151. Event representations constrain the structure of language: Sign language as a window into universally accessible linguistic biases
- Author
-
Meltem Kelepir, Emmanuel Chemla, Philippe Schlenker, Roland Pfau, Carlo Geraci, Brent Strickland, Institut Jean-Nicod (IJN), Département d'Etudes Cognitives - ENS Paris (DEC), École normale supérieure - Paris (ENS Paris), Université Paris sciences et lettres (PSL)-Université Paris sciences et lettres (PSL)-École normale supérieure - Paris (ENS Paris), Université Paris sciences et lettres (PSL)-Université Paris sciences et lettres (PSL)-École des hautes études en sciences sociales (EHESS)-Collège de France (CdF (institution))-Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS)-Département de Philosophie - ENS Paris, Université Paris sciences et lettres (PSL)-Université Paris sciences et lettres (PSL), Laboratoire de sciences cognitives et psycholinguistique (LSCP), Université Paris sciences et lettres (PSL)-Université Paris sciences et lettres (PSL)-École des hautes études en sciences sociales (EHESS)-Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), and ACLC (FGw)
- Subjects
060201 languages & linguistics ,Multidisciplinary ,Italian Sign Language ,Computer science ,05 social sciences ,Telicity ,Sign (semiotics) ,Social Sciences ,06 humanities and the arts ,[SCCO.COGPSY]Cognitive science/domain_scco.cogpsy ,Sign language ,[SCCO.LING]Cognitive science/Linguistics ,050105 experimental psychology ,Linguistics ,language.human_language ,Comprehension ,[SCCO]Cognitive science ,Action (philosophy) ,0602 languages and literature ,language ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,[SHS.LANGUE]Humanities and Social Sciences/Linguistics ,Linguistic universal ,ComputingMilieux_MISCELLANEOUS ,Gesture - Abstract
According to a theoretical tradition dating back to Aristotle, verbs can be classified into two broad categories. Telic verbs (e.g., “decide,” “sell,” “die”) encode a logical endpoint, whereas atelic verbs (e.g., “think,” “negotiate,” “run”) do not, and the denoted event could therefore logically continue indefinitely. Here we show that sign languages encode telicity in a seemingly universal way and moreover that even nonsigners lacking any prior experience with sign language understand these encodings. In experiments 1–5, nonsigning English speakers accurately distinguished between telic (e.g., “decide”) and atelic (e.g., “think”) signs from (the historically unrelated) Italian Sign Language, Sign Language of the Netherlands, and Turkish Sign Language. These results were not due to participants' inferring that the sign merely imitated the action in question. In experiment 6, we used pseudosigns to show that the presence of a salient visual boundary at the end of a gesture was sufficient to elicit telic interpretations, whereas repeated movement without salient boundaries elicited atelic interpretations. Experiments 7–10 confirmed that these visual cues were used by all of the sign languages studied here. Together, these results suggest that signers and nonsigners share universally accessible notions of telicity as well as universally accessible “mapping biases” between telicity and visual form.
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF