768 results on '"Raab, Markus"'
Search Results
52. Where do embodied choices come from?
- Author
-
Raab, Markus, primary
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
53. Ten statements for simplifying your life with embodied choices
- Author
-
Raab, Markus, primary
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
54. Simple heuristics—How we make decisions
- Author
-
Raab, Markus, primary
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
55. How to cope with uncertainty in COVID-19 times
- Author
-
Raab, Markus, primary
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
56. Decisions when moving your mind
- Author
-
Raab, Markus, primary
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
57. Preface to the special issue: 50 years of FEPSAC
- Author
-
Elbe, Anne-Marie, Bertollo, Maurizio, Debois, Nadine, de Oliveira, Rita F., Fritsch, Julian, Hatzigeorgiadis, Antonis, Moesch, Karin, Raab, Markus, Sanchez, Xavier, and Vaisetaite, Lina
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
58. The past, present and future of research on judgment and decision making in sport
- Author
-
Raab, Markus, Bar-Eli, Michael, Plessner, Henning, and Araújo, Duarte
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
59. Post-traumatic stress disorder among elite athletes affected by war.
- Author
-
KOROBEINIKOVA, IVANNA, RAAB, MARKUS, LABORDE, SYLVAIN, KOKUN, OLED, KOROBEINIKOVA, LESIA, KOROBEYNIKOV, GEORGIY, ROMANCHUK, SERHII, CYNARSKI, WOJCIECH J., SHTANAGEY, DMYTRIY, and YEHORENKOV, ANATOLII
- Abstract
Introduction. Current research on the Russian-Ukrainian conflict explores various aspects of psychological distr ess among affected individuals. However, there is a lack of information regarding post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) among Ukrainian athletes affected by the conflict. Objective: In this study we aimed to investigate the prevalence of PTSD among Ukrainian athletes affected by the ongoing war. Materials and methods: A total of 163 Ukrainians with varying degrees of exposure to the war participated in the study. Among them, 109 (67%) were non-athletes, while 54 (33%) were athletes. Non-athletes were individuals who remained in Ukraine after the onset of the conflict. Athletes with many having traveled for competitions and subsequently returned to Ukraine. This study used the primary care PTSD screen for DSM-5 (PC-PTSD-5). According to this method, the main scales are: anxiety, depression and post-traumatic stress disorder. Additional stress disorder scales were used as a supplement to the main questionnaire. Results The results show the prevalence of PTSD in the general sample compared to the general population. 54.4% persons reported PTSD. Non-athletes felt more in danger and threatened than athletes. We hypothesize that stress resistance in athletes supports the ability to reduce the effects of post-traumatic stress. The results of clinical symptoms was obtained that athletes avoid negative memories associated with military situations compared to non-athletes. This demonstrates that athletes are very busy with the training process. Non-athletes lack this tendency. In conclusion, we argue that the consequences of PTSD in athletes are lower than in non-athletes, and this may be an opportunity to prevent PTSD through sport. Conclusions The obtained studies show a connection between the war situation and the development of posttraumatic stress syndrome in people. A higher level of stress resistance in athletes has been established. This manifests itself in resistance to traumatic stress. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
60. Technology Meets Sport Psychology: How Technology and Artificial Intelligence Can Shape the Future of Elite Sport Performance.
- Author
-
Raab, Markus, Schinke, Robert, and Maher, Charles A.
- Subjects
SPORTS & technology ,ARTIFICIAL intelligence ,SPORTS psychology ,PROFESSIONAL sports ,SPORTS ,BIG data - Abstract
Data analytics have become increasingly popular within professional sport organizations. Big data exists upon which decisions are made in relation to athlete selection and performance analysis. The data draw upon vary from big data and more idiosyncratic data that is contextually focused. We consider the importance of data technology in relation to the prediction of athlete and team performance within high-performance sport. Here we expand upon existing approaches to analytics, and why the prediction of athletes' behaviors is often found to be ineffective. Recommendations are provided how sport psychology can offer insights for analytics departments. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
61. Embodied planning in climbing: how pre-planning informs motor execution.
- Author
-
Campo, Vicente Luis-del, Martín, Jesús Morenas, Musculus, Lisa, and Raab, Markus
- Subjects
VISUAL perception ,STAIR climbing ,CLIMBING gyms ,GAZE - Abstract
Introduction: The aim of the study is to address embodied planning in climbing. Embodied planning was conceptualized as the interaction between perceptualcognitive pre-planning and motor execution. Methods: In an experimental study, 18 climbers were asked to pre-plan a climbing route and to perform the route afterward. During pre-planning, the visual search pattern of climbers was captured using a portable eye tracker. After previewing, they were invited to climb the wall. Results: Results revealed that holds looked at during pre-planning were used twice as much during route execution than those not looked at. The duration of fixations was longer for holds used than those not used during route execution. The experience of climbers (training years) correlated with visual strategies and climbing performance, such that experienced participants climbed faster and fixated at the holds not used for a shorter time. Discussion: The visual behaviors of climbers were influenced by their past sensorimotor experiences during route previewing, impacting subsequent climbing performance. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
