142 results on '"Potter, Mary C."'
Search Results
102. Syntactic Priming in Immediate Recall of Sentences
- Author
-
Potter, Mary C., primary and Lombardi, Linda, additional
- Published
- 1998
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
103. Supportability Investment Decision Analysis Center (SIDAC): A Customer-Oriented Solution to Modeling and Analysis Problems
- Author
-
WRIGHT RESEARCH AND DEVELOPMENT CENTER WRIGHT-PATTERSON AFB OH, Silverman, Michael B, Potter, Mary C, WRIGHT RESEARCH AND DEVELOPMENT CENTER WRIGHT-PATTERSON AFB OH, Silverman, Michael B, and Potter, Mary C
- Abstract
The move to reduce the DoD budget, and the escalating cost of the systems DoD will buy and maintain, demands exceedingly astute decisions relating to supportability investments. Within the world of analysis and decision support there are a myriad of effective, sophisticated tools and techniques, scattered through the DoD and private sector. The SIDAC is dedicated to becoming a Center of Excellence for the dissemination of quality information, modeling and analysis advice, and an expert source for supportability-related studies and services. The SIDAC promises a credible, quality product or service for the investment manager -the future demands it., Presented at the IEEE 1990 National Aerospace and Electronics Conference (NAECON 1990) held in Dayton, Ohio, on 21-25 May 1990.
- Published
- 1990
104. Alternating between reading and listening within a sentence
- Author
-
Potter, Mary C., primary and Hooker, Christopher J., additional
- Published
- 1997
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
105. Conceptual Similarity Between Targets and Distractors in the Attentional Blink
- Author
-
Chun, Marvin M., primary, Bromberg, Hilary S., additional, and Potter, Mary C., additional
- Published
- 1994
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
106. The regeneration of syntax in short term memory
- Author
-
Lombardi, Linda, primary and Potter, Mary C, additional
- Published
- 1992
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
107. Interference in Detecting Multiple RSVP Targets: Effects of Similarity
- Author
-
Chun, Myungwoo M., primary and Potter, Mary C., additional
- Published
- 1992
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
108. Visual and phonological codes in repetition blindness
- Author
-
Bavelier, Daphne, primary and Potter, Mary C., additional
- Published
- 1991
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
109. Very short-term conceptual memory
- Author
-
Potter, Mary C., primary
- Published
- 1991
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
110. Constrained Formation of Object Representations.
- Author
-
O’Connor, Kevin J. and Potter, Mary C.
- Subjects
- *
RECOGNITION (Psychology) , *VISUAL perception - Abstract
Viewers were presented with a rapid sequence of very brief stimulus pairs, each of which consisted of a pictured object followed by a related or unrelated word. The form of relatedness between the picture and word was manipulated across experiments (identical concept, associated concept, ink color of the picture). Recognition memory for the pictures was affected not only by whether or not paired items were conceptually identical or semantically related, but also by whether or not the words named an irrelevant feature, ink color. These results show that sequential items are integrated on the basis of similarity at whatever level is available, so that the stability of the memory representation of one or both items is increased. We propose that a common mechanism may underlie integration, priming, and selective attention. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2002
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
111. The activation of phonology during silent Chinese word reading.
- Author
-
Yaoda Xu and Potter, Mary C.
- Subjects
- *
PHONOLOGY , *ORTHOGRAPHY & spelling , *CHINESE characters , *SEMANTICS - Abstract
Studies the role of phonology and orthography in silent Chinese compound-character reading using a semantic relatedness judgment task. Interference from a homophone of a target word semantically related to a cue word; Interference from orthographically similar nonhomophones of the targets; Phonology-first verification model.
- Published
- 1999
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
112. Forces that Should But Often Don't
- Author
-
Potter, Mary C.
- Published
- 1972
113. Neisser's Challenge
- Author
-
Potter, Mary C., primary
- Published
- 1983
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
114. Time to understand pictures and words
- Author
-
POTTER, MARY C., primary and FAULCONER, BARBARA A., additional
- Published
- 1975
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
115. Representation of a sentence and its pragmatic implications: Verbal, imagistic, or abstract?
- Author
-
Potter, Mary C., primary, Valian, Virginia V., additional, and Faulconer, Barbara A., additional
- Published
- 1977
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
116. Short-term conceptual memory for pictures.
- Author
-
Potter, Mary C., primary
- Published
- 1976
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
117. A tale of twin brothers
- Author
-
Potter, Mary C., Salley, Beverlee, and Fahy, J.C.
- Subjects
Twins -- Psychological aspects - Published
- 1980
118. Contingent Attentional Capture by Conceptually Relevant Images.
- Author
-
Wyble, Brad, Folk, Charles, and Potter, Mary C.
