9 results on '"Kidd, Julie K."'
Search Results
2. A Dynamic Process Model for Continued Integration between ECE and EI/ECSE Teacher Preparation Programs
- Author
-
La Croix, Leslie, Kidd, Julie K., Walter, Heather, Stone, Deborah, Ferguson, Daniel, Fisher-Maltese, Carley, Steen, Bweikia, and Vesely, Colleen
- Abstract
Early childhood and early childhood special education programs have predominantly been separate teacher preparation programs that prepare novice teachers for general or special education settings. However, with a growing shift toward inclusion, as well as a shortage of educators with training to teach all children, there is a need for teacher preparation programs to prepare all early childhood educators to support all children and engage all families. This article describes one institute of higher education's integrated program and the program's process to align coursework with both the "Professional Standards and Competencies for Early Childhood Educators" and "Initial Practice-Based Professional Preparation Standards for Early Interventionists/Early Childhood Special Educators." We do this by discussing (a) the landscape of teacher education and integrated programs, (b) one IHE's integrated program history, (c) our process for embedding standards, and (d) next steps.
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
3. Teaching Early Literacy, Mathematics, and Patterning to Kindergartners
- Author
-
Pasnak, Robert, Gagliano, Katrina, Righi, Matthew, and Kidd, Julie K.
- Abstract
The effects of instructing kindergarten students (five-year-old in their first year of public school) on patterning were tested. Up to nine students in each class were randomly assigned to three groups in each of six classes, i.e., as many as three from a class in each group. One group received instruction on complex patterns similar to those employed by other researchers with first graders. Another group was instructed in early literacy, and another in early mathematics. For each group, the experimental instruction was scheduled in a counterbalanced order for 15 minutes in the morning, five days per week for most of the school year. When tested in late May-early June, the children taught patterning scored significantly better on a test of patterning. There were no significant differences on tests of mathematics and early literacy. However, there were correlations between the children's patterning scores and some literacy scales.
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
4. Evaluation of Patterning Instruction for Kindergartners
- Author
-
Strauss, Lauren I., Peterson, Matthew S., Kidd, Julie K., Choe, Jihyae, Lauritzen, Hans Christian, Patterson, Allyson B., Holmberg, Courtney A., Gallington, Debbie A., and Pasnak, Robert
- Abstract
To determine whether patterning instruction was as useful or more useful than other forms of instruction, kindergarten children (age five) were taught either patterning or early literacy or mathematics or social studies in matched sessions. Instruction was conducted in 15-minute sessions from November through mid-April. Posttests on patterning, mathematics, early literacy, and three executive functions showed that the children taught patterning became significantly better at patterning than those in the other instructional conditions. No differences were found between the children taught mathematics, early literacy, or social studies. Correlational analyses indicated that the relations of patterning ability, working memory, and inhibitory control to mathematics achievement were similar. Cognitive flexibility was not very strongly related to any other measure and the executive functions were relatively independent of each other for the children who were age five.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
5. A Teacher-Friendly Method of Improving Reading and Mathematics
- Author
-
Society for Research on Educational Effectiveness (SREE), Kidd, Julie K., Gadzichowski, K. Marinka, Gallington, Deb A., Lopez, Claudia, and Pasnak, Robert
- Abstract
In early elementary school in most English-speaking countries children are taught "patterning," which involves learning repetitive patterns of colors or shapes (e.g., red, blue, green, red, blue, green). The present study was designed to test the effectiveness of patterning instruction when compared to equal amounts of instruction in reading or mathematics, or social studies. Further, The effectiveness of using more complex patterns was also tested. The authors used: (1) symmetrical patterns--gray, blue, pink, pink, blue, gray; (2) patterns that had increasing numbers of one element--red, tan, red, tan, tan, red, tan, tan, tan; (3) arbitrary repeating patterns--white, green, black, brown, yellow, white; and (4) patterns that showed an object rotating through 6 or 8 positions. Results of the study indicate that this patterning instruction involved patterns that were more complex than those used in conventional patterning instruction; the instruction continued all year; and required individual mastery of each pattern before moving on to the next. A gain of even two months in grade equivalents has great practical significance to educators, and the patterning children often made gains much larger than that. In addition, long-term instruction on and mastery of increasingly complex patterns can lead to substantially improved performance on some standardized tests of reading and mathematics, but not all. Tables are appended.
