26 results on '"Awh, Edward"'
Search Results
2. Distinguishing guesses from fuzzy memories: Further evidence for item limits in visual working memory
- Author
-
Ngiam, William XQ, Foster, Joshua J, Adam, Kirsten CS, and Awh, Edward
- Subjects
Cognitive and Computational Psychology ,Psychology ,Clinical Research ,Mental health ,Humans ,Memory ,Short-Term ,Mental Recall ,Visual Perception ,Visual working memory ,Working memory ,Memory ,Visual working and short-term memory ,Memory: Visual working and short-term memory ,Cognitive Sciences ,Experimental Psychology ,Biological psychology ,Cognitive and computational psychology - Abstract
There is consistent debate over whether capacity in working memory (WM) is subject to an item limit, or whether an unlimited number of items can be held in this online memory system. The item limit hypothesis clearly predicts guessing responses when capacity is exceeded, and proponents of this view have highlighted evidence for guessing in visual working memory tasks. Nevertheless, various models that deny item limits can explain the same empirical patterns by asserting extremely low fidelity representations that cannot be distinguished from guesses. To address this ambiguity, we employed a task for which guess responses elicited a qualitatively distinct pattern from low fidelity memories. Inspired by work from Rouder et al. (2014), we employed an orientation WM task that required subjects to recall the precise orientation of each of six memoranda presented 1 s earlier. The orientation stimuli were created by rotating the position of a "clock hand" inside a circular region that was demarcated by four colored quadrants. Critically, when observers guess with these stimuli, the distribution of responses is biased towards the center of these quadrants, creating a "banded" pattern that cannot be explained by a low precision memory. We confirmed the presence of this guessing pattern using formal model comparisons, and we show that the prevalence of this pattern matches observers' own reports of when they thought they were guessing. Thus, these findings provide further evidence for guessing behaviors predicted by item limit models of WM capacity.
- Published
- 2023
3. Multivariate analysis reveals a generalizable human electrophysiological signature of working memory load
- Author
-
Adam, Kirsten CS, Vogel, Edward K, and Awh, Edward
- Subjects
Cognitive and Computational Psychology ,Psychology ,Neurosciences ,Brain Disorders ,Mental Health ,Clinical Research ,Adult ,Cerebral Cortex ,Datasets as Topic ,Electroencephalography ,Humans ,Memory ,Short-Term ,Multivariate Analysis ,Psychomotor Performance ,Space Perception ,Visual Perception ,classification ,EEG ,multivariate pattern analysis ,working memory ,Biological Sciences ,Medical and Health Sciences ,Psychology and Cognitive Sciences ,Experimental Psychology ,Biological sciences ,Biomedical and clinical sciences - Abstract
Working memory (WM) is an online memory system that is critical for holding information in a rapidly accessible state during ongoing cognitive processing. Thus, there is strong value in methods that provide a temporally resolved index of WM load. While univariate EEG signals have been identified that vary with WM load, recent advances in multivariate analytic approaches suggest that there may be rich sources of information that do not generate reliable univariate signatures. Here, using data from four published studies (n = 286 and >250,000 trials), we demonstrate that multivariate analysis of EEG voltage topography provides a sensitive index of the number of items stored in WM that generalizes to novel human observers. Moreover, multivariate load detection ("mvLoad") can provide robust information at the single-trial level, exceeding the sensitivity of extant univariate approaches. We show that this method tracks WM load in a manner that is (1) independent of the spatial position of the memoranda, (2) precise enough to differentiate item-by-item increments in the number of stored items, (3) generalizable across distinct tasks and stimulus displays, and (4) correlated with individual differences in WM behavior. Thus, this approach provides a powerful complement to univariate analytic approaches, enabling temporally resolved tracking of online memory storage in humans.
- Published
- 2020
4. Pupillometry signatures of sustained attention and working memory
- Author
-
Keene, Paul A., deBettencourt, Megan T., Awh, Edward, and Vogel, Edward K.
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
5. Cortically Disparate Visual Features Evoke Content-Independent Load Signals during Storage in Working Memory.
- Author
-
Jones, Henry M., Thyer, William S., Suplica, Darius, and Awh, Edward
- Subjects
SHORT-term memory ,YOUNG adults ,CLASSROOM activities ,ELECTROENCEPHALOGRAPHY - Abstract
It is well established that holding information in working memory (WM) elicits sustained stimulus-specific patterns of neural activity. Nevertheless, here we provide evidence for a distinct class of neural activity that tracks the number of individuated items in working memory, independent of the type of visual features stored. We present two EEG studies of young adults of both sexes that provide robust evidence for a signal tracking the number of individuated representations in working memory, regardless of the specific feature values stored. In Study 1, subjects maintained either colors or orientations across separate blocks in a single session. We found near-perfect generalization of the load signal between these two conditions, despite being able to simultaneously decode which feature had been voluntarily stored. In Study 2, participants attended to two features with very distinct cortical representations: color and motion coherence. We again found evidence for a neural load signal that robustly generalized across these distinct visual features, even though cortically disparate regions process color and motion coherence. Moreover, representational similarity analysis provided converging evidence for a content-independent load signal, while simultaneously showing that unique variance in EEG activity tracked the specific features that were stored. We posit that this load signal reflects a content-independent “pointer” operation that binds objects to the current context while parallel but distinct neural signals represent the features that are stored for each item in memory. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
6. A Common Discrete Resource for Visual Working Memory and Visual Search
- Author
-
Anderson, David E., Vogel, Edward K., and Awh, Edward
- Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
7. The Capacity of Audiovisual Integration Is Limited to One Item
- Author
-
Van der Burg, Erik, Awh, Edward, and Olivers, Christian N. L.
- Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
8. Stimulus-Specific Delay Activity in Human Primary Visual Cortex
- Author
-
Serences, John T., Ester, Edward F., Vogel, Edward K., and Awh, Edward
- Published
- 2009
9. How to Exploit Diversity for Scientific Gain: Using Individual Differences to Constrain Cognitive Theory
- Author
-
Vogel, Edward K. and Awh, Edward
- Published
- 2008
10. Visual Working Memory Represents a Fixed Number of Items Regardless of Complexity
- Author
-
Awh, Edward, Barton, Brian, and Vogel, Edward K.
- Published
- 2007
11. Evidence for a fixed capacity limit in attending multiple locations
- Author
-
Ester, Edward F., Fukuda, Keisuke, May, Lisa M., Vogel, Edward K., and Awh, Edward
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
12. Evidence for Two Components of Object-Based Selection
- Author
-
Awh, Edward, Dhaliwal, Harpreet, Christensen, Shauna, and Matsukura, Michi
- Published
- 2001
13. Rehearsal in Spatial Working Memory: Evidence from Neuroimaging
- Author
-
Awh, Edward, Jonides, John, Smith, Edward E., Buxton, Richard B., Frank, Larry R., Love, Tracy, Wong, Eric C., and Gmeindl, Leon
- Published
- 1999
14. Dissociation of Storage and Rehearsal in Verbal Working Memory: Evidence from Positron Emission Tomography
- Author
-
Awh, Edward, Jonides, John, Smith, Edward E., Schumacher, Eric H., Koeppe, Robert A., and Katz, Stewart
- Published
- 1996
15. Benchmarks for Models of Short-Term and Working Memory.
- Author
-
Oberauer, Klaus, Awh, Edward, Conway, Andrew, Donkin, Christopher, Hitch, Graham J., Wei Ji Ma, Nee, Derek Evan, Vergauwe, Evie, Lewandowsky, Stephan, Brown, Gordon D. A., Cowan, Nelson, Farrell, Simon, Hurlstone, Mark J., Morey, Candice C., Schweppe, Judith, and Ward, Geoff
- Subjects
- *
SHORT-term memory , *EMPIRICAL research , *COGNITION , *REASONING , *SURVEYS - Abstract
Any mature field of research in psychology--such as short-term/working memory--is characterized by a wealth of empirical findings. It is currently unrealistic to expect a theory to explain them all; theorists must satisfice with explaining a subset of findings. The aim of the present article is to make the choice of that subset less arbitrary and idiosyncratic than is current practice. We propose criteria for identifying benchmark findings that every theory in a field should be able to explain: Benchmarks should be reproducible, generalize across materials and methodological variations, and be theoretically informative. We propose a set of benchmarks for theories and computational models of short-term and working memory. The benchmarks are described in as theory-neutral a way as possible, so that they can serve as empirical common ground for competing theoretical approaches. Benchmarks are rated on three levels according to their priority for explanation. Selection and ratings of the benchmarks is based on consensus among the authors, who jointly represent a broad range of theoretical perspectives on working memory, and they are supported by a survey among other experts on working memory. The article is accompanied by a web page providing an open forum for discussion and for submitting proposals for new benchmarks; and a repository for reference data sets for each benchmark. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
16. Benchmarks Provide Common Ground for Model Development: Reply to Logie (2018) and Yandierendonck (2018).
- Author
-
Oberauer, Klaus, Awh, Edward, Conway, Andrew, Donkin, Christopher, Hitch, Graham J., Wei Ji Ma, Nee, Derek Evan, Vergauwe, Evie, Lewandowsky, Stephan, Brown, Gordon D. A., Cowan, Nelson, Farrell, Simon, Hurlstone, Mark J., Morey, Candice C., Schweppe, Judith, and Ward, Geoff
- Subjects
- *
SHORT-term memory , *COGNITION - Abstract
We respond to the comments of Logie and Vandierendonck to our article proposing benchmark findings for evaluating theories and models of short-term and working memory. The response focuses on the two main points of criticism: (a) Logie and Vandierendonck argue that the scope of the set of benchmarks is too narrow. We explain why findings on how working memory is used in complex cognition, findings on executive functions, and findings from neuropsychological case studies are currently not included in the benchmarks, and why findings with visual and spatial materials are less prevalent among them, (b) The critics question the usefulness of the benchmarks and their ratings for advancing theory development. We explain why selecting and rating benchmarks is important and justifiable, and acknowledge that the present selection and rating decisions are in need of continuous updating. The usefulness of the benchmarks of all ratings is also enhanced by our concomitant online posting of data for many of these benchmarks. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