62. Psychophysiological states of elite athletes after critical life events.
- Author
-
Korobeinikova, Ivanna, Kokun, Oleg, Raab, Markus, Korobeinikova, Lesia, Korobeynikov, Georgiy, Kostiuchenko, Vasil, Aksutin, Viktor, and Dekha, Nataliia
- Subjects
ELITE athletes ,POLITICAL elites ,RUSSIAN armed forces ,INFORMATION overload ,PSYCHOLOGICAL stress ,TEST anxiety - Abstract
Background and Study Aim Military conflicts are a stress factor that provokes negative changes in the mental state of people. But, how susceptible are athletes to post-traumatic disorders at the level of psychophysiological functions? The purpose is to study the psychophysiological state of the elite athletes after critical life events related to the war in Ukraine. Material and Methods Twelve elite female fencers, aged 18-23 years and members of the National Team of Ukraine, were examined. Their mental condition was assessed using the Lüscher color test, neurodynamic functions were evaluated with an anticipation test, cognitive characteristics through a decisionmaking test, and levels of anxiety were also measured. The psychophysiological state of the fencers was studied both before and after six months of the Russian military aggression against Ukraine. Results The results indicated changes in the mental state of fencers under military conditions. Six months after the onset of the war, elite fencers exhibited signs of mental stress and a decreased resistance to stress. According to the obtained results, during the war, athletes showed a predominance of excitation processes over inhibition processes. The study revealed a deterioration in performance on decision-making tests and a decreased ability to perceive visual information in fencers six months into the war. As a compensatory mechanism for the reduced ability to process visual stimuli under conditions of information overload, a decrease in impulsivity among fencers was observed. Research on anxiety demonstrated that, over the six months of war compared to peacetime, trait anxiety among athletes significantly increased. Conclusions Significant impacts of military actions on the psychophysiological state of elite athletes have been observed. This deterioration in mental state, along with decreases in neurodynamic and cognitive functions and an increase in personal anxiety levels, calls for the development and implementation of specialized psychological support programs. Such programs should aim to bolster athletes’ resilience to stressful situations. The importance of accounting for psychophysiological aspects in the training of athletes within conflict zones is underscored, providing a basis for further research in this field. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
63. The dynamic role of the left dlPFC in Neurovisceral Integration: Differential effects of Theta Burst Stimulation on vagally-mediated Heart Rate Variability and cognitive-affective processing
- Author
-
Schmaußer, Maximilian, primary, Raab, Markus, additional, and Laborde, Sylvain, additional
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
64. Judgement and decision-making
- Author
-
Raab, Markus, primary and MacMahon, Clare, additional
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
65. Challenges in Validating FLOSS Configuration
- Author
-
Raab, Markus, Barany, Gergö, Rannenberg, Kai, Editor-in-chief, Sakarovitch, Jacques, Series editor, Goedicke, Michael, Series editor, Tatnall, Arthur, Series editor, Neuhold, Erich J., Series editor, Pras, Aiko, Series editor, Tröltzsch, Fredi, Series editor, Pries-Heje, Jan, Series editor, Whitehouse, Diane, Series editor, Reis, Ricardo, Series editor, Furnell, Steven, Series editor, Furbach, Ulrich, Series editor, Winckler, Marco, Series editor, Rauterberg, Matthias, Series editor, Balaguer, Federico, editor, Di Cosmo, Roberto, editor, Garrido, Alejandra, editor, Kon, Fabio, editor, Robles, Gregorio, editor, and Zacchiroli, Stefano, editor
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
66. Linking self-efficacy and decision-making processes in developing soccer players
- Author
-
Musculus, Lisa, Raab, Markus, Belling, Patrick, and Lobinger, Babett
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
67. Rapid Audio-Visual Integration Guides Predictive Actions
- Author
-
Kreyenmeier, Philipp, primary, Schroeger, Anna, additional, Cañal-Bruland, Rouwen, additional, Raab, Markus, additional, and Spering, Miriam, additional
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
68. Unanticipated Context Awareness for Software Configuration Access Using the getenv API
- Author
-
Raab, Markus, Kacprzyk, Janusz, Series editor, and Lee, Roger, editor
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
69. Motor heuristics and embodied choices: how to choose and act
- Author
-
Raab, Markus
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
70. Comparative analysis of psychophysiological state among in physical active and sedentary persons.
- Author
-
KOROBEINIKOVA, LESIA, RAAB, MARKUS, KOROBEYNIKOV, GEORGIY, PRYIMAKOV, OLEKSANDR, KERIMOV, FIKRAT, CHERNOZUB, ANDRIY, KOROBEINIKOVA, IVANNA, and GONCHAROVA, OLGA
- Abstract
Introduction The use of new technologies in human life leads to an improvement in lifestyle. However, many factors of modern life (environmental, social and others) can provoke a decrease in physical and mental performance, which, as a result, worsens health. One of main factor causing deterioration in health is physical inactivity. Physical activity, in turn, can help improve health and mental well-being. The study of the psychophysiological state of persons with different level of physical active may provide new information about the mechanism of physical inactivity and be of use to improve health. The purpose: We study psychophysiological state in physically active and sedentary persons. We predict that physically active participants buffer environmental stressors better and thus have higher values on their psychophysiological states than sedentary persons. Material and methods 19 physically active men (22-35 years) who practiced sports no more than three times a week were examined. Men with low physical activity and a sedentary lifestyle (20 persons, 26-36 years) were also examined. All of persons are non-clinical subjects. The mental state was studied using an adaptive version of the Luscher color test. Neurodynamic tests included a test of a person's ability to make decisions and anticipate them. Among the cognitive tests, the "Number Comparison" test for perception speed was used. All tests used in the study were part of the Multipsychometer-05 hardware and software complex. Results It was revealed that the lack of physical activity leads to increased tension of the autonomic regulation system and limitation of the body's functional reserves. The relationship of physical inactivity with increased anxiety and tension of the autonomic nervous system was revealed. Thus, the increase in effectiveness and speed of perception in physically active persons indicates the stimulating effect of the motor activity factor. In addition, it was revealed that physically active people have better accuracy and quality of non-verbal perception and information processing. Conclusions Physical activity affects the mental state, cognitive functions and anxiety levels. This is promoted by an increase in the performance of perception and the speed of processing non-verbal information. One of the main useful results of this study is the possibility of the influence of physical activity on improving the mental and cognitive properties of an individual. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
71. Embodied Cognition
- Author
-
Löffler, Jonna, primary, Cañal-Bruland, Rouwen, additional, and Raab, Markus, additional
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
72. Mind and Motion The Model Movid (Movements Influence Decisions)
- Author
-
Green, Nikos, primary and Raab, Markus, additional
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
73. Heuristics, biases, and decision making
- Author
-
Raab, Markus, primary, MacMahon, Clare, additional, Avugos, Simcha, additional, and Bar-Eli, Michael, additional
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
74. List of Contributors
- Author
-
Abrams, Mitch, primary, Allan, Veronica, additional, Altfeld, Sebastian, additional, Amorose, Anthony J., additional, Araújo, Duarte, additional, Atkinson, Michael, additional, Baird, Shannon M., additional, Baker, Joseph, additional, Balish, Shea, additional, Bardid, Farid, additional, Bar-Eli, Michael, additional, Barker, Jamie, additional, Barkoukis, Vassilis, additional, Barnett, Lisa, additional, Battochio, Randy C., additional, Baumeister, Roy F., additional, Beauchamp, Mark R., additional, Beckmann, Jürgen, additional, Behm, David G., additional, Bellomo, Eduardo, additional, Benson, Alex, additional, Berger, Bonnie G., additional, Bernache-Assollant, Iouri, additional, Bertollo, Maurizio, additional, Bhavsar, Nikita, additional, Biddell, Hannah L., additional, Biddle, Stuart, additional, Blodgett, Amy T., additional, Bloom, Gordon A., additional, Blumenstein, Boris, additional, Bolter, Nicole D., additional, Boudreau, Patrick, additional, Bredemeier, Brenda Light, additional, Brown, Chris, additional, Brueckner, Sebastian, additional, Bruner, Mark W., additional, Burke, Shauna M., additional, Busanich, Rebecca, additional, Busch, Lena, additional, Butcher, Lois A., additional, Carless, David, additional, Caron, Jeffrey G., additional, Carter, Leeja, additional, Castillo, Sarah L., additional, Chang, Yu-Kai, additional, Charboneau, Nicole D., additional, Chatzisarantis, Nikos L.D., additional, Cheng, Ming-Yang, additional, Chermette, Chloé, additional, Chow, Graig M., additional, Chu, I-Hua, additional, Chu, Theo, additional, Coakley, Jay, additional, Coholic, Diana, additional, Compton, Bernadette, additional, Corbetta, Daniela, additional, Côté, Jean, additional, Cotterill, Stewart, additional, Cropley, Brendan, additional, Cruz, Jaume, additional, Darroch, Francine, additional, Davids, Keith, additional, Deal, Colin J., additional, Debanne, Thierry, additional, Den Hartigh, Ruud J.R., additional, DesClouds, Poppy, additional, di Fronso, Selenia, additional, Douglas, Kitrina, additional, Downs, Danielle S., additional, Dreiskämper, Dennis, additional, Durand-Bush, Natalie, additional, Dzikus, Lars, additional, Eccles, David W., additional, Effenberg, Alfred O., additional, Elbe, Anne-Marie, additional, Emrich, Eike, additional, Etnier, Jennifer, additional, Etzel, Edward, additional, Evans, M. Blair, additional, Eys, Mark, additional, Fasting, Kari, additional, Feistenauer, Clemens, additional, Feltz, Deborah L., additional, Ferguson, Leah J., additional, Filho, Edson, additional, Fisher, Leslee A., additional, Fleming, Audrey, additional, Fletcher, David, additional, Frank, Cornelia, additional, Fransen, Katrien, additional, Freeman, Paul, additional, Friesen, Andrew, additional, Furley, Philip, additional, Galloway, Shaun M., additional, Gassmann, Freya, additional, Gaudreau, Patrick, additional, Ge, Yang, additional, Gernigon, Christophe, additional, Gershgoren, Lael, additional, Geukes, Katharina, additional, Gilbert, Wade D., additional, Gill, Diane L., additional, Glenn, Nicola, additional, Godfrey, Michael, additional, Gonsalves, Christine A., additional, Gould, Daniel, additional, Granacher, Urs, additional, Graupensperger, Scott, additional, Greenlees, Iain, additional, Grey, Robert, additional, Gucciardi, Daniel F., additional, Guerrero, Michelle D., additional, Güldenpenning, Iris, additional, H. Kerr, John, additional, Hackfort, Dieter, additional, Hagan, John E., additional, Hagemann, Norbert, additional, Halson, Shona L., additional, Hancock, David J., additional, Hanton, Sheldon, additional, Hardy, James, additional, Hardy, Will, additional, Harwood, Chris G., additional, Hausenblas, Heather A., additional, Heidari, Jahan, additional, Herbison, Jordan D., additional, Heuer, Andreas, additional, Hilliard, Bobby, additional, Hodge, Ken, additional, Hodges, Nicola J., additional, Holmes, Paul, additional, Holt, Nicholas L., additional, Horn, Thelma S., additional, Hristovski, Robert, additional, Hudson, Joanne, additional, Hüffmeier, Joachim, additional, Hung, Tsung-Min, additional, James, Jeffrey D., additional, Johnson, Carra, additional, Jones, Martin I., additional, Jowett, Gareth E., additional, Jowett, Sophia, additional, Justen, Christoph, additional, Kao, San-Fu, additional, Karau, Steven J., additional, Kavussanu, Maria, additional, Keegan, Richard, additional, Kellmann, Michael, additional, Kerr, Gretchen, additional, Killham, Margo E., additional, Kim, Jeemin, additional, Kim, Youngho, additional, Klämpfl, Martin, additional, Kleinert, Jens, additional, Kljajic, Kristina, additional, Klöppel, York-Peter, additional, Knowles, Zoe, additional, Koester, Dirk, additional, Kölling, Sarah, additional, Kornspan, Alan S., additional, Krane, Vikki, additional, Krause, Daniel, additional, Kunz, Philipp, additional, Laborde, Sylvain, additional, Lane, Andrew