- Published
- 2013
6. Is Patterning Helpful in Children's Education?
- Author
-
Society for Research on Educational Effectiveness (SREE), Kidd, Julie K., Gadzichowski, K. Marinka, Gallington, Debbie A., Boyer, Caroline E., and Pasnak, Robert
- Abstract
The authors' purpose was to study the efficacy of patterning instruction in improving the reading and mathematics proficiency of first grade children. Patterning instruction was compared to equal time and effort spent on instruction on reading or mathematics or social studies, which are control conditions in this experiment. The question for educators was whether instruction on patterning would be produce greater overall gains than instruction on the other subject matter. The research was conducted in a public school system of an urban school district in Northern Virginia, which served many low income and immigrant families. (Contains 1 table.)
- Published
- 2012
7. Cognitive Underpinnings of Preschool Literacy and Numeracy
- Author
-
Society for Research on Educational Effectiveness (SREE), Kidd, Julie K., Pasnak, Robert, Curby, Timothy W., Ferhat, Caroline Boyer, Gadzichowski, K. Marinka, Gallington, Debbie A., and Machado, Jessica
- Abstract
The present research represents a test of the effect of adding seriation instruction to oddity instruction to produce an advantage in both forms of abstraction. Pasnak et al. (2007) and Kidd, Pasnak, Gadzichowski, Ferral-Like, & Gallington (2008) have shown that at risk kindergartners profit academically from instruction in both oddity and seriation. This study extends this work into the preschool classroom, testing whether oddity and seriation instruction benefits at risk preschoolers in both literacy and numeracy. The research was conducted in seven Head Start preschools in Alexandria, Virginia, an urban community just south of Washington DC. That the literacy and numeracy control lessons were effective is shown by the high scores the children in those groups made in the specific domain in which they were instructed, However, the children who received the literacy or art instruction scored below national norms on numeracy, and the children taught numeracy or art likewise scored below norms on literacy, despite the enrichment offered by their Head Start program. Such outcomes are likely when children have been raised in impoverished homes. Lacking the cognitive enrichment that would be found in many middle-class homes, they were probably functioning closer to the floor provided by their inherent abilities than to the ceiling. At least, that is one of the assumptions on which this research was based. Lagging in normal cognitive development, they would be less apt to understand and profit from preschool activities aimed at fostering literacy and numeracy. That the cognitive group matched the numeracy group in numeracy, and the literacy group in literacy, testifies to the importance of the advantages in abstraction that group had gained. These advantages appear to have enabled it to better understand the normal preschool curriculum, which offered plenty of chances to improve on literacy and numeracy. Preschool learning activities assume that children can detect relevant differences and understand the relations between big, medium, small, and so forth. When children are deficient in these abstractions, many of the instructional activities are over their heads. The authors conclude that strengthening children's understanding of differences on one dimension and unidimensional ordering enables them to gain more from learning opportunities in the preschool classroom.
