings that require complex language comprehension, even after a patient regains core language capacities. To assess this notion, we use an empathic accuracy task and eye-tracking measurements with chronic stroke patients with either right (N = 13) or left (N = 11) hemispheric damage-together with aged-matched controls (N = 15)-to explore the patients' understanding of others' affect inferred from stimuli that separates audio and visual cues. While we find that patients with right-hemisphere lesions showed visual attention bias compared to the other two groups, we uncover a disadvantage for patients with left-hemisphere lesions in empathic accuracy, especially when only auditory cues are present. These results suggest that patients with left-hemisphere damage have long-lasting difficulties comprehending real-world complex emotional situations.
- Published
- 2022
23. The influence of emotional state on empathic accuracy
- Author
-
Zhiyun Chen, Qi Gao, Karine Jospe, Hanxi Pan, Jinyou Sheng, Anat Perry, and Zaifeng Gao
- Subjects
Ophthalmology ,Sensory Systems - Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
24. Grasp-specific high-frequency broadband mirror neuron activity during reach-and-grasp movements in humans
- Author
-
Alexander M Dreyer, Leo Michalke, Anat Perry, Edward F Chang, Jack J Lin, Robert T Knight, and Jochem W Rieger
- Subjects
Cellular and Molecular Neuroscience ,Cognitive Neuroscience - Abstract
Broadly congruent mirror neurons, responding to any grasp movement, and strictly congruent mirror neurons, responding only to specific grasp movements, have been reported in single-cell studies with primates. Delineating grasp properties in humans is essential to understand the human mirror neuron system with implications for behavior and social cognition. We analyzed electrocorticography data from a natural reach-and-grasp movement observation and delayed imitation task with 3 different natural grasp types of everyday objects. We focused on the classification of grasp types from high-frequency broadband mirror activation patterns found in classic mirror system areas, including sensorimotor, supplementary motor, inferior frontal, and parietal cortices. Classification of grasp types was successful during movement observation and execution intervals but not during movement retention. Our grasp type classification from combined and single mirror electrodes provides evidence for grasp-congruent activity in the human mirror neuron system potentially arising from strictly congruent mirror neurons.
- Published
- 2021
25. National identity predicts public health support during a global pandemic
- Author
-
Jay Joseph Van Bavel, Aleksandra Cichocka, Valerio Capraro, Hallgeir Sjåstad, John B. Nezlek, Mark Alfano, Flavio Azevedo, Aleksandra Cislak, Patricia Lockwood, Robert M Ross, Elena Agadullina, Matthew A J Apps, JOHN JAMIR BENZON R. ARUTA, Alexander Bor, Charles Crabtree, William A. Cunningham, Koustav De, Christian T. Elbaek, Waqas Ejaz, Andrej Findor, Biljana Gjoneska, Yusaku Horiuchi, Toan Luu Duc Huynh, Agustin Ibanez, Jacob Israelashvili, Katarzyna Jasko, Jaroslaw Kantorowicz, Elena Kantorowicz-Reznichenko, André Krouwel, Michael Laakasuo, Claus Lamm, Caroline Leygue, Mohammad Sabbir Mansoor, Lewend Mayiwar, Honorata Mazepus, Cillian McHugh, Panagiotis Mitkidis, Andreas Olsson, Tobias Otterbring, Anat Perry, Dominic Packer, Michael Bang Petersen, Arathy Puthillam, Tobias Rothmund, SHRUTI TEWARI, Manos Tsakiris, Hans Tung, Meltem Yucel, Edmunds Vanags, Madalina Vlasceanu, Benedict Guzman Antazo, Sergio Barbosa, Brock Bastian, Ennio Bilancini, Natalia Bogatyreva, Leonardo Boncinelli, Jonathan E. Booth, Sylvie Borau, Ondrej Buchel, Chrissie Ferreira Carvalho, Tatiana Celadin, Chiara Cerami, Luca Cian, Chiara Crespi, Jo Cutler, Sylvain Delouvée, Guillaume Dezecache, Roberto Di Paolo, Uwe Dulleck, Tom Etienne, Fahima Farkhari, Jonathan Albert Fugelsang, Theofilos Gkinopoulos, Kurt Gray, Siobhán M Griffin, Bjarki Gronfeldt, June Gruber, Elizabeth Ann Harris, Matej Hruška, Ozan Isler, Simon Jangard, Frederik Juhl Jørgensen, Lina Koppel, Josh Leota, Eva Lermer, Neil L Levy, Chiara Longoni, Asako Miura, Rafał Muda, Annalisa Myer, Kyle Nash, Jonas P. Nitschke, Yohsuke Ohtsubo, Victoria Oldemburgo de Mello, Yafeng Pan, Papp Zsófia, Philip Pärnamets, Mariola Paruzel-Czachura, Michael Mark Pitman, Joanna Pyrkosz-Pacyna, Steve Rathje, Ali Raza, Kasey Rhee, Gabriel Gaudencio do Rêgo, Claire Robertson, Octavio Salvador-Ginez, Waldir M. Sampaio, David Alan Savage, Julian Andrew Scheffer, Philipp Schönegger, Andy Scott, Ahmed Skali, Brent Strickland, Clara Alexandra Stafford, Anna Stefaniak, Anni Sternisko, Gustav Tinghög, Benno Torgler, Raffaele Tucciarelli, Nick D'Angelo Ungson, Mete Sefa Uysal, Jan-Willem van Prooijen, Dirk Van Rooy, Daniel Västfjäll, Joana Vieira, Alexander C. Walker, Erik Wetter, Robin Richard Willardt, Adrian Dominik Wojcik, Kaidi Wu, Yuki Yamada, Onurcan Yilmaz, Kumar Yogeeswaran, Rolf Antonius Zwaan, Paulo Boggio, Daryl Cameron, Michael Tyrala, Estrella Gualda, David Moreau, Jussi Palomäki, and Matthias F. C. Hudecek
- Subjects
medicine.medical_specialty ,Economic growth ,Distancing ,Public health ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Psychological intervention ,Public policy ,Hygiene ,Political science ,Pandemic ,National identity ,medicine ,World Values Survey ,media_common - Abstract
Changing collective behaviour and supporting non-pharmaceutical interventions is an important component in mitigating virus transmission during a pandemic. In a large international collaboration (Study 1, N = 49,968 across 67 countries), we investigated self-reported factors that associated with people reported adopting public health behaviours (e.g., spatial distancing and stricter hygiene) and endorsed public policy interventions (e.g., closing bars and restaurants) during the early stage of the pandemic (April-May 2020). Respondents who reported identifying more strongly with their nation consistently reported greater engagement in public health behaviours and support for public health policies. Results were similar for representative and non-representative national samples. Study 2 (N = 42 countries) conceptually replicated the central finding using aggregate indices of national identity (obtained using the World Values Survey) and a measure of actual behaviour change during the pandemic (obtained from Google mobility reports). Higher levels of national identification prior to the pandemic predicted lower mobility during the early stage of the pandemic (r = -.40). We discuss the potential implications of links between national identity, leadership, and public health for managing COVID-19 and future pandemics.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
26. Behavioral and EEG Measures Show no Amplifying Effects of Shared Attention on Attention or Memory
- Author
-
Mor Nahum, Noam Mairon, Arjen Stolk, Rob Knight, and Anat Perry