M., additional, Lariviere, Michel, additional, Larsen, Carsten H., additional, Lastella, Michele, additional, LaVoi, Nicole M., additional, Law, Barbi, additional, Lazuras, Lambros, additional, Leslie-Toogood, Adrienne, additional, Lewthwaite, Rebecca, additional, Lidor, Ronnie, additional, Lindeman, Kent, additional, Lippke, Sonia, additional, Lobinger, Babett, additional, Loffing, Florian, additional, Loughead, Todd M., additional, Lucidi, Fabio, additional, Mack, Diane E., additional, MacMahon, Clare, additional, Malhotra, Neha, additional, Marks, Donald R., additional, Marques, Marta M., additional, Martin, Luc J., additional, Masters, Rich, additional, McCullagh, Penny, additional, McGannon, Kerry R., additional, Meier, Henk E., additional, Mellalieu, Stephen D., additional, Memmert, Daniel, additional, Mentzel, Stijn V., additional, Mesagno, Christopher, additional, Michie, Susan, additional, Middleton, Thierry R.F., additional, Mokhtari, Pooneh, additional, Moore, Lee, additional, Moore, Whitney, additional, Moran, Aidan, additional, Morres, Ioannis D., additional, Mosley, Emma, additional, Moss, Tayo, additional, Munroe-Chandler, Krista J., additional, Munzert, Jörn, additional, Murgia, Mauro, additional, Murphy, Shane, additional, Neil, Rich, additional, Nicholls, Adam R., additional, Nitsch, Jürgen R., additional, Noce, Franco, additional, Norman, Paul, additional, Ntoumanis, Nikos, additional, Oddson, Bruce, additional, Oghene, Patrick Odirin, additional, Oglesby, Carole, additional, Oluyedun, Olufemi A., additional, Orbach, Iris, additional, Oudejans, Raôul R.D., additional, Papathomas, Anthony, additional, Paradis, Kyle F., additional, Parham, William D., additional, Pelka, Maximilian, additional, Pels, Fabian, additional, Peters, Heather J., additional, Petersen, Brennan, additional, Petitpas, Albert, additional, Petty, Lisa, additional, Phoenix, Cassandra, additional, Pierce, Scott, additional, Plessner, Henning, additional, Poczwardowski, Artur, additional, Podlog, Leslie, additional, Pöppel, Katharina, additional, Prochaska, James O., additional, Prochaska, Janice M., additional, Quartiroli, Alessandro, additional, Raab, Markus, additional, Rhind, Daniel J.A., additional, Rhodes, Ryan E., additional, Richard, Veronique, additional, Robazza, Claudio, additional, Robbins, Jamie E., additional, Roberts, Ross, additional, Ruiz, Montse C., additional, Sabiston, Catherine M., additional, Sachs, Michael L., additional, Sackett, Sarah C., additional, Samuel, Roy David, additional, Sarkar, Mustafa, additional, Scanlan, Tara, additional, Scanlan, Tara K., additional, Schack, Thomas, additional, Schinke, Robert J., additional, Schlattmann, Andreas, additional, Schleu, Joyce E., additional, Schöllhorn, Wolfgang I., additional, Schorer, Jörg, additional, Schott, Nadja, additional, Schwarzer, Ralf, additional, Seanor, Michelle, additional, Seiler, Roland, additional, Seiler, Stephen, additional, Selby, Christine L.B., additional, Shephard, Roy J., additional, Shields, David, additional, Si, Gangyan, additional, Smith, Alan L., additional, Smith, Brett, additional, Smith, Kristina, additional, Smith, Ronald E., additional, Sonn, Christopher C., additional, Sperlich, Billy, additional, Stambulova, Natalia B., additional, Standage, Martyn, additional, Steggemann-Weinrich, Yvonne, additional, Stirling, Ashley, additional, Storm, Vera, additional, Strauß, Bernd, additional, Surya, Mark, additional, Swann, Christian, additional, Tamminen, Katherine A., additional, Taylor, Ian M., additional, Tenenbaum, Gershon, additional, Terry, Peter C., additional, Thelwell, Richard, additional, Theodorakis, Yannis, additional, Thogersen-Ntoumani, Cecilie, additional, Thrower, Sam N., additional, Tietjens, Maike, additional, Tirp, Judith, additional, Toner, John, additional, Toogood, Adrienne L., additional, Turnnidge, Jennifer, additional, Uiga, Liis, additional, Utesch, Till, additional, Vallerand, Robert J., additional, van der Kamp, John, additional, van Duijn, Tina, additional, Van Slingerland, Krista, additional, Velentzas, Konstantinos, additional, Verner-Filion, Jérémie, additional, Vine, Samuel J., additional, Waldron, Jennifer J., additional, Wann, Daniel L., additional, Watson, Jack C., additional, Wattie, Nick, additional, Way, William, additional, Weigelt, Matthias, additional, Weinberg, Robert, additional, Westerberg, Anders I., additional, Whitehead, Amy, additional, Wiese-Bjornstal, Diane M., additional, Williams, Kipling D., additional, Williams, Toni L., additional, Wilson, Mark, additional, Wolanin, Andrew T., additional, Woodman, Tim, additional, Wright, Emily, additional, Wu, Carolyn, additional, Wulf, Gabriele, additional, Wunsch, Kathrin, additional, Yarrow, Kielan, additional, Zakrajsek, Rebecca A., additional, and Zhang, Chun-Qing, additional
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
75. Життєстійкість у професії спортивних тренерів та вчителів фізичної культури
- Author
-
Korobeinikova, Ivanna, Korobeynikov, Georgiy, Kokun, Oleg, Raab, Markus, Korobeinikova, Lesia, and Syvash, Iryna
- Subjects
psychological properties ,hardiness ,професійна діяльність ,professional activity ,sports coaches ,загартованість ,психологічні властивості ,physical education teachers ,вчителі фізкультури ,спортивні тренери - Abstract
Background and Study Aim. To study psychological features of sports coaches and physical education teachers’ professional hardiness. Material and methods. The study involved 60 participants. Among them, 24 were coaches of children's and youth sports schools, 19 coaches of national teams of Ukraine and 17 physical education teachers. English-language version of the Professional Hardiness Questionnaire (PHQ) were used. The development of the English-language version of the PHQ was based on its initial Ukrainian-language version (http://prof-diagnost.org). The accuracy of its translation into English was checked and corrected by 12 bilingual specialists with degrees in medicine and/or psychology. To identify the peculiarities of the relationship between the indicators of professional resilience in different professional groups, we conducted a Spearman correlation analysis. Results. The analysis of professional hardiness revealed the presence of significant differences between the surveyed groups of coaches and physical education teachers by various indicators. It turned out that the length of service in the respective positions of national team coaches is significantly higher than that of