- Published
- 2010
8. Effects of Patterning Instruction on the Academic Achievement of 1st-Grade Children
- Author
-
Kidd, Julie K., Carlson, Abby G., Gadzichowski, K. Marinka, Boyer, Caroline E., Gallington, Debbie A., and Pasnak, Robert
- Abstract
A test of the effectiveness of patterning instruction was conducted with 140 first-graders. First, 383 first-graders from 20 classes were screened on a patterning test. The eight in each class who scored worst were given individual 15-minute lessons on patterning or reading or mathematics or social studies three times weekly for a period of 6 1/2 months. Test results for 140 children still available in May showed that the children receiving patterning instruction were generally superior on a patterning test. They also scored significantly higher on the Woodcock-Johnson (W-J) III Mathematics Concepts Scale 18A than children who received mathematics, reading, or social studies lessons. On the W-J III Mathematics Concepts Scale 18B, children who received either patterning or mathematics instruction scored significantly higher than those who received social studies instruction. There were no significant differences on the W-J III Applied Problems Scale 10, nor on three W-J III reading scales. These results suggest that instruction on understanding patterns can substantially improve 1st-graders' understanding of mathematical concepts. Such outcomes may be specific to the sort of children, primarily minority children from diverse ethnic backgrounds, who were participants in this study. (Contains 3 tables.)
- Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
9. Can Emphasising Cognitive Development Improve Academic Achievement?
- Author
-
Pasnak, Robert, Kidd, Julie K., Gadzichowski, Marinka K., Gallington, Deborah A., and Saracina, Robin P.
- Abstract
Background: Children ordinarily begin their formal education at the age when the great majority of them are capable of understanding the role of addition and subtraction in changing number. In determining critical differences they can apply the oddity principle--the first "pure" abstraction that children ever develop--understanding that when all but one item are alike on some dimension, it is the relation between items, not their absolute qualities, which determines which belongs to a different set, i.e. is "odd". They are also capable of inserting items into a unidimensional series, a form of abstraction which develops a bit later. Children slow to develop such abilities often experience difficulty understanding classroom instruction in the first year of school. Purpose: The purpose of the research was to teach children oddity, insertions and conservation and compare their subsequent literacy and numeracy with that of children taught literacy and numeracy directly. All children develop oddity, insertions and conservation through unstructured interaction with the environment, but some do so belatedly, after patterns of school failure have been established. Is it worthwhile to spend classroom time teaching these thinking abilities? This research was designed to test whether teaching them produced better academic achievement than equal time spent teaching academic material. Programme: Teachers can use a wide variety of common objects to teach in an efficient and structured way the abstractions 5 year olds ordinarily learn less efficiently in their daily lives. This method, called a "learning set" approach, is easily applied by teachers or assistants without the need for special training. Sample: The participants were culturally diverse 5 year olds, generally of low socio-economic status, enrolled in five urban schools. Students from 25 classrooms were screened to determine whether they already understood the concepts which were to be taught. The final sample had 82 boys and 74 girls. Fifty-two were Hispanic/Latino, 36 African American, 33 White US born, 21 Mideastern, 7 East African, 3 Asian Indian, 2 West African, and 2 East Asian. Design and methods: In an experimental design, the children were randomly assigned to one experimental and three control groups. The former were taught number conservation, the oddity principle, and how to insert objects into a series. One control group was taught numeracy: recognition and identification of numbers, counting by 1s, 5s, or 10s, and other aspects of numeracy. Another control group was taught literacy: upper and lower case letters, letter sounds, rhyming and blending. A third control group was taught social studies: family structure and activities, the major senses and body parts, community resources, mapping and citizenship. The instruction was conducted from October through February. In May and June, the children were tested on oddity, insertions, number conservation, literacy and numeracy. Numeracy was measured with the Woodcock-Johnson III and literacy with the "Stanford early school achievement test". Research instruments were used to measure the cognitive concepts. Results: The children taught the three thinking abilities outscored the control children on tests of these concepts. They surpassed the literacy control group in numeracy and matched it in literacy, surpassed the numeracy group in literacy while matching it in numeracy, and surpassed the social studies group in both. Conclusions: Small group lessons on the oddity principle, insertions into series and number conservation may benefit kindergartners who lack these concepts. Mastery of these abstractions may enable them to understand academic subject matter which they would otherwise have difficulty comprehending. (Contains 3 tables.)
- Published
- 2008
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.