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Adolescent ,Emotions ,lcsh:Medicine ,Attention task ,Electroencephalography ,Stimulus (physiology) ,050105 experimental psychology ,Article ,03 medical and health sciences ,Young Adult ,0302 clinical medicine ,Memory ,P3b ,Human behaviour ,medicine ,Reaction Time ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Attention ,lcsh:Science ,Evoked Potentials ,Emotion ,Motivation ,Multidisciplinary ,medicine.diagnostic_test ,Extramural ,lcsh:R ,05 social sciences ,Emotional stimuli ,Cognition ,Recognition, Psychology ,lcsh:Q ,Female ,Social neuroscience ,Simultaneous attention ,Psychology ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Photic Stimulation ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Shared attention experiments examine the potential differences in function or behavior when stimuli are experienced alone or in the presence of others, and when simultaneous attention of the participants to the same stimulus or set is involved. Previous work has found enhanced reactions to emotional stimuli in social situations, yet these changes might represent enhanced communicative or motivational purposes. This study examines whether viewing emotional stimuli in the presence of another person influences attention to or memory for the stimulus. Participants passively viewed emotionally-valenced stimuli while completing another task (counting flowers). Each participant performed this task both alone and in a shared attention condition (simultaneously with another person in the same room) while EEG signals were measured. Recognition of the emotional pictures was later measured. A significant shared attention behavioral effect was found in the attention task but not in the recognition task. Compared to event-related potential responses for neutral pictures, we found higher P3b response for task relevant stimuli (flowers), and higher Late Positive Potential (LPP) responses for emotional stimuli. However, no main effect was found for shared attention between presence conditions. To conclude, shared attention may therefore have a more limited effect on cognitive processes than previously suggested.
- Published
- 2020
27. The role of oxytocin in implicit personal space regulation: An fMRI study
- Author
-
Naama Mayseless, Daniela Cohen, Anat Perry, Simone G. Shamay-Tsoory, and Oded M. Kleinmintz
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Dorsum ,Endocrinology, Diabetes and Metabolism ,Decision Making ,Closeness ,Interpersonal communication ,Stimulus (physiology) ,Social stimuli ,Oxytocin ,050105 experimental psychology ,Personal Space ,Young Adult ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,Endocrinology ,Double-Blind Method ,Reward ,Personal space ,medicine ,Humans ,Interpersonal Relations ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Social Behavior ,Biological Psychiatry ,Motivation ,Endocrine and Autonomic Systems ,05 social sciences ,Brain ,Magnetic Resonance Imaging ,Psychiatry and Mental health ,Nerve Net ,Psychology ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,medicine.drug ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Personal space, defined as the distance individuals choose to maintain between themselves and others, is an indicator of affiliation and closeness. Most paradigms that measure personal space preferences involve explicit choice and therefore fail to examine the implicit aspects of such preferences. In the current study, we sought to investigate an implicit form of interpersonal space that is more closely related to real-life situations involving affiliation. We studied the effects of oxytocin (OT) on neural networks that involve affiliation and tested the impact on personal space preferences. In a double-blind placebo-controlled study, we asked participants to choose between two rooms that differed only in the distances between two stimuli. The stimuli were either social stimuli (two chairs) or non-social stimuli (table and plant). The behavioral results showed that OT caused participants to choose a closer space in social blocks but did not affect their choices in non-social blocks. Imaging results revealed an interaction between stimulus and treatment (OT/PL) in the dorsal striatum, an area that is related to approach motivation and is part of the reward circuitry. Specifically, OT increased activity in the dorsal striatum in the social blocks and decreased this activity in the non-social blocks. The results of the study strengthen the social salience theory regarding OT, indicating that OT does not uniformly affect all social responses and that context has a determining impact on our behavior.
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
28. Corrigendum to The role of oxytocin in implicit personal space regulation: An fMRI study [Psychoneuroendocrinology 91 (2018) 206–215]
- Author
-
Daniela Cohen, Anat Perry, Naama Mayseless, Oded Kleinmintz, and Simone G. Shamay-Tsoory
- Subjects
Psychiatry and Mental health ,Endocrinology ,Endocrine and Autonomic Systems ,Endocrinology, Diabetes and Metabolism ,Biological Psychiatry - Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
29. Where does one stand: a biological account of preferred interpersonal distance
- Author
-
Nikolay Nichiporuk, Robert T. Knight, and Anat Perry
- Subjects
Male ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Sensory system ,Stimulation ,Interpersonal communication ,Electroencephalography ,050105 experimental psychology ,Developmental psychology ,Personal Space ,Young Adult ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,Stress (linguistics) ,medicine ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,medicine.diagnostic_test ,Sensory sensitivity ,05 social sciences ,Brain ,Original Articles ,General Medicine ,Social relation ,Alpha Rhythm ,Metric (mathematics) ,Female ,Psychology ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
What determines how close you choose to stand to someone? Why do some people prefer farther distances than others? We hypothesized that an important factor is one’s sensory sensitivity level, i.e. how sensitive one is to nearby visual stimulation, noise, touch or smell. This study characterizes the behavioral, hormonal and electrophysiological metrics of interpersonal distance (IPD) preferences in relation to levels of sensory sensitivity. Using both an ecologically realistic task and electroencephalogram (EEG), we found that sensory sensitivity levels predicted IPD preferences, such that the more sensitive one is the farther distance they prefer. Furthermore, electrophysiological evidence revealed that individuals with higher sensory sensitivity show more alpha suppression for approaching stimuli, strengthening the notion that early sensory cortical excitability is involved in one’s social decision of how close to stand to another. The results provide evidence that a core human metric of social interaction is influenced by individual levels of sensory sensitivity.