Youth Sports School (YSS) coaches and physical education teachers. This indicates the presence of greater professional experience among this category of coaches. Accordingly, physical education teachers and coaches of national teams have significantly higher professional experience than coaches of YSS. As a result, the majority of young specialists are among the coaching staff of the YSS. The correlation coefficient between the general level of professional hardiness and the namely professional component of 0.80 is much higher for coaches of national teams. Among physical education teachers it reaches 0.61, most likely this is because the control over the work of this group is less specific in contrast to the work of the coach. Coaches of YSS have the lowest level of the namely professional component 0.50, we explain this by the fact that the overall level of experience in the position is half as much as in the previous two groups. Conclusions. The conducted analysis made it possible to establish relationships between the general level of professional hardiness and indicators of professional hardiness in each group of coaches and teachers., Передумови та мета дослідження. Мета: дослідити психологічні особливості професійної життєстійкості спортивних тренерів та вчителів фізичної культури. Матеріал і методи. В дослідженні взяли участь 60 учасників. Серед яких 24, це тренери ДЮСШ, 19 тренери національних збірних команд України та 17 вчителів фізичної культури. При психофізіологічному тестуванні ми використовували англомовну версію опитувальника професійної життєстійкості (PHQ). Розробка англомовної версії PHQ базувалася на її початковій україномовній версії (http://prof-diagnost.org). Точність перекладу англійською мовою перевіряли та виправляли 12 двомовних фахівців із медичними та/або психологічними ступенями. Для виявлення особливостей зв’язків між показниками професійної життєстійкості в різних професійних групах нами було проведено кореляційний аналіз за Спірменом. Результати. Аналіз професійної життєстійкості виявив наявність достовірних відмінностей між обстеженими групами тренерів та вчителів фізичної культури за різними показниками. Виявилося, що стаж роботи на відповідних посадах тренерів національних збірних команд значно вищий, ніж у тренерів і та вчителів фізичної культури. Це свідчить про наявність більшого професійного досвіду у цієї категорії тренерів. Відповідно, викладачі та тренери збірних мають значно вищий професійний досвід, ніж тренери ДЮСШ. В результаті чого більшість молодих спеціалістів серед тренерського складу ДЮСШ. Коефіцієнт кореляції між загальним рівнем професійної загартованості та саме професійною складовою 0, 80 значно вищий у тренерів національних збірних команд. У вчителів фізичної культури він досягає 0, 61, швидше за все це тому, що контроль за роботою цієї групи менш специфічний на відміну від роботи тренера. Тренери ДЮСШ мають найнижчий рівень саме професійної складової 0, 50, пояснюємо це тим, що загальний рівень досвіду на посаді вдвічі менший, ніж у попередніх двох групах. Висновки. Проведений аналіз дав змогу встановити зв’язки між загальним рівнем професійної життєстійкості та показниками професійної життєстійкості в кожній групі досліджуваних.
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
76. What is a task? An ideomotor perspective
- Author
-
Künzell, Stefan, Broeker, Laura, Dignath, David, Ewolds, Harald, Raab, Markus, and Thomaschke, Roland
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
77. Multitasking as a choice: a perspective
- Author
-
Broeker, Laura, Liepelt, Roman, Poljac, Edita, Künzell, Stefan, Ewolds, Harald, de Oliveira, Rita F., and Raab, Markus
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
78. A dissociable functional relevance of theta- and beta-band activities during complex sensorimotor integration
- Author
-
Böttcher, Adriana, primary, Wilken, Saskia, additional, Adelhöfer, Nico, additional, Raab, Markus, additional, Hoffmann, Sven, additional, and Beste, Christian, additional
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
79. Individual differences in athletes' perception of expressive body movements
- Author
-
Sevdalis, Vassilis and Raab, Markus
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
80. Does Framing the Hot Hand Belief Change Decision-Making Behavior in Volleyball?
- Author
-
Raab, Markus and MacMahon, Clare
- Abstract
Purpose: Previous discussions of the hot hand belief, wherein athletes believe that they have a greater chance of scoring after 2 or 3 hits (successes) compared with 2 or 3 misses, have focused on whether this is the case within game statistics. Researchers have argued that the perception of the hot hand in random sequences is a bias of the cognitive system. Yet most have failed to explore the impact of framing on the stability of the belief and the behavior based on it. Method: The authors conducted 2 studies that manipulated the frame of a judgment task. In Study 1, framing was manipulated via instructions in a playmaker allocation paradigm in volleyball. In Study 2, the frame was manipulated by presenting videos for allocation decisions from either the actor or observer perspective. Results: Both manipulations changed the hot hand belief and sequential choices. We found in both studies that the belief in continuation of positive or negative streaks is nonlinear and allocations to the same player after 3 successive hits are reduced. Conclusions: The authors argue that neither the hot hand belief nor hot hand behavior is stable, but rather, both are sensitive to decision frames. The results can inform coaches on the importance of how to provide information to athletes.
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
81. Apollo’s Curse: Causes and Cures of Motor Failures in Musicians: A Proposal for a New Classification
- Author
-
Altenmüller, Eckart, Ioannou, Christos I., Raab, Markus, Lobinger, Babett, Cohen, Irun R., Series editor, Lajtha, N.S. Abel, Series editor, Paoletti, Rodolfo, Series editor, Lambris, John D., Series editor, and Levin, Mindy F., editor
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
82. Entscheiden und Problemlösen
- Author
-
Musculus, Lisa, primary, Werner, Karsten, additional, Lobinger, Babett, additional, and Raab, Markus, additional
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
83. Embodied Cognition: Sprache, Metaphern und Instruktionen im Sport
- Author
-
Loeffler, Jonna, primary, Haunhorst, Hannah, additional, Cañal-Bruland, Rouwen, additional, and Raab, Markus, additional