- Subjects
- *
VISUAL perception , *STIMULUS & response (Biology) , *NEUROPHYSIOLOGY , *EXPERIMENTAL psychology , *VISUAL discrimination - Abstract
Attentional capture is an unintentional shift of visuospatial attention to the location of a distractor that is either highly salient, or relevant to the current task set. The latter situation is referred to as contingent capture, in that the effect is contingent on a match between characteristics of the stimuli and the task-defined attentional-control settings of the viewer. Contingent capture has been demonstrated for low-level features, such as color, motion, and orientation. In the present paper we show that contingent capture can also occur for conceptual information at the superordinate level (e.g., sports equipment, marine animal, dessert food). This effect occurs rapidly (i.e., within 200 ms), is a spatial form of attention, and is contingent on attentional-control settings that change on each trial, suggesting that natural images can be decoded into their conceptual meaning to drive shifts of attention within the time course of a single fixation. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
119. Categorically Defined Targets Trigger Spatiotemporal Visual Attention.
- Author
-
Wyble, Brad, Bowman, Howard, and Potter, Mary C.
- Subjects
- *
VISUAL perception , *ATTENTION , *TELEPROMPTERS , *AUDITORY perception , *PRIMING (Psychology) - Abstract
Transient attention to a visually salient cue enhances processing of a subsequent target in the same spatial location between 50 to 150 ms after cue onset (K. Nakayama & M. Mackeben, 1989). Do stimuli from a categorically defined target set, such as letters or digits, also generate transient attention? Participants reported digit targets among keyboard symbols in a changing array of 8 items. When 1 target preceded a second target in the same location at a stimulus onset asynchrony of 107 ms (but not 213 ms), the second target was reported more often than in a condition in which there was no leading target. When the 2 targets were at different locations, report of the second target was impaired. With both letters and digits as targets, the enhancement effect was shown not to be due to category priming. Critically, the attentional benefit was present whether or not participants reported the leading target. Transient attention, contingent attentional capture, popout, and Lag 1 sparing in the attentional blink may involve a common mechanism for orienting processing resources towards salient and task relevant stimuli. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2009
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
120. COLLEGE AND UNIVERSITY GOVERNANCE: RENSSELAER POLYTECHNIC INSTITUTE.
- Author
-
BURGAN, MARY A., KEZAR, ADRIANNA, POTTER, MARY C., and STORTI, DUANE
- Subjects
- *
REPORT writing , *UNIVERSITY faculty , *SCHOOL administration , *TENURE of college teachers - Abstract
The article presents a report on the action taken in the summer of 2007 by the board of trustees and the administration of Rensselaer Polytechnic Institute (RPI) which suspended its faculty senate and replaced it with a transitional structure of faculty governance. The step was taken following the refusal of the senate to follow a board directive to amend its constitution to exclude all except the tenured and tenure-track faculty from the senate's constituency and membership. The investigating committee of the American Association of University Professors (AAUP) found the suspension of RPI's senate as a prima facie violation of shared governance.
- Published
- 2011
121. So Much to Read, So Little Time.
- Author
-
Rayner, Keith, Schotter, Elizabeth R., Masson, Michael E. J., Potter, Mary C., and Treiman, Rebecca
- Subjects
- *
SPEED reading , *READING comprehension , *SPEED reading instruction , *EYE movements , *WORD recognition - Abstract
The prospect of speed reading—reading at an increased speed without any loss of comprehension—has undeniable appeal. Speed reading has been an intriguing concept for decades, at least since Evelyn Wood introduced her Reading Dynamics training program in 1959. It has recently increased in popularity, with speed-reading apps and technologies being introduced for smartphones and digital devices. The current article reviews what the scientific community knows about the reading process—a great deal—and discusses the implications of the research findings for potential students of speed-reading training programs or purchasers of speed-reading apps. The research shows that there is a trade-off between speed and accuracy. It is unlikely that readers will be able to double or triple their reading speeds (e.g., from around 250 to 500–750 words per minute) while still being able to understand the text as well as if they read at normal speed. If a thorough understanding of the text is not the reader’s goal, then speed reading or skimming the text will allow the reader to get through it faster with moderate comprehension. The way to maintain high comprehension and get through text faster is to practice reading and to become a more skilled language user (e.g., through increased vocabulary). This is because language skill is at the heart of reading speed. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
122. Banana or fruit? Detection and recognition across categorical levels in RSVP
- Author
-
Mary C. Potter, Carl Erick Hagmann, Massachusetts Institute of Technology. Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Potter, Mary C., and Hagmann, Carl Erick
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Adolescent ,Concept Formation ,Entry Level ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Serial Learning ,Stimulus (physiology) ,Superordinate goals ,Young Adult ,Discrimination, Psychological ,Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,Concept learning ,Reaction Time ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,Humans ,Attention ,Categorical variable ,Cognitive neuroscience of visual object recognition ,Musa ,Recognition, Psychology ,Middle Aged ,Comprehension ,Pattern Recognition, Visual ,Categorization ,Fruit ,Female ,Psychology ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Pictured objects and scenes can be understood in a brief glimpse, but there is a debate about whether they are first encoded at the basic level (e.g., banana), as proposed by Rosch et al. (1976, Cognitive Psychology) , or at a superordinate level (e.g., fruit). The level at which we first categorize an object matters in everyday situations because it determines whether we approach, avoid, or ignore the object. In the present study, we limited stimulus duration in order to explore the earliest level of object understanding. Target objects were presented among five other pictures using RSVP at 80, 53, 27, or 13 ms/picture. On each trial, participants viewed or heard 1 of 28 superordinate names or a corresponding basic-level name of the target. The name appeared before or after the picture sequence. Detection (as d′) improved as duration increased but was significantly above chance in all conditions and for all durations. When the name was given before the sequence, d′ was higher for the basic than for the superordinate name, showing that specific advance information facilitated visual encoding. In the name-after group, performance on the two category levels did not differ significantly; this suggests that encoding had occurred at the basic level during presentation, allowing the superordinate category to be inferred. We interpret the results as being consistent with the claim that the basic level is usually the entry level for object perception., National Institutes of Health (U.S.) (Grant MH47432)
- Published
- 2014
123. Something from (almost) nothing: buildup of object memory from forgettable single fixations
- Author
-
Mary C. Potter, Ansgar D. Endress, Massachusetts Institute of Technology. Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, and Potter, Mary C
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Linguistics and Language ,Memory, Long-Term ,Visual perception ,BF ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Fixation, Ocular ,050105 experimental psychology ,Language and Linguistics ,Young Adult ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,Nothing ,Immediate memory ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Recognition memory ,Communication ,business.industry ,05 social sciences ,Recognition, Psychology ,Object (philosophy) ,Sensory Systems ,Memory, Short-Term ,Visual Perception ,business ,Psychology ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
We can recognize thousands of individual objects in scores of familiar settings, and yet we see most of them only through occasional glances that are quickly forgotten. How do we come to recognize any of these objects? Here, we show that when objects are presented intermittently for durations of single fixations, the originally fleeting memories become gradually stabilized, such that, after just eight separated fixations, recognition memory after half an hour is as good as during an immediate memory test. However, with still shorter presentation durations, memories take more exposures to stabilize. Our results thus suggest that repeated glances suffice to remember the objects of our environment., National Institutes of Health (U.S.) (Grant MH47432), Spain. Ministerio de Economía y Competitividad, Marie Curie Incoming Fellowship 303163-COMINTENT
- Published
- 2014
124. Detecting meaning in RSVP at 13 ms per picture
- Author
-
Mary C. Potter, Carl Erick Hagmann, Brad Wyble, Emily S. McCourt, Massachusetts Institute of Technology. Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Potter, Mary C, Hagmann, Carl, and McCourt, Emily Sarah
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Linguistics and Language ,Adolescent ,Concept Formation ,Speech recognition ,Forward pass ,Poison control ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Fixation, Ocular ,Stimulus (physiology) ,Language and Linguistics ,Young Adult ,Neural activity ,Humans ,Attention ,Analysis of Variance ,Communication ,business.industry ,Interstimulus interval ,Feed forward ,Sensory Systems ,Comprehension ,Rapid serial visual presentation ,Pattern Recognition, Visual ,Female ,business ,Psychology - Abstract
The visual system is exquisitely adapted to the task of extracting conceptual information from visual input with every new eye fixation, three or four times a second. Here we assess the minimum viewing time needed for visual comprehension, using rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) of a series of six or 12 pictures presented at between 13 and 80 ms per picture, with no interstimulus interval. Participants were to detect a picture specified by a name (e.g., smiling couple) that was given just before or immediately after the sequence. Detection improved with increasing duration and was better when the name was presented before the sequence, but performance was significantly above chance at all durations, whether the target was named before or only after the sequence. The results are consistent with feedforward models, in which an initial wave of neural activity through the ventral stream is sufficient to allow identification of a complex visual stimulus in a single forward pass. Although we discuss other explanations, the results suggest that neither reentrant processing from higher to lower levels nor advance information about the stimulus is necessary for the conscious detection of rapidly presented, complex visual information., National Institutes of Health (U.S.) (Grant No. MH47432)
- Published
- 2013
125. RSVP in orbit: Identification of single and dual targets in motion
- Author
-
Marcelo G. Mattar, Brad Wyble, Mary C. Potter, McGovern Institute for Brain Research at MIT, and Potter, Mary C
- Subjects
Linguistics and Language ,Communication ,business.industry ,Motion Perception ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Fixation, Ocular ,Serial Learning ,Attentional Blink ,Pursuit, Smooth ,Sensory Systems ,Language and Linguistics ,Motion (physics) ,Identification (information) ,Discrimination, Psychological ,Rapid serial visual presentation ,Pattern Recognition, Visual ,Fixation (visual) ,Reaction Time ,Humans ,Attention ,Attentional blink ,Computer vision ,Artificial intelligence ,Psychology ,business - Abstract