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
30. Interpersonal distance and social anxiety in autistic spectrum disorders: A behavioral and ERP study
- Author
-
Gal Richter-Levin, Anat Perry, Simone G. Shamay-Tsoory, and Einat Levy-Gigi
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Social Psychology ,Population ,Interpersonal communication ,Anxiety ,Development ,Autistic spectrum ,Developmental psychology ,Young Adult ,Behavioral Neuroscience ,Social space ,medicine ,Humans ,Interpersonal Relations ,Autistic Disorder ,Social Behavior ,education ,Evoked Potentials ,Psychiatric Status Rating Scales ,Brain Mapping ,education.field_of_study ,Social anxiety ,Electroencephalography ,Variance (accounting) ,medicine.disease ,Autism ,Female ,Psychology ,Photic Stimulation ,Social behavior - Abstract
An inherent feature of social interactions is the use of social space or interpersonal distance-the space between one individual and another. Because social deficits are core symptoms of Autistic Spectrum Disorder (ASD), we hypothesized that individuals on this spectrum will exhibit abnormal interpersonal distance preferences. The literature on interpersonal distance in ASD is not conclusive. While some studies show preferences for closer distances among this group, others show preferences for farther distances than controls. A common symptom of ASD that may explain the variance in responses to interpersonal distance in this population is social anxiety (SA), which has been shown to correlate with interpersonal distance preferences. In the current study, we investigated interpersonal distance preferences in a group of individuals with ASD using both behavioral and ERP measures. We found greater variance in interpersonal distance preferences in the ASD group than in the control group. Furthermore, we showed that this variance can be explained by differences in SA level and can be predicted by the N1 amplitude, an early ERP component related to attention and discrimination processes. These results hint at the early sensory and attentional processes that may be affecting higher social behaviors, both in subclinical and in clinical populations.
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
31. OT promotes closer interpersonal distance among highly empathic individuals
- Author
-
David Mankuta, Simone G. Shamay-Tsoory, and Anat Perry
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Empathy ,Interpersonal communication ,Neuropsychological Tests ,Social stimuli ,Oxytocin ,Affect (psychology) ,Developmental psychology ,Young Adult ,Humans ,Interpersonal Relations ,media_common ,Social distance ,Original Articles ,General Medicine ,Perceptual salience ,Social cue ,Psychological Distance ,Social Perception ,Feeling ,Cues ,Psychology ,Social psychology ,Photic Stimulation - Abstract
The space between people, or ‘interpersonal distance’, creates and defines the dynamics of social interactions and is a salient cue signaling responsiveness and feeling comfortable. This distance is implicit yet clearly felt, especially if someone stands closer or farther away than expected. Increasing evidence suggests that Oxytocin (OT) serves as a social hormone in humans, and that one of its roles may be to alter the perceptual salience of social cues. Considering that empathic ability may shape the way individuals process social stimuli, we predicted that OT will differentially affect preferred interpersonal distance depending on individual differences in empathy. Participants took part in two interpersonal distance experiments: In the first, they had to stop a (computer visualized) protagonist when feeling most comfortable; in the second, they were asked to choose the room in which they would later discuss intimate topics with another. Both experiments revealed an interaction between the effect of OT and empathy level. Among highly empathic individuals, OT promoted the choice of closer interpersonal distances. Yet, OT had an opposite effect on individuals with low empathic traits. We conclude that the enhancement of social cues following OT administration may have opposite effects on individuals with different empathic abilities.
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
32. Dynamics of the EEG power in the frequency and spatial domains during observation and execution of manual movements
- Author
-
Dario G. Liebermann, Anat Perry, Silvi Frenkel-Toledo, Nachum Soroker, and Shlomo Bentin
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Photic Stimulation ,Movement ,Alpha (ethology) ,Electroencephalography ,Brain mapping ,Rhythm ,medicine ,Humans ,Mirror Neurons ,Molecular Biology ,Mirror neuron ,Aged ,Brain Mapping ,Communication ,medicine.diagnostic_test ,business.industry ,General Neuroscience ,Brain ,Middle Aged ,Brain Waves ,Alpha Rhythm ,medicine.anatomical_structure ,Scalp ,Mu wave ,Female ,Neurology (clinical) ,Psychology ,business ,Neuroscience ,Psychomotor Performance ,Developmental Biology - Abstract
Mu suppression is the attenuation of EEG power in the alpha frequency range (8-12 Hz) while executing or observing a motor action. Whereas typically observed at central scalp sites, there are diverging reports about the extent of the attenuation over the cortical mantle, its exact frequency range and the specificity of this phenomenon. We investigated the modulation of EEG oscillations in frequency-bands between 4 to 12 Hz at frontal, central, parietal and occipital sites during the execution of manual movements and during observation of similar actions from allocentric (i.e., facing the actor) and egocentric (i.e., seeing the actor from behind) viewpoints. Suppression was determined relative to observation of a non-biological movement. Action observation elicited greater suppression in the lower (8-10 Hz) compared to the higher mu range (10-12 Hz), and greater suppression in the entire range (4-12 Hz) at frontal and central sites compared to parietal and occipital sites. In addition, suppression tended to be greater during observation of a motor action from allocentric compared to egocentric viewpoints. During execution of movement, suppression of the EEG occurred primarily in the higher alpha range and was absent at occipital sites. In the theta range (4-8 Hz), the EEG amplitude was suppressed during action observation and execution. The results suggest a functional distinction between modulation of mu and alpha rhythms, and between the higher and lower ranges of the mu rhythms. The activity of the presumed human mirror-neuron system seems primarily evident in the lower mu range and in the theta range.
- Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
33. Mirroring in the Human Brain: Deciphering the Spatial-Temporal Patterns of the Human Mirror Neuron System
- Author
-
Josef Parvizi, Jack J. Lin, Jennifer Stiso, Robert T. Knight, Anat Perry, and Edward F. Chang
- Subjects
0301 basic medicine ,Adult ,Male ,Time Factors ,Computer science ,1.1 Normal biological development and functioning ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,media_common.quotation_subject ,motor simulation ,imitation ,03 medical and health sciences ,Cellular and Molecular Neuroscience ,Electrocardiography ,Young Adult ,0302 clinical medicine ,Clinical Research ,Underpinning research ,medicine ,Reaction Time ,Psychology ,Humans ,Electrocorticography ,Mirror Neurons ,Mirror neuron ,media_common ,Brain Mapping ,medicine.diagnostic_test ,Hand Strength ,Neurosciences ,Representation (systemics) ,Motor Cortex ,Experimental Psychology ,Cognition ,Human brain ,Original Articles ,Middle Aged ,ECoG ,030104 developmental biology ,medicine.anatomical_structure ,Embodied cognition ,Neurological ,Cognitive Sciences ,Female ,Imitation ,Neuroscience ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Psychomotor Performance ,Mirroring - Abstract
Embodied theories of cognition emphasize the central role of sensorimotor transformations in the representation of others' actions. Support for these theories is derived from the discovery of the mirror neuron system (MNS) in primates, from noninvasive techniques in humans, and from a limited number of intracranial studies. To understand the neural dynamics of the human MNS, more studies with precise spatial and temporal resolutions are essential. We used electrocorticography to define activation patterns in sensorimotor, parietal and/or frontal neuronal populations, during a viewing and grasping task. Our results show robust high gamma activation for both conditions in classic MNS sites. Furthermore, we provide novel evidence for 2 different populations of neurons: sites that were only active for viewing and grasping ("pure mirroring") and sites that were also active between viewing and grasping, and perhaps serve a more general attentional role. Lastly, a subgroup of parietal electrodes showed earlier peaks than all other regions. These results highlight the complexity of spatial-temporal patterns within the MNS and provide a critical link between single-unit research in monkeys and noninvasive techniques in human.
- Published
- 2017
34. Effects of prefrontal cortex damage on emotion understanding: EEG and behavioural evidence
- Author
-
Anne-Kristin Solbakk, Robert T. Knight, Tor Endestad, Torstein R. Meling, Anat Perry, Jamie Lubell, Jennifer Stiso, Samantha N. Saunders, and Callum Dewar
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Image Processing ,Emotions ,Motion Perception ,Inferior frontal gyrus ,Prefrontal Cortex ,Brain Neoplasms/surgery ,Electroencephalography ,050105 experimental psychology ,Task (project management) ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,Computer-Assisted ,Kinesics ,medicine ,Image Processing, Computer-Assisted ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Prefrontal cortex ,Mirror Neurons ,Prefrontal Cortex/diagnostic imaging/injuries ,Mirror neuron ,media_common ,medicine.diagnostic_test ,Gestures ,Brain Neoplasms ,05 social sciences ,Original Articles ,Middle Aged ,ddc:616.8 ,Stroke ,Stroke/diagnostic imaging/psychology ,Action (philosophy) ,Feeling ,Social Perception ,Female ,Neurology (clinical) ,Cues ,Consumer neuroscience ,Psychology ,Comprehension ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Psychomotor Performance ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Humans are highly social beings that interact with each other on a daily basis. In these complex interactions, we get along by being able to identify others' actions and infer their intentions, thoughts and feelings. One of the major theories accounting for this critical ability assumes that the understanding of social signals is based on a primordial tendency to simulate observed actions by activating a mirror neuron system. If mirror neuron regions are important for action and emotion recognition, damage to regions in this network should lead to deficits in these domains. In the current behavioural and EEG study, we focused on the lateral prefrontal cortex including dorsal and ventral prefrontal cortex and utilized a series of task paradigms, each measuring a different aspect of recognizing others' actions or emotions from body cues. We examined 17 patients with lesions including (n = 8) or not including (n = 9) the inferior frontal gyrus, a core mirror neuron system region, and compared their performance to matched healthy control subjects (n = 18), in behavioural tasks and in an EEG observation-execution task measuring mu suppression. Our results provide support for the role of the lateral prefrontal cortex in understanding others' emotions, by showing that even unilateral lesions result in deficits in both accuracy and reaction time in tasks involving the recognition of others' emotions. In tasks involving the recognition of actions, patients showed a general increase in reaction time, but not a reduction in accuracy. Deficits in emotion recognition can be seen by either direct damage to the inferior frontal gyrus, or via damage to dorsal lateral prefrontal cortex regions, resulting in deteriorated performance and less EEG mu suppression over sensorimotor cortex.
- Published
- 2017
35. Are you looking at me? Mu suppression modulation by facial expression direction
- Author
-
Hillel Aviezer, Noga S. Ensenberg, and Anat Perry
- Subjects
Male ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,Emotions ,Motion Perception ,Wavelet Analysis ,Neuropsychological Tests ,Facial recognition system ,050105 experimental psychology ,03 medical and health sciences ,Behavioral Neuroscience ,Young Adult ,0302 clinical medicine ,Orientation ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Interpersonal Relations ,Motion perception ,Mirror neuron ,Facial expression ,Communication ,Analysis of Variance ,Fourier Analysis ,Social perception ,business.industry ,05 social sciences ,Brain ,Cognition ,Electroencephalography ,Expression (mathematics) ,Facial Expression ,Social Perception ,Female ,Psychology ,business ,Facial Recognition ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Photic Stimulation ,Mirroring ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Although we encounter numerous expressive faces on a daily basis, those that are not aimed at us will often be disregarded. Facial expressions aimed at our direction appear far more relevant and evoke an engaging affective experience, while the exact same expressions aimed away from us may not. While the importance of expression directionality is intuitive and commonplace, the neural mechanisms underlying this phenomenon are largely unknown. In the current study we measured EEG mu rhythm suppression, an established measure of mirror neuron activity, while participants viewed short video clips of dynamic facial expressions. Critically, the videos portrayed facial emotions which turned towards or away from the viewer, thus manipulating their degree of social relevance. Mirroring activity increased as a function of social relevance such that expressions turning toward the viewer resulted in increased sensorimotor activation (i.e., stronger mu suppression) compared to identical expressions turning away from the viewer. Additional analyses confirmed that expressions turning toward the viewer were perceived as more relevant and engaging than expressions turning away from the viewer, a finding not explained by perceived intensity or recognition accuracy. Mirror sensorimotor mechanisms may play a key role in determining the relevance of perceived facial expressions.