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
84. Excellence and expert performance in sports: what do we know and where are we going?
- Author
-
Zentgraf, Karen and Raab, Markus
- Subjects
GENOTYPE-environment interaction ,PERFORMANCES ,SPORTS ,EXPERTISE ,FACTOR structure ,PSYCHOLOGICAL research ,SPORTS psychology - Abstract
This narrative review in this special issue marking the 20-year anniversary of the International Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology aims to summarise work on expertise research in sport psychology and beyond. We shall review findings based on factors affecting the path to excellence. The structure of factors is aligned to the multifactorial gene–environment interaction model of Ullén et al. [2016. Rethinking expertise: A multifactorial gene–environment interaction model of expert performance. Psychological Bulletin, 142(4), 427–446]. Our research synthesis indicates that individual factors for expertise are well discussed; but, too often, not examined well empirically in terms of their quantity and quality. Further, it is notable that most of the interactions between important factors have not been investigated together, and thus it is too early to draw specific conclusions on how the combined effects of factors influence expertise development and sport performance. A literature search that differentiates findings on factors for expertise in all sport-related research (using SPORTDiscus as a source) in comparison to papers published in the International Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology provides new ways of focusing future research in unexplored factors and their interactions. We shall provide an example of a recent large-scale project of about 600 national team level athletes in seven different sports to illustrate how general expertise models such as that from Ullén et al. can be applied to diagnostics in elite performance as one way to advance research in the field. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
85. How Much Is Winning a Matter of Luck? A Comparison of 3 × 3 and 5v5 Basketball
- Author
-
Csurilla, Gergely, primary, Boros, Zoltán, additional, Fűrész, Diána Ivett, additional, Gyimesi, András, additional, Raab, Markus, additional, and Sterbenz, Tamás, additional
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
86. Cognitive functions and special working capacity in elite boxers
- Author
-
Korobeynikov, Georgiy, primary, Korobeinikova, Lesia, additional, Raab, Markus, additional, Baić, Mario, additional, Borysova, Olga, additional, Korobeinikova, Ivanna, additional, Shengpeng, Gou, additional, and Khmelnitska, Irene, additional
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
87. TRACK—a new algorithm and open-source tool for the analysis of pursuit-tracking sensorimotor integration processes
- Author
-
Böttcher, Adriana, primary, Adelhöfer, Nico, additional, Wilken, Saskia, additional, Raab, Markus, additional, Hoffmann, Sven, additional, and Beste, Christian, additional
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
88. Embodied choices bypass narratives under radical uncertainty
- Author
-
Cañal-Bruland, Rouwen, primary and Raab, Markus, additional
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
89. On the Mechanisms of Audio-Visual Integration for Manual Interception
- Author
-
Kreyenmeier, Philipp, primary, Schroeger, Anna, additional, Cañal-Bruland, Rouwen, additional, Raab, Markus, additional, and Spering, Miriam, additional
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
90. Психологическое состояние спортсменов высокой квалификации
- Author
-
Korobeynikov, Georgiy, primary, Baich, Mario, additional, Korobeinikova, Lesea, additional, Raab, Markus, additional, Korobeinikova, Ivanna, additional, and Starchevich, Nikola, additional
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
91. Theoretically, it's all about joy - Does joy mediate or moderate the positive effect of physical activity on executive functions?
- Author
-
Voigt, Laura, Musculus, Lisa, Gollas, Lisa, and Raab, Markus
- Subjects
FOS: Psychology ,Sports Studies ,Cognitive Psychology ,Psychology ,Social and Behavioral Sciences - Abstract
There is an ongoing debate about whether an acute bout of physical activity has positive effects on executive functions (EF) with the latest meta-analyses revealing inconsistent results (Ishihara et al., 2021; Ludyga et al., 2016). So far, positive effects have mainly been explained on a physiological level (e.g., catecholamines, event-related potentials, cerebral oxygenation, see McMorris, 2016). Less attention has been paid to the psychological aspects of physical activity. Regarding psychological effects, Diamond and Ling (2020) have suggested that emotional investment and joy during the activity might contribute to the positive effect. In the present study, we conceptualize joy in terms of the broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions (Fredrickson, 2004). From this perspective, positive emotions, such as joy, expand one’s momentary thoughts and action repertoires to facilitate the “building” of new patterns of thought and behavior, resulting in improved cognition (for a review see Isen, 2000). Considering that physical activity below the ventilatory or lactate threshold has been shown to elicit positive affect (Ekkekakis et al., 2011), we assume that joy plays a role in the positive effect of acute exercise on EF. The mechanism behind the potential role of joy in the positive effect of physical activity on EF has, however, not been theoretically specified yet. To address this gap, we will empirically test whether joy is a moderator or mediator of the positive effect of physical acute exercise on EF (see Figure 1 and 2). In a within-subject crossover pre- and posttest comparison, 51 participants will perform an EF task battery (i.e., n-back task, flanker task, and number-letter task) during a baseline session and after a moderate cycling bout (40-59% VO2R) and seated rest in counterbalanced order on different days. Joy will be assessed by the Positive and Negative Affect extended Scale (Watson & Clarke, 1999).