Three experiments using rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) tested participants' ability to detect targets in streams that are in motion. These experiments compared the ability to identify moving versus stationary RSVP targets and examined the attentional blink with pairs of targets that were moving or stationary. One condition presented RSVP streams in the center of the screen; a second condition used an RSVP that was orbiting in a circle, with participants instructed to follow the stream with their eyes; and a third condition had participants fixate in the middle while observing a circling RSVP stream. Relative to performance in stationary RSVP streams, participants were not markedly impaired in detecting single targets in RSVP streams that were moving, either with or without instructions to pursue the motion. In streams with two targets, a normal attentional blink effect was observed when participants were instructed to pursue the moving stream. When participants had to maintain central fixation as the RSVP stream moved, the attentional blink was nearly absent even when a trailing mask was added. We suggest that the reduction of the attentional blink for moving RSVP streams may reflect a reduced ability to perceive the temporal boundaries of the individual items.
- Published
- 2011
126. Attentional episodes in visual perception
- Author
-
Brad Wyble, Howard Bowman, Mary C. Potter, Mark Nieuwenstein, Massachusetts Institute of Technology. Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Potter, Mary C, and Experimental Psychology
- Subjects
Adult ,Visual perception ,Adolescent ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Short-term memory ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Models, Psychological ,attentional blink ,Article ,working memory ,episode ,WORKING-MEMORY ,Developmental Neuroscience ,RESOURCE DEPLETION ,TARGETS ,Perception ,Reaction Time ,Humans ,Contrast (vision) ,Computer Simulation ,Attentional blink ,Repetition blindness ,General Psychology ,media_common ,TEMPORAL ATTENTION ,BLINK ,SHORT-TERM-MEMORY ,Working memory ,Attentional control ,attention ,MODEL ,Memory, Short-Term ,Lag 1 sparing ,TIME-COURSE ,Visual Perception ,REPETITION BLINDNESS ,Psychology ,RSVP ,Photic Stimulation ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Is one's temporal perception of the world truly as seamless as it appears? This article presents a computationally motivated theory suggesting that visual attention samples information from temporal episodes (episodic simultaneous type/serial token model; Wyble, Bowman, & Nieuwenstein, 2009). Breaks between these episodes are punctuated by periods of suppressed attention, better known as the attentional blink (Raymond, Shapiro, & Arnell, 1992). We test predictions from this model and demonstrate that participants were able to report more letters from a sequence of 4 targets presented in a dense temporal cluster than from a sequence of 4 targets interleaved with nontargets. However, this superior report accuracy comes at a cost in impaired temporal order perception. Further experiments explore the dynamics of multiple episodes and the boundary conditions that trigger episodic breaks. Finally, we contrast the importance of attentional control, limited resources, and memory capacity constructs in the model., National Institutes of Health (U.S.) (MH47432)
- Published
- 2011
127. Working memory effects in speeded RSVP tasks
- Author
-
Mary C. Potter, Carmen Rodríguez, Beatriz Gil-Gómez de Liaño, Massachusetts Institute of Technology. Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Potter, Mary C, and UAM. Departamento de Psicología Social y Metodología
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Relation (database) ,Adolescent ,Computer science ,Speech recognition ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Memory load ,Task (project management) ,Young Adult ,Single task ,Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,Humans ,Attention ,Visual short-term memory ,Working memory ,General Medicine ,Psicología ,Identification (information) ,Rapid serial visual presentation ,Memory, Short-Term ,Short term memory ,Visual Perception ,Female ,Visual memory ,Psychomotor Performance ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
The author may only post his/her version provided acknowledgement is given to the original source of publication and a link is inserted to the published article on Springer's website. The link must be provided by inserting the DOI number of the article in the following sentence: “The final publication is available at Springer via http://dx.doi.org/[insert DOI]”, The present paper examines the effects of memory contents and memory load in rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) speeded tasks, trying to explain previous inconsistent results. We used a one target (Experiment 1) and a two-target (Experiment 2) RSVP task with a concurrent memory load of one or four items, in a dual-task paradigm. A relation between material in working memory and the target in the RSVP impaired the identification of the target. In Experiments 3 and 4, the single task was to determine whether any information in memory matched the target in the RSVP, while varying the memory load. A match was detected faster than a non-match, although only when there was some distance between targets in the RSVP (Experiment 4). The results suggest that memory contents automatically capture attention, slowing processing when the memory contents are irrelevant to the task, and speeding processing when they are relevant., Data collection was made possible thanks to the financed research project by the ‘‘Ministerio de Ciencia e Innovación. Plan Nacional I+D+i 2008–2011. Ref. PSI2010-20175’’. This work was also supported by MH47432.