- Published
- 2016
36. The role of oxytocin in modulating interpersonal space: A pharmacological fMRI study
- Author
-
Daniela Cohen, Tal Gonen, Anat Perry, Simone G. Shamay-Tsoory, Talma Hendler, Naama Mayseless, and Gadi Gilam
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Adolescent ,Distancing ,Endocrinology, Diabetes and Metabolism ,Closeness ,Theory of Mind ,Prefrontal Cortex ,Context (language use) ,Interpersonal communication ,Oxytocin ,050105 experimental psychology ,Developmental psychology ,03 medical and health sciences ,Nonverbal communication ,Personal Space ,Young Adult ,0302 clinical medicine ,Endocrinology ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Interpersonal Relations ,Social Behavior ,Biological Psychiatry ,Neurotransmitter Agents ,Salience (language) ,Endocrine and Autonomic Systems ,05 social sciences ,Social cue ,Magnetic Resonance Imaging ,Psychiatry and Mental health ,Mentalization ,Psychology ,Social psychology ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery - Abstract
Interpersonal space is a nonverbal indicator of affiliation and closeness. In this study we investigated the effects of oxytocin (OT), a neuropeptide known for its social role in humans, on interpersonal space. In a double blind placebo controlled study we measured the effect of intranasal OT on the personal distance preferences of different familiar (friend) and unfamiliar (stranger) protagonists. Behavioral results showed that participants preferred to be closer to a friend than to a stranger. Intranasal OT was associated with an overall distancing effect, but this effect was significant for the stranger and not for the friend. The imaging results showed interactions between treatment (OT, placebo) and protagonist (friend, stranger) in regions that mediate social behavior including the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (dmPFC), a region associated with the mentalizing system. Specifically, OT increased activity in the dmPFC when a friend approached the participants but not when a stranger approached. The results indicate that the effect of OT on interpersonal space greatly depends on the participant's relationship with the protagonist. This supports the social salience theory, according to which OT increases the salience of social cues depending on the context.
- Published
- 2016
37. Does focusing on hand-grasping intentions modulate electroencephalogram μ and α suppressions?
- Author
-
Anat Perry and Shlomo Bentin
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Electroencephalography ,Somatosensory system ,Young Adult ,Rhythm ,medicine ,Humans ,Attention ,Motor activity ,Mirror neuron ,medicine.diagnostic_test ,General Neuroscience ,Neural Inhibition ,Body movement ,Brain Waves ,Imitative Behavior ,Alpha Rhythm ,Electrophysiology ,Female ,Psychology ,Perceptual Masking ,Neuroscience ,Psychomotor Performance ,Cognitive load - Abstract
Understanding the intentions of others presumably involves a human analog of the mirror neuron system. A putative marker of such mirror activity is the suppression of electroencephalographic oscillations in the 8-12 Hz range, which, when recorded over somatosensory areas, is associated with motor activity and labeled μ rhythms. We investigated whether μ-suppression can be modulated by attention to another person's intention as expressed by her hand movement toward an object and whether this suppression is distinguished from the suppression of α waves that oscillate in the same frequency range and are modulated by attention and cognitive load. Both μ and α suppressions were modulated by task difficulty, and not distinctively by intention, reflecting the recruitment of resources needed for task performance.
- Published
- 2010
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
38. Exploring motor system contributions to the perception of social information: Evidence from EEG activity in the mu/alpha frequency range
- Author
-
Shlomo Bentin, Anat Perry, and Nikolaus F. Troje
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Social Psychology ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Motion Perception ,Empathy ,Intention ,Motor Activity ,Neuropsychological Tests ,Development ,Audiology ,Electroencephalography ,Motion (physics) ,Developmental psychology ,Young Adult ,Behavioral Neuroscience ,Sex Factors ,Perception ,Motor system ,Reaction Time ,medicine ,Humans ,Mirror neuron ,Emotional Intelligence ,media_common ,Brain Mapping ,medicine.diagnostic_test ,Motor Cortex ,Body movement ,Alpha Rhythm ,Female ,Psychology ,Photic Stimulation ,Biological motion - Abstract
Putative contributions of a human mirror neuron system (hMNS) to the perception of social information have been assessed by measuring the suppression of EEG oscillations in the mu/alpha (8-12 Hz), beta (15-25 Hz) and low-gamma (25-25 Hz) ranges while participants processed social information revealed by point-light displays of human motion. Identical dynamic displays were presented and participants were instructed to distinguish the intention, the emotion, or the gender of a moving image of a person, while they performed an adapted odd-ball task. Relative to a baseline presenting a nonbiological but meaningful motion display, all three biological motion conditions reduced the EEG amplitude in the mu/alpha and beta ranges, but not in the low-gamma range. Suppression was larger in the intention than in the emotion and gender conditions, with no difference between the latter two. Moreover, the suppression in the intention condition was negatively correlated with an accepted measure of empathy (EQ), revealing that participants high in empathy scores manifested less suppression. For intention and emotion the suppression was larger at occipital than at central sites, suggesting that factors other than motor system were in play while processing social information embedded in the motion of point-light displays.