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
92. Poster FEPSAC 2022
- Author
-
Voigt, Laura, Sparascio, Simone, Musculus, Lisa, and Raab, Markus
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
93. Implicit learning of movement selection and execution to improve motor performance under demanding circumstances
- Author
-
Musculus, Lisa, Lobinger, Babett, Raab, Markus, Prof. Rich Masters, Loffing, Florian, Van Duijn, Tina, Nieuwenhuys, Arne, Redlich, Dennis, Voigt, Laura, and Chris
- Subjects
Cognition and Perception ,Community Psychology ,Experimental Analysis of Behavior ,analogies ,heuristics ,Social and Behavioral Sciences ,less is more ,motor behavior ,sport psychology ,FOS: Psychology ,embodied cognition ,motor performance ,Psychology ,table tennis - Abstract
The current experiment is considered a “Proof-of-Concept” in which we aim to establish the feasibility and rationale for an integrated theory that has been developed as part of the German Research Foundation (DFG) project “Motor heuristics and movement analogies in performance and health” (PIs: Markus Raab and Laura Voigt, RA 940/27-1 Ι VO 2789/1-1). For successful sports performance, athletes must choose between actions (movement selection) and then execute the selected action skillfully (movement execution; Raab, 2017; Voigt et al., 2022). A table tennis player who can execute a backhand stroke effectively but does not know when to use the backhand stroke will not succeed in the sport. Equally, a player who knows when to employ the backhand stroke but cannot execute it effectively is unlikely to succeed. Movement selection and execution often need to be performed when time is limited, stress is present, more than one task needs to be processed, or when fatigue or unfamiliar situations occur. Given limited ability to process multiple channels of information, performers need to find ways to manage the wealth of available information economically to make appropriate decisions and execute the chosen movement skillfully in their discipline. The idea that economic management of information is central for successful performance is supported by a common understanding of expertise in sports: During the development of expertise, the nature of the knowledge structures that support motor performance gradually changes over time, with an increasing degree of implicit (unconscious) processing and a decreasing level of explicit (conscious) processing (Fitts & Posner, 1967). Explicit processes involve the retrieval of consciously accessible (declarative) knowledge and depend on working memory. Thus, highly explicit sport performance has often been shown to be disrupted by demands resulting from performance pressure or multiple task requirements (Masters & Maxwell, 2008; Nieuwenhuys & Oudejans, 2012, 2017). In contrast, implicit processes are faster and involve sophisticated complexes of procedural knowledge that can be applied without conscious thought, with greater automaticity and fewer demands for attentional resources (e.g., Anderson, 1983; Lewicki et al., 1992; Kal et al., 2018; Masters & Maxwell, 2004; Shiffrin & Schneider, 1977; Willingham, 1998). Implicit processes are therefore less dependent on working memory, which allows the expert to economically manage multiple streams of information for movement selection and movement execution while being taxed with other demands (for a review of the theoretical architecture and function of working memory, see Baddeley, 2003). But how exactly are these streams of information for movement selection and movement execution managed? In the current study, we aim to deliver a “Proof-of-Concept” for the integration of two theories – motor heuristics (Raab, 2017) and motor analogies (Masters, 2000, 2012). We aim to investigate whether implicit learning helps athletes to manage information during movement selection and movement execution and protects performance under demanding circumstances. Motor heuristics have their theoretical basis in the simple heuristics used for decision making (Gigerenzer & Goldstein, 1996) and advise “what” movement to choose (i.e., movement selection). Motor heuristics are fast and frugal decision-making strategies (i.e., rules of thumb) that exploit information in the environment by ordering pieces of information (i.e., cues) by their validity. Decision makers judge the cues’ validity (i.e., how often in the past a particular piece of information was helpful in making a choice), and then choose the option that is favored by the cue with the highest validity. In sports, a large body of empirical findings demonstrates that superior decision making by experts is characterized by focusing on fewer (task-relevant) options and higher-quality options and decisions (Basevitch et al., 2020; Belling et al., 2015; Laborde & Raab, 2013; Musculus, 2018; Musculus et al., 2021; Raab & Johnson, 2007), especially in demanding circumstances, such as time pressure, opponent pressure, and stress (Musculus et al., 2021). For heuristics to be successful, their use needs to be matched to the environmental structures. Leuker et al. (2018, 2019) showed that statistical regularities (i.e., risk-reward structures) can be learned without explicit instructions via incidental, unsupervised learning and then utilized as heuristics for decision making under uncertainty. Implicit learning processes can thus be an important aspect in building a representation of the environment that, in turn, guides choice behavior (Hertwig et al., 2022). Importantly, the so-called description-experience gap further suggests that learning environmental structures by experience is more beneficial for performance than receiving explicit information about the environmental structures (e.g., Hertwig et al., 2004; Armstrong & Spaniol, 2017). Motor analogies have their theoretical basis in the theory of implicit motor learning (Masters, 1992, 2000) and advise ‘how’ to move (i.e., movement execution). Motor analogies leverage a concept that is already well known, such as “strike the ball while bringing the bat up the hypotenuse of a triangle” in order to convey the complex structure of the motor skill (e.g., a table tennis topspin forehand; Liao & Masters, 2001). It has been proposed that they promote economic management of information for movement control by chunking fundamental technical information (i.e., relevant pieces of information) into one well-known concept – many small “bits” of information are collapsed to into fewer larger chunks (Poolton & Masters, 2014). Although fewer chunks are processed, they contain the relevant information, meaning that information can be processed with relatively less cognitive effort and processing becomes more efficient. At the same time, motor analogies minimize accrual of conscious knowledge of the underlying rules governing the mechanics of movements (e.g., Liao & Masters, 2001). Learning movements implicitly through analogies has been shown to result in more robust motor performance under demanding conditions than learning movements by explicit step-by-step instructions (e.g., Koedijker et al., 2007, Lam et al., 2009; Liao & Masters, 2001; Schlapkohl et al., 2012; for a meta-analysis see Cabral et al., 2022), which has been attributed to the placement of fewer demands on cognitive resources than explicit motor learning. To date, motor heuristics and analogies have only been researched independently from each other. Notably, it has been shown that implicit motor learning through motor analogies did not only improve motor performance, but also decision making, potentially because implicit processing of movement execution left sufficient cognitive resources available for decision making (Lola & Tzetzis, 2021; Masters et al., 2008). Likewise, the use of heuristics (instead of a complex step-by-step planning strategy) showed combined selection and execution advantages, when participants were required to select one of two computer cursors that displace in different directions (i.e., selection) and subsequently navigate from a starting position to a goal as efficiently as possible (i.e., execution; Dundon et al., 2023). We aim to extend these findings by testing performance in demanding circumstances after promoting the economic management of information for both movement selection and execution via implicit learning of motor heuristics and motor analogies.
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
94. Advancing judgment and decision-making research in sport psychology by using the body as an informant in embodied choices
- Author
-
Voigt, Laura, Friedrich, Jannis, Grove, Patricia, Heinrich, Nils, Ittlinger, Sandra, Iskra, Maša, Koop, Lisa, Michirev, Alexej, Sparascio, Simone, and Raab, Markus
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
95. Sport and exercise psychology in 2050
- Author
-
Raab, Markus
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
96. The Hot Hand Belief and Framing Effects
- Author
-
MacMahon, Clare, Köppen, Jörn, and Raab, Markus
- Abstract
Purpose: Recent evidence of the hot hand in sport--where success breeds success in a positive recency of successful shots, for instance--indicates that this pattern does not actually exist. Yet the belief persists. We used 2 studies to explore the effects of framing on the hot hand belief in sport. We looked at the effect of sport experience and task on the perception of baseball pitch behavior as well as the hot hand belief and free-throw behavior in basketball. Method: Study 1 asked participants to designate outcomes with different alternation rates as the result of baseball pitches or coin tosses. Study 2 examined basketball free-throw behavior and measured predicted success before each shot as well as general belief in the hot hand pattern. Results: The results of Study 1 illustrate that experience and stimulus alternation rates influence the perception of chance in human performance tasks. Study 2 shows that physically performing an act and making judgments are related. Specifically, beliefs were related to overall performance, with more successful shooters showing greater belief in the hot hand and greater predicted success for upcoming shots. Conclusions: Both of these studies highlight that the hot hand belief is influenced by framing, which leads to instability and situational contingencies. We show the specific effects of framing using accumulated experience of the individual with the sport and knowledge of its structure and specific experience with sport actions (basketball shots) prior to judgments.
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
97. Fahrerassistenzsysteme
- Author
-
Schopper, Michael, Kandemir, Taner, Fröming, Robert, Messner, Heiner, Raab, Markus, Grimm, Michael, Ernstberger, Uwe, editor, Weissinger, Jürgen, editor, and Frank, Jürgen, editor
- Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
98. Psychological Assessments in Physical Exercise
- Author
-
Daamen, Marcel, Raab, Markus, Boecker, Henning, editor, Hillman, Charles H., editor, Scheef, Lukas, editor, and Strüder, Heiko K., editor
- Published
- 2012
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
99. Force of numbers: number magnitude modulates force magnitude
- Author
-
Michirev, Alexej, Raab, Markus, Lindemann, Oliver, Fischer, Martin, and Kühne, Katharina
- Subjects
FOS: Psychology ,Psychology ,Social and Behavioral Sciences - Abstract
This project aims to measure how the cognitive processing of number magnitude affects spontaneous force production under highly controlled spatial influences and no active responses. For this, we will employ a novel method of continuous grip force measurement with high temporal and force resolution (ms/mN) during number presentations across two tasks. In the first task (1-back) we aim to measure force magnitudes during numbers from 1-9 (with a focus on 1,2,8,9). In the second task (Arithmetic task), we aim to replicate the force to number magnitude relation for the numbers 1,2,8,9. We will also explore how 1,2,8,9 affect force magnitudes following an arithmetic operation (either subtraction or addition). If there is an effect, we base our theoretical predictions based on two models: A Theory of Magnitude (ATOM, Walsh, 2003;2015) and the confidence model (Balota & Abrams, 1995). According to ATOM we would expect increasing force magnitude to increasing numbers while the confidence model would predict decreasing force magnitude to increasing numbers.
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
100. Tactile time-based expectancy - Experiment 2
- Author
-
Rodríguez-Velásquez, Alejandra, Kiesel, Andrea, Broeker, Laura, Raab, Markus, Ewolds, Harald, Künzell, Stefan, Thomaschke, Roland, Schulz, Melanie, and Billian, Janina
- Subjects
FOS: Psychology ,Cognitive Psychology ,Psychology ,time-based expectancy ,Social and Behavioral Sciences - Abstract
This is the second of a series of experiments on time-based expectancy for finger responses to vibrotactile stimulation. In this experiment, we induce time-based expectancy by pairing two events and two preparatory intervals. In a binary randomized response choice setting, participants respond by keypresses to vibrotactile stimulation to their fingers. Stimulus response mapping is compatible (i.e. respond with the finger that was stimulated), and stimuli are preceded by either a short or a long preparatory interval which predicts with 90% probability which finger will be stimulated next. This pairing of vibrotactile stimulation (left-right finger) and interval duration (short-long) is counterbalanced across participants. For half of the participants the short interval will predict stimulation on the left finger, and the long interval will predict stimulation on the right finger with 90% validity. For the other half, this relation will be inverted. We decide between two hypotheses: 1) Participants respond about equally fast to frequent combinations of interval duration and stimulation side than infrequent ones. Such finding would mean that participants did not form time-based expectancies to vibrotactile stimuli. 2) Participants respond faster to frequent than infrequent combinations of interval duration and stimulation side. Such finding would mean that participants build time-based expectancies to vibrotactile stimuli, and would be consistent with the time-based expectancy literature (see, Thomaschke et al., 2011; Thomaschke et al., 2015) We decide between these hypotheses by a Bayesian analogue to a paired sample t-test (Morey & Rouder, 2011). We cumulatively test participants until we reach a JZS Bayes factor over 5 for either of the hypotheses, or until we have tested 120 participants without reaching a JZS Bayes factor over 5. We collect data with the E-Prime program uploaded to this repository. We will pre-process the data as follows: We do not include participants with an overall mean RT or an overall mean error rate lying more than 2.5 SDs away from the sample mean. We do not include the first trial of each block, the first block, error trials, and trials following an error. Since we will compare two conditions observations (frequent vs. infrequent) with different number of, we will follow special recommendations to calculate the median RT. We will take the regular RT median for the infrequent condition and we will compare it with a special “average median” from the frequent condition, separately for the short and long intervals durations (for a more detailed explanation, see Miller, 1988) By frequent we mean trials with combinations of interval duration and stimulation side with 90% validity and as infrequent count trials with combinations of interval duration and stimulation side with 10% validity. We conduct the Bayesian t-test over these conditions. All further analyses will be of exploratory nature: For example, effects on error rates, effects of interval duration, etc. For following exploratory analyses, the script will be extended.
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.