- Published
- 2012
128. Categorically defined targets trigger spatiotemporal visual attention
- Author
-
Mary C. Potter, Brad Wyble, Howard Bowman, Massachusetts Institute of Technology. Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences, Potter, Mary C., and Wyble, Brad
- Subjects
Adult ,Time Factors ,Visual perception ,Adolescent ,Spatial ability ,Field Dependence-Independence ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Article ,Young Adult ,Behavioral Neuroscience ,Discrimination, Psychological ,Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,Reference Values ,Orientation ,Reaction Time ,Humans ,Attention ,Attentional blink ,Set (psychology) ,Analysis of Variance ,Eye movement ,Stimulus onset asynchrony ,Rapid serial visual presentation ,Space Perception ,Cues ,Probability Learning ,Psychology ,Priming (psychology) ,Photic Stimulation ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Transient attention to a visually salient cue enhances processing of a subsequent target in the same spatial location between 50 to 150 ms after cue onset (K. Nakayama & M. Mackeben, 1989). Do stimuli from a categorically defined target set, such as letters or digits, also generate transient attention? Participants reported digit targets among keyboard symbols in a changing array of 8 items. When 1 target preceded a second target in the same location at a stimulus onset asynchrony of 107 ms (but not 213 ms), the second target was reported more often than in a condition in which there was no leading target. When the 2 targets were at different locations, report of the second target was impaired. With both letters and digits as targets, the enhancement effect was shown not to be due to category priming. Critically, the attentional benefit was present whether or not participants reported the leading target. Transient attention, contingent attentional capture, popout, and Lag 1 sparing in the attentional blink may involve a common mechanism for orienting processing resources towards salient and task relevant stimuli., National Institutes of Health (U.S) (MH47432), Engineering and Physical Sciences Research Council (GR/ S15075/01)
- Published
- 2009
129. Letters to the Editor.
- Author
-
Becker, Mary J., Chase, Bud, Havard, Edna Lea, Walther, W., Potter, Mary C., Brown, Sybille, Asher, Grant, Wicox, Antoinette Le Donne, Thornley, Kerry, Innenbaum, Clara, Boscia, Rita, Catharine South-Cheney, Mayo, Mary, Raynolds, Robert, Lyons, Arthur, Mrs. Margaret O'Kane, Lindsay, Mrs. Fred, Bonnet, Capt. Francis A., Wilcox, Dan, and Cooper, Theron R.
- Subjects
- *
LETTERS to the editor , *INTERNATIONAL trade , *VOYAGES & travels , *RHINOCEROSES , *PUBLISHING - Abstract
Presents letters to the editor related to various articles published in the February 1, 1964 issue of the journal "The Saturday Evening Post." Reference to the article "Should the U.S. Trade With Enemies?"; Appreciation of the article "Passion for Rhinoceros"; Comment on the article "Last Voyage of the Lakonia."
- Published
- 1964
130. Enhanced recognition of memorable pictures in ultra-fast RSVP.
- Author
-
Broers N, Potter MC, and Nieuwenstein MR
- Subjects
- Adolescent, Adult, Female, Humans, Male, Young Adult, Memory, Long-Term physiology, Mental Recall physiology, Pattern Recognition, Visual physiology, Recognition, Psychology physiology
- Abstract
Long-term recognition memory for some pictures is consistently better than for others (Isola, Xiao, Parikh, Torralba, & Oliva, IEEE Transaction on Pattern Analysis and Machine Intelligence (PAMI), 36(7), 1469-1482, 2014). Here, we investigated whether pictures found to be memorable in a long-term memory test are also perceived more easily when presented in ultra-rapid RSVP. Participants viewed 6 pictures they had never seen before that were presented for 13 to 360 ms per picture in a rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) sequence. In half the trials, one of the pictures was a memorable or a nonmemorable picture and perception of this picture was probed by a visual recognition test at the end of the sequence. Recognition for pictures from the memorable set was higher than for those from the nonmemorable set, and this difference increased with increasing duration. Nonmemorable picture recognition was low initially, did not increase until 120 ms, and never caught up with memorable picture recognition performance. Thus, the long-term memorability of an image is associated with initial perceptibility: A picture that is hard to grasp quickly is hard to remember later.