- Published
- 2010
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
39. Mirror activity in the human brain while observing hand movements: A comparison between EEG desynchronization in the μ-range and previous fMRI results
- Author
-
Anat Perry and Shlomo Bentin
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,genetic structures ,Movement ,Neuropsychological Tests ,Electroencephalography ,Alpha wave ,Functional Laterality ,Hand movements ,Young Adult ,Rhythm ,medicine ,Humans ,Cortical Synchronization ,Evoked Potentials ,Molecular Biology ,Mirror neuron ,Cerebral Cortex ,Brain Mapping ,medicine.diagnostic_test ,General Neuroscience ,Motor Cortex ,Human brain ,Hand ,Imitative Behavior ,Magnetic Resonance Imaging ,Functional imaging ,Alpha Rhythm ,Electrophysiology ,medicine.anatomical_structure ,Female ,Neurology (clinical) ,Psychology ,Neuroscience ,Photic Stimulation ,Psychomotor Performance ,Developmental Biology - Abstract
Mu (μ) rhythms are EEG oscillations between 8–13 Hz distinguished from alpha by having more anterior distribution and being desynchronized by motor rather than visual activity. Evidence accumulating during the last decade suggests that the desynchronization of μ rhythms (μ suppression) might be also a manifestation of a human Mirror Neuron System (MNS). To further explore this hypothesis we used a paradigm that, in a previous fMRI study, successfully activated this putative MNS in humans. Our direct goal was to provide further support for a link between modulation of μ rhythms and the MNS, by finding parallels between the reported patterns of fMRI activations and patterns of μ suppression. The EEG power in the μ range has been recorded while participants passively observed either a left or a right hand, reaching to and grasping objects, and compared it with that recorded while participants observed the movement of a ball, and while observing static grasping scenes or still objects. Mirroring fMRI results (Shmuelof, L., Zohary, E., 2005. Dissociation between ventral and dorsal fMRI activation during object and action recognition. Neuron 47, 457–470), μ suppression was larger in the hemisphere contra-lateral to the moving hand and larger when the hands grasped different objects in different ways than when the movement was repetitive. No suppression was found while participants observed still objects but μ suppression was also found while seeing static grasping postures. These data are discussed in light of similar parallels between modulations of alpha waves and fMRI while recording EEG in the magnet. The present data support a link between μ suppression and a human MNS.
- Published
- 2009
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
40. Seeing with Profoundly Deactivated Mid-level Visual Areas: Non-hierarchical Functioning in the Human Visual Cortex
- Author
-
Shlomo Bentin, Sharon Gilaie-Dotan, Rafael Malach, Yoram Bonneh, and Anat Perry
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,genetic structures ,Visual N1 ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,Articles ,Visual system ,Cellular and Molecular Neuroscience ,Visual cortex ,medicine.anatomical_structure ,Visual memory ,Biased Competition Theory ,Agnosia ,Visual Perception ,medicine ,Evoked Potentials, Visual ,Humans ,Female ,Vision for perception and vision for action ,Psychology ,N2pc ,Visual Cortex ,Cognitive psychology ,Visual agnosia - Abstract
A fundamental concept in visual processing is that activity in high-order object-category distinctive regions (e.g., lateral occipital complex, fusiform face area, middle temporal+) is dependent on bottom-up flow of activity in earlier retinotopic areas (V2, V3, V4) whose main input originates from primary visual cortex (V1). Thus, activity in down stream areas should reflect lower-level inputs. Here we qualify this notion reporting case LG, a rare case of developmental object agnosia and prosopagnosia. In this person, V1 was robustly activated by visual stimuli, yet intermediate areas (V2–V4) were strongly deactivated. Despite this intermediate deactivation, activity in down stream visual areas remained robust, showing selectivity for houses and places, while selectivity for faces and objects was impaired. The extent of impairment evident in functional magnetic resonance imaging and electroencephalography activations was somewhat larger in the left hemisphere. This pattern of brain activity, coupled with fairly adequate everyday visual performance is compatible with models emphasizing the role of nonlinear local “amplification” of neuronal inputs in eliciting activity in ventral and dorsal visual pathways as well as perceptual experience in the human brain. Thus, while the proper functioning of intermediate areas appears essential for specialization in the cortex, daily visual behavior and reading are maintained even with deactivated intermediate visual areas.
- Published
- 2008
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
41. The role of the orbitofrontal cortex in regulation of interpersonal space: evidence from frontal lesion and frontotemporal dementia patients
- Author
-
Robert T. Knight, Anat Perry, Sandy J. Lwi, Robert W. Levenson, Callum Dewar, and Alice Verstaen
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,Prefrontal Cortex ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia ,050105 experimental psychology ,Lesion ,03 medical and health sciences ,Personal Space ,0302 clinical medicine ,mental disorders ,medicine ,Social Norms ,Psychology ,Dementia ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Prefrontal cortex ,Social Behavior ,Aged ,Neural correlates of consciousness ,05 social sciences ,Neurosciences ,Experimental Psychology ,General Medicine ,Original Articles ,interpersonal distance ,Middle Aged ,medicine.disease ,Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex ,medicine.anatomical_structure ,Frontotemporal Dementia ,Cognitive Sciences ,Orbitofrontal cortex ,Female ,medicine.symptom ,orbitofrontal cortex ,Neuroscience ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Social behavior ,Frontotemporal dementia - Abstract
Interpersonal distance is central to communication and complex social behaviors but the neural correlates of interpersonal distance preferences are not defined. Previous studies suggest that damage to the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) is associated with impaired interpersonal behavior. To examine whether the OFC is critical for maintaining appropriate interpersonal distance, we tested two groups of patients with OFC damage: Patients with OFC lesions and patients with behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia. These two groups were compared to healthy controls and to patients with lesions restricted to the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Only patients with OFC damage showed abnormal interpersonal distance preferences, which were significantly different from both controls and patients with dorsolateral prefrontal damage. The comfortable distances these patients chose with strangers were significantly closer than the other groups and resembled distances normally used with close others. These results shed light on the role of the OFC in regulating social behavior and may serve as a simple diagnostic tool for dementia or lesion patients.
- Published
- 2016
42. How Do We Understand Other People?
- Author
-
Anat Perry and Jennifer Stiso
- Subjects
0301 basic medicine ,03 medical and health sciences ,030104 developmental biology ,0302 clinical medicine ,General Medicine ,Psychology ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Neuroscience - Abstract
Imagine this: you walk into class and see your friend sitting alone at a table. You notice your friend is looking downward, with a frown on her face. You would probably think from these clues that your friend is sad. But how did you know that? One way that your brain accomplishes this is by simulating or copying in your mind what you see the other person doing. This may help you understand that when you are frowning and looking downward, you are usually sad, so it is probably the case that your friend is sad too. While there are other hypotheses for how our brain understands others, we are going to focus on simulation, and how special cells in the brain – called mirror neurons – may help to make simulation possible. We will first examine neuroimaging experiments, in monkeys and in humans, in which we use technology to get an indication of brain activity. These brain activations help us understand simulation better. Lastly, we will discuss disorders such as autism, in which it may be more difficult to understand others’ actions, intentions, and emotions.
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
43. Working memory capacity of biological movements predicts empathy traits
- Author
-
Tian Ye, Anat Perry, Mowei Shen, and Zaifeng Gao
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Emotional empathy ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Motion Perception ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Empathy ,050105 experimental psychology ,03 medical and health sciences ,Young Adult ,0302 clinical medicine ,Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,Social cognition ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Social information ,media_common ,Working memory ,05 social sciences ,Cognition ,Memory, Short-Term ,Social Perception ,Female ,Psychology ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Working memory (WM) and empathy are core issues in cognitive and social science, respectively. However, no study so far has explored the relationship between these two constructs. Considering that empathy takes place based on the others’ observed experiences, which requires extracting the observed dynamic scene into WM and forming a coherent representation, we hypothesized that a sub-type of WM capacity, i.e., WM for biological movements (BM), should predict one’s empathy level. Therefore, WM capacity was measured for three distinct types of stimuli in a change detection task: BM of human beings (BM; Experiment 1), movements of rectangles (Experiment 2), and static colors (Experiment 3). The first two stimuli were dynamic and shared one WM buffer which differed from the WM buffer for colors; yet only the BM conveyed social information. We found that BM-WM capacity was positively correlated with both cognitive and emotional empathy, with no such correlations for WM capacity of movements of rectangles or of colors. Thus, the current study is the first to provide evidence linking a specific buffer of WM and empathy, and highlights the necessity for considering different WM capacities in future social and clinical research.
- Published
- 2015
44. Training-induced recovery of low-level vision followed by mid-level perceptual improvements in developmental object and face agnosia
- Author
-
Shlomo Bentin, Oren Yehezkel, Uri Polat, Dana Gotthilf-Nezri, Anat Perry, Sharon Gilaie-Dotan, Joseph L. Brooks, Maria Lev, and Yoram Bonneh
- Subjects
Male ,Visual acuity ,Visual perception ,genetic structures ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Visual Acuity ,BF ,Functional Laterality ,Young Adult ,Face perception ,Perception ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,medicine ,Humans ,BF712 ,media_common ,Teaching ,Cognitive neuroscience of visual object recognition ,Recognition, Psychology ,Recovery of Function ,Crowding ,eye diseases ,Stereopsis ,Agnosia ,Face ,Papers ,Visual Perception ,medicine.symptom ,Psychology ,Photic Stimulation ,Psychomotor Performance ,Cognitive psychology ,Follow-Up Studies - Abstract
Long-term deprivation of normal visual inputs can cause perceptual impairments at various levels of visual function, from basic visual acuity deficits, through mid-level deficits such as contour integration and motion coherence, to high-level face and object agnosia. Yet it is unclear whether training during adulthood, at a post-developmental stage of the adult visual system, can overcome such developmental impairments. Here, we visually trained LG, a developmental object and face agnosic individual. Prior to training, at the age of 20, LG's basic and mid-level visual functions such as visual acuity, crowding effects, and contour integration were underdeveloped relative to normal adult vision, corresponding to or poorer than those of 5-6 year olds (Gilaie-Dotan, Perry, Bonneh, Malach & Bentin, 2009). Intensive visual training, based on lateral interactions, was applied for a period of 9 months. LG's directly trained but also untrained visual functions such as visual acuity, crowding, binocular stereopsis and also mid-level contour integration improved significantly and reached near-age-level performance, with long-term (over 4 years) persistence. Moreover, mid-level functions that were tested post-training were found to be normal in LG. Some possible subtle improvement was observed in LG's higher-order visual functions such as object recognition and part integration, while LG's face perception skills have not improved thus far. These results suggest that corrective training at a post-developmental stage, even in the adult visual system, can prove effective, and its enduring effects are the basis for a revival of a developmental cascade that can lead to reduced perceptual impairments.
- Published
- 2014
45. Understanding emotional and cognitive empathy
- Author
-
Simone G. Shamay-Tsoory and Anat Perry
- Subjects
Cognitive empathy ,Emotional expression ,Psychology ,Cognitive psychology - Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
46. Face or body? Oxytocin improves perception of emotions from facial expressions in incongruent emotional body context
- Author
-
Pavel Goldstein, Sharon Palgi, Simone G. Shamay-Tsoory, Anat Perry, Hillel Aviezer, and Ehud Klein
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Visual perception ,Endocrinology, Diabetes and Metabolism ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Emotions ,Oxytocin ,Endocrinology ,Double-Blind Method ,Social cognition ,Emotion perception ,Perception ,Kinesics ,Humans ,Emotional expression ,Emotion recognition ,Biological Psychiatry ,Administration, Intranasal ,media_common ,Facial expression ,Endocrine and Autonomic Systems ,Recognition, Psychology ,Middle Aged ,Facial Expression ,Psychiatry and Mental health ,Visual Perception ,Female ,Psychology ,Photic Stimulation ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
The neuropeptide oxytocin (OT) has been repeatedly reported to play an essential role in the regulation of social cognition in humans in general, and specifically in enhancing the recognition of emotions from facial expressions. The later was assessed in different paradigms that rely primarily on isolated and decontextualized emotional faces. However, recent evidence has indicated that the perception of basic facial expressions is not context invariant and can be categorically altered by context, especially body context, at early perceptual levels. Body context has a strong effect on our perception of emotional expressions, especially when the actual target face and the contextually expected face are perceptually similar. To examine whether and how OT affects emotion recognition, we investigated the role of OT in categorizing facial expressions in incongruent body contexts. Our results show that in the combined process of deciphering emotions from facial expressions and from context, OT gives an advantage to the face. This advantage is most evident when the target face and the contextually expected face are perceptually similar.