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
131. Banana or fruit? Detection and recognition across categorical levels in RSVP.
- Author
-
Potter MC and Hagmann CE
- Subjects
- Adolescent, Adult, Attention, Comprehension, Female, Humans, Male, Middle Aged, Reaction Time, Serial Learning, Young Adult, Concept Formation, Discrimination, Psychological, Fruit, Musa, Pattern Recognition, Visual, Recognition, Psychology
- Abstract
Pictured objects and scenes can be understood in a brief glimpse, but there is a debate about whether they are first encoded at the basic level (e.g., banana), as proposed by Rosch et al. (1976, Cognitive Psychology) , or at a superordinate level (e.g., fruit). The level at which we first categorize an object matters in everyday situations because it determines whether we approach, avoid, or ignore the object. In the present study, we limited stimulus duration in order to explore the earliest level of object understanding. Target objects were presented among five other pictures using RSVP at 80, 53, 27, or 13 ms/picture. On each trial, participants viewed or heard 1 of 28 superordinate names or a corresponding basic-level name of the target. The name appeared before or after the picture sequence. Detection (as d') improved as duration increased but was significantly above chance in all conditions and for all durations. When the name was given before the sequence, d' was higher for the basic than for the superordinate name, showing that specific advance information facilitated visual encoding. In the name-after group, performance on the two category levels did not differ significantly; this suggests that encoding had occurred at the basic level during presentation, allowing the superordinate category to be inferred. We interpret the results as being consistent with the claim that the basic level is usually the entry level for object perception.
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
132. Something from (almost) nothing: buildup of object memory from forgettable single fixations.
- Author
-
Endress AD and Potter MC
- Subjects
- Adult, Humans, Male, Recognition, Psychology physiology, Young Adult, Fixation, Ocular physiology, Memory, Long-Term physiology, Memory, Short-Term physiology, Visual Perception physiology
- Abstract
We can recognize thousands of individual objects in scores of familiar settings, and yet we see most of them only through occasional glances that are quickly forgotten. How do we come to recognize any of these objects? Here, we show that when objects are presented intermittently for durations of single fixations, the originally fleeting memories become gradually stabilized, such that, after just eight separated fixations, recognition memory after half an hour is as good as during an immediate memory test. However, with still shorter presentation durations, memories take more exposures to stabilize. Our results thus suggest that repeated glances suffice to remember the objects of our environment.
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
133. Early conceptual and linguistic processes operate in independent channels.
- Author
-
Endress AD and Potter MC
- Subjects
- Female, Humans, Male, Pattern Recognition, Visual, Young Adult, Attention, Concept Formation, Language, Memory, Recognition, Psychology
- Abstract
Language and concepts are intimately linked, but how do they interact? In the study reported here, we probed the relation between conceptual and linguistic processing at the earliest processing stages. We presented observers with sequences of visual scenes lasting 200 or 250 ms per picture. Results showed that observers understood and remembered the scenes' abstract gist and, therefore, their conceptual meaning. However, observers remembered the scenes at least as well when they simultaneously performed a linguistic secondary task (i.e., reading and retaining sentences); in contrast, a nonlinguistic secondary task (equated for difficulty with the linguistic task) impaired scene recognition. Further, encoding scenes interfered with performance on the nonlinguistic task and vice versa, but scene processing and performing the linguistic task did not affect each other. At the earliest stages of conceptual processing, the extraction of meaning from visually presented linguistic stimuli and the extraction of conceptual information from the world take place in remarkably independent channels.
- Published
- 2012
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
134. Attention blinks for selection, not perception or memory: reading sentences and reporting targets.
- Author
-
Potter MC, Wyble B, and Olejarczyk J
- Subjects
- Attention, Humans, Task Performance and Analysis, Visual Perception, Attentional Blink, Mental Recall, Reading
- Abstract
In whole report, a sentence presented sequentially at the rate of about 10 words/s can be recalled accurately, whereas if the task is to report only two target words (e.g., red words), the second target suffers an attentional blink if it appears shortly after the first target. If these two tasks are carried out simultaneously, is there an attentional blink, and does it affect both tasks? Here, sentence report was combined with report of two target words (Experiments 1 and 2) or two inserted target digits, Arabic numerals or word digits (Experiments 3 and 4). When participants reported only the targets an attentional blink was always observed. When they reported both the sentence and targets, sentence report was quite accurate but there was an attentional blink in picking out the targets when they were part of the sentence. When targets were extra digits inserted in the sentence there was no blink when viewers also reported the sentence. These results challenge some theories of the attentional blink: Blinks result from online selection, not perception or memory.
- Published
- 2011
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
135. Attentional episodes in visual perception.
- Author
-
Wyble B, Potter MC, Bowman H, and Nieuwenstein M
- Subjects
- Adolescent, Adult, Computer Simulation, Humans, Memory, Short-Term physiology, Models, Psychological, Photic Stimulation, Reaction Time physiology, Visual Perception physiology, Attention physiology, Attentional Blink physiology
- Abstract
Is one's temporal perception of the world truly as seamless as it appears? This article presents a computationally motivated theory suggesting that visual attention samples information from temporal episodes (episodic simultaneous type/serial token model; Wyble, Bowman, & Nieuwenstein, 2009). Breaks between these episodes are punctuated by periods of suppressed attention, better known as the attentional blink (Raymond, Shapiro, & Arnell, 1992). We test predictions from this model and demonstrate that participants were able to report more letters from a sequence of 4 targets presented in a dense temporal cluster than from a sequence of 4 targets interleaved with nontargets. However, this superior report accuracy comes at a cost in impaired temporal order perception. Further experiments explore the dynamics of multiple episodes and the boundary conditions that trigger episodic breaks. Finally, we contrast the importance of attentional control, limited resources, and memory capacity constructs in the model.
- Published
- 2011
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
136. Picture detection in rapid serial visual presentation: features or identity?
- Author
-
Potter MC, Wyble B, Pandav R, and Olejarczyk J
- Subjects
- Female, Humans, Male, Orientation, Semantics, Association Learning, Attention, Color Perception, Discrimination Learning, Pattern Recognition, Visual, Reaction Time, Serial Learning
- Abstract
A pictured object can be readily detected in a rapid serial visual presentation sequence when the target is specified by a superordinate category name such as animal or vehicle. Are category features the initial basis for detection, with identification of the specific object occurring in a second stage (Evans & Treisman, 2005), or is identification of the object the basis for detection? When 2 targets in the same superordinate category are presented successively (lag 1), only the identification-first hypothesis predicts lag 1 sparing of the second target. The results of 2 experiments with novel pictures and a wide range of categories supported the identification-first hypothesis and a transient-attention model of lag 1 sparing and the attentional blink (Wyble, Bowman, & Potter, 2009).
- Published
- 2010
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
137. Unmasking the attentional blink.
- Author
-
Nieuwenstein MR, Potter MC, and Theeuwes J
- Subjects
- Attention, Cues, Humans, Memory, Short-Term, Attentional Blink, Perceptual Masking
- Abstract
When asked to identify 2 visual targets (T1 and T2 for the 1st and 2nd targets, respectively) embedded in a sequence of distractors, observers will often fail to identify T2 when it appears within 200-500 ms of T1--an effect called the attentional blink. Recent work shows that attention does not blink when the task is to encode a sequence of consecutive targets, suggesting that distractor interference plays a causal role in the attentional blink. Here, however, the authors show that an attentional blink occurs even in the absence of distractors, with 2 letter targets separated by a blank interval. In addition, the authors found that the impairment for identification of the 2nd of 2 targets separated by a blank interval is substantially attenuated either when the intertarget interval is filled with additional target items or when the 2nd target is precued by an additional target. These findings show that the root cause of the blink lies in the difficulty of engaging attention twice within a short period of time for 2 temporally discrete target events., (Copyright 2009 APA, all rights reserved.)
- Published
- 2009
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
138. Detecting and remembering simultaneous pictures in a rapid serial visual presentation.
- Author
-
Potter MC and Fox LF
- Subjects
- Attention, Female, Humans, Male, Mental Recall, Signal Detection, Psychological, Visual Perception
- Abstract
Viewers can easily spot a target picture in a rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP), but can they do so if more than 1 picture is presented simultaneously? Up to 4 pictures were presented on each RSVP frame, for 240 to 720 ms/frame. In a detection task, the target was verbally specified before each trial (e.g., man with violin); in a memory task, recognition was tested after each sequence. Target detection was much better than recognition memory, but in both tasks the more pictures on the frame, the lower the performance. When the presentation duration was set at 160 ms with a variable interframe interval such that the total times were the same as in the initial experiments, the results were similar. The results suggest that visual processing occurs in 2 stages: fast, global processing of all pictures in Stage 1 (usually sufficient for detection) and slower, serial processing in Stage 2 (usually necessary for subsequent memory)., (Copyright 2009 APA, all rights reserved.)