- Published
- 2013
47. 'Feeling' the pain of those who are different from us: Modulation of EEG in the mu/alpha range
- Author
-
Inbal Ben-Ami Bartal, Shlomo Bentin, Anat Perry, Claus Lamm, and Jean Decety
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Adolescent ,Healthy participant ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Emotions ,Pain ,Empathy ,Stimulation ,Audiology ,Electroencephalography ,Developmental psychology ,Behavioral Neuroscience ,Young Adult ,Alexithymia ,Physical Stimulation ,medicine ,Reaction Time ,Humans ,Mirror neuron ,Empathic concern ,media_common ,Pain Measurement ,Analysis of Variance ,Brain Mapping ,medicine.diagnostic_test ,Neural Inhibition ,medicine.disease ,Hand ,Alpha Rhythm ,Feeling ,Female ,Psychology - Abstract
We explored how apparently painful stimuli and the ability to identify with the person on whom the pain is inflicted modulate EEG suppression in the mu/alpha range (8-12 Hz). In a 2 × 2 design, we presented pictures of hands either experiencing needle pricks or being touched by a Q-tip. In the dissimilar-other condition, the hand was assigned to a patient suffering from a neurological disease in which Q-tips inflicted pain, whereas needle pricks did not. In the similar-other condition, the hand was assigned to a patient who responded to stimulation in the same way as the healthy participant. Participants were instructed to imagine the feeling of the person whose hand was shown and to evaluate his or her affective state. Pain conditions elicited greater EEG suppression than did nonpain conditions, particularly over frontocentral regions. Moreover, an interaction between pain and similarity revealed that for similar others, the pain effect was significant, whereas in the dissimilar-other group, suppression was equally large in the pain and no-pain conditions. We conclude that mu/alpha suppression is elicited both automatically, by observing a situation that is potentially painful for the observer, and by empathy for pain, even if the other person is different from oneself.
- Published
- 2010
48. Intranasal oxytocin modulates EEG mu/alpha and beta rhythms during perception of biological motion
- Author
-
Richard P. Ebstein, Anat Perry, Florina Uzefovsky, Idan Shalev, Dori Bar-On, Salomon Israel, and Shlomo Bentin
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Endocrinology, Diabetes and Metabolism ,Motion Perception ,Neuropeptide ,Alpha (ethology) ,Electroencephalography ,Oxytocin ,Brain mapping ,Young Adult ,Endocrinology ,Double-Blind Method ,medicine ,Humans ,Beta Rhythm ,Social Behavior ,Biological Psychiatry ,Administration, Intranasal ,Brain Mapping ,medicine.diagnostic_test ,Endocrine and Autonomic Systems ,Body movement ,Human brain ,Psychiatry and Mental health ,Alpha Rhythm ,medicine.anatomical_structure ,Psychology ,Neuroscience ,Photic Stimulation ,Psychomotor Performance ,medicine.drug - Abstract
Oxytocin (OT) plays a determining role in social and pair bonding in many vertebrates and increasing evidence suggests it is a social hormone also in humans. Indeed, intranasal administration of OT modulates several social cognitive processes in humans. Electrophysiological studies in humans associated the suppression of EEG in the mu/alpha and beta bands with perception of biological motion and social stimuli. It has been suggested that mu and beta suppression over sensory-motor regions reflects a resonance system in the human brain analogous to mirror neurons in the monkey. We therefore hypothesized that OT, a social hormone, would enhance this suppression, hence, for the first time, link the action of this neuropeptide with a human correlate of mirror neuron activity. Twenty-four students were administered 24 IU of OT or placebo intranasally in a robust, double-blind within-subject design. 45 min later participants were shown a point-light display of continuous biological motion of a human figure's walk. In the 8-10 Hz (low alpha/mu band) and in the 15-25 Hz beta band, a significant main effect of treatment showed that suppression was significantly enhanced in the OT versus the placebo conditions and that this suppression was widespread across the scalp. These results are a first step linking OT to the modulation of EEG rhythms in humans, suggesting that OT may have a role in allocating cortical resources to social tasks partly mediated by mirror neuron activity.
- Published
- 2010
49. Training-induced recovery of low-level vision followed by high-level perceptual improvements in an adult with developmental object and face agnosia
- Author
-
Anat Perry, Sharon Gilaie-Dotan, Yoram Bonneh, Shlomo Bentin, Oren Yehezkel, Dana Gotthilf-Nezri, Maria Lev, and Uri Polat
- Subjects
Ophthalmology ,Agnosia ,Perception ,media_common.quotation_subject ,medicine ,Face (sociological concept) ,medicine.symptom ,Psychology ,Object (philosophy) ,Sensory Systems ,media_common ,Visual agnosia ,Cognitive psychology - Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
50. "Feeling" the pain of those who are different from us: Modulation of EEG in the mu/alpha range.
- Author
-
Anat Perry
- Subjects
- *
SENSORY perception , *ELECTROENCEPHALOGRAPHY , *PAIN , *BRAIN diseases , *BRAIN stimulation , *IDENTITY (Psychology) , *DIFFERENTIAL psychology , *PATIENTS - Abstract
We explored how apparently painful stimuli and the ability to identify with the person on whom the pain is inflicted modulate EEG suppression in the mu/alpha range (8â12 Hz). In a 2 x 2 design, we presented pictures of hands either experiencing needle pricks or being touched by a Q-tip. In the dissimilar-other condition, the hand was assigned to a patient suffering from a neurological disease in which Q-tips inflicted pain, whereas needle pricks did not. In the similar-other condition, the hand was assigned to a patient who responded to stimulation in the same way as the healthy participant. Participants were instructed to imagine the feeling of the person whose hand was shown and to evaluate his or her affective state. Pain conditions elicited greater EEG suppression than did nonpain conditions, particularly over frontocentral regions. Moreover, an interaction between pain and similarity revealed that for similar others, the pain effect was significant, whereas in the dissimilar-other group, suppression was equally large in the pain and no-pain conditions. We conclude that mu/alpha suppression is elicited both automatically, by observing a situation that is potentially painful for the observer, and by empathy for pain, even if the other person is different from oneself. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2010
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.