- Published
- 2009
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
139. Detecting and remembering pictures with and without visual noise.
- Author
-
Meng M and Potter MC
- Subjects
- Color Perception physiology, Humans, Photic Stimulation, Artifacts, Pattern Recognition, Visual physiology, Recognition, Psychology physiology
- Abstract
Objects in a scene are often partially occluded without causing the viewer any problem: the occluded parts are apparently represented via amodal completion. To evaluate human ability to perceive and remember partially occluded pictures, we showed sequences of pictures using rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) for durations of 53 ms, 107 ms, 213 ms, or 426 ms/picture. Participants either attempted to detect a named target (e.g., "businessmen at table") or were given a yes-no recognition memory test of one item. In Experiment 1, with as much as 30% of the picture area covered, detection and recognition were both well above chance. More interestingly, occlusion significantly affected recognition memory but not target detection. In Experiment 2, when pictures were inverted, occlusion impaired detection as severely as recognition. For target detection, the interaction between occlusion and inversion was significant. By contrast, taking away color information did not significantly reduce detection's tolerance of occlusion (Experiment 3). Finally, Experiment 4 showed that with 40% of the picture area occluded, detection performance was impaired. These results support the hypothesis that contextual gist information facilitates visual processes that tolerate occluding noise. Although inversion and color were tested in particular, the presented paradigm can also be used to investigate the role of other factors in gist representation.
- Published
- 2008
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
140. Whole Report versus Partial Report in RSVP Sentences.
- Author
-
Potter MC, Nieuwenstein M, and Strohminger N
- Abstract
A sentence is readily understood and recalled when presented 1 word at a time using rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) at 10 words/s (Potter, 1984). In contrast, selecting just 2 colored letters at 10 letters/s results in easy detection of the first target but poor recall for the second when it appears 200-500 ms later. This attentional blink disappears when all letters must be reported; instead, performance drops more gradually over serial position (Nieuwenstein & Potter, 2006). Would target words in sentences escape an attentional blink? Subjects either reported 2 target words (in red or uppercase) or the whole 10-word sentence. There was a blink for Target 2 in partial report, but that target was easily remembered in whole report. With scrambled sentences whole report dropped but partial report was unaffected, again showing a blink. The attentional blink is not due to memory processing of Target 1, but to target selection, which is incompatible with sentence processing.
- Published
- 2008
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
141. Temporal limits of selection and memory encoding: A comparison of whole versus partial report in rapid serial visual presentation.
- Author
-
Nieuwenstein MR and Potter MC
- Subjects
- Adult, Attention, Cues, Female, Humans, Male, Choice Behavior, Memory physiology, Reaction Time, Visual Perception
- Abstract
People often fail to recall the second of two visual targets presented within 500 ms in rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP). This effect is called the attentional blink. One explanation of the attentional blink is that processes involved in encoding the first target into memory are slow and capacity limited. Here, however, we show that the attentional blink should be ascribed to attentional selection, not consolidation of the first target. Rapid sequences of six letters were presented, and observers had to report either all the letters (whole-report condition) or a subset of the letters (partial-report condition). Selection in partial report was based on color (e.g., report the two red letters) or identity (i.e., report all letters from a particular letter onward). In both cases, recall of letters presented shortly after the first selected letter was impaired, whereas recall of the corresponding letters was relatively accurate with whole report.
- Published
- 2006
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
142. Pictorial and conceptual representation of glimpsed pictures.
- Author
-
Potter MC, Staub A, O'Connor DH, and Potter MC
- Subjects
- Humans, Light, Memory, Reaction Time, Recognition, Psychology, Attention, Motion Perception, Semantics, Visual Perception
- Abstract
Pictures seen in a rapid sequence are remembered briefly, but most are forgotten within a few seconds (M. C. Potter. A. Staub, J. Rado. & D. H. O'Connor. 2002). The authors investigated the pictorial and conceptual components of this fleeting memory by presenting 5 pictured scenes and immediately testing recognition of verbal titles (e.g., people at a table) or recognition of the pictures themselves. Recognition declined during testing, but initial performance was higher and the decline steeper when pictures were tested. A final experiment included test decoy pictures that were conceptually similar to but visually distinct from the original pictures. Yeses to decoys were higher than yeses to other distractors. Fleeting memory for glimpsed pictures has a strong conceptual component (conceptual short-term memory), but there is additional highly volatile pictorial memory (pictorial short-term memory) that is not tapped hy a gist title or decoy picture., (((c) 2004 APA, all rights reserved))
- Published
- 2004
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.