17. The topography of alpha-band activity tracks the content of spatial working memory.
- Author
-
Edward Awh, Joshua J., Sutterer, David W., Serences, John T., Vogel, Edward K., and Awh, Edward
- Subjects
SHORT-term memory ,MEMORY ,INTELLECT ,MENTAL discipline ,NEUROPHYSIOLOGY - Abstract
Working memory (WM) is a system for the online storage of information. An emerging view is that neuronal oscillations coordinate the cellular assemblies that code the content of WM. In line with this view, previous work has demonstrated that oscillatory activity in the alpha band (8-12 Hz) plays a role in WM maintenance, but the exact contributions of this activity have remained unclear. Here, we used an inverted spatial encoding model in combination with electroencephalography (EEG) to test whether the topographic distribution of alpha-band activity tracks spatial representations held in WM. Participants in three experiments performed spatial WM tasks that required them to remember the precise angular location of a sample stimulus for 1,000-1,750 ms. Across all three experiments, we found that the topographic distribution of alpha-band activity tracked the specific location that was held in WM. Evoked (i.e., activity phase-locked to stimulus onset) and total (i.e., activity regardless of phase) power across a range of low-frequency bands transiently tracked the location of the sample stimulus following stimulus onset. However, only total power in the alpha band tracked the content of spatial WM throughout the memory delay period, which enabled reconstruction of locationselective channel tuning functions (CTFs). These findings demonstrate that alpha-band activity is directly related to the coding of spatial representations held in WM and provide a promising method for tracking the content of this online memory system. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
18. Induced Alpha Rhythms Track the Content and Quality of Visual Working Memory Representations with High Temporal Precision.
- Author
-
Anderson, David E., Serences, John T., Vogel, Edward K., and Awh, Edward
- Subjects
VISUAL memory ,ALPHA rhythm ,PRECISION (Information retrieval) ,NEUROPHYSIOLOGY ,ELECTROENCEPHALOGRAPHY ,BRAIN imaging - Abstract
Past work has suggested that neuronal oscillations coordinate the cellular assemblies that represent items in working memory (WM). In line with this hypothesis, we show that the spatial distribution of power in the alpha frequency band (8 -12 Hz) can be used to decode the content and quality of the representations stored in visualWM.WeacquiredEEGdata during an orientationWMtask, and used a forward encoding model of orientation selectivity to reconstruct orientation-specific response profiles (termed channel tuning functions, or CTFs) that tracked the orientation of the memorandum during both encoding and delay periods of the trial. Critically, these EEG-based CTFs were robust predictors of both between- and within-subject differences in mnemonic precision, showing that EEG-based CTFs provide a sensitive measure of the quality of sensory population codes. Experiments 2 and 3 established that these EEG-based CTFs are contingent on the voluntary storage goals of the observer. When observers were given a postsample cue to store or drop the memorandum, the resulting CTF was sustained in the "store" condition and rapidly eliminated following the "drop" cue. When observers were instructed to store one of two simultaneously presented stimuli, only the stored item was represented in a sustained fashion throughout the delay period. These findings suggest that the oscillatory activity in the alpha frequency band plays a central role in the active storage of information in visual WM, and demonstrate a powerful approach for tracking the precision of on-line memories with high temporal resolution. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
19. Factorial Comparison of Working Memory Models.
- Author
-
van den Berg, Ronald, Awh, Edward, and Wei Ji Ma
- Subjects
- *
SHORT-term memory , *VISUAL memory , *VISUAL perception , *MNEMONICS , *ESTIMATION theory , *FACTOR analysis - Abstract
Three questions have been prominent in the study of visual working memory limitations: (a) What is the nature of mnemonic precision (e.g., quantized or continuous)? (b) How many items are remembered? (c) To what extent do spatial binding errors account for working memory failures? Modeling studies have typically focused on comparing possible answers to a single one of these questions, even though the result of such a comparison might depend on the assumed answers to both others. Here, we consider every possible combination of previously proposed answers to the individual questions. Each model is then a point in a 3-factor model space containing a total of 32 models, of which only 6 have been tested previously. We compare all models on data from 10 delayed-estimation experiments from 6 laboratories (for a total of 164 subjects and 131,452 trials). Consistently across experiments, we find that (a) mnemonic precision is not quantized but continuous and not equal but variable across items and trials; (b) the number of remembered items is likely to be variable across trials, with a mean of 6.4 in the best model (median across subjects); (c) spatial binding errors occur but explain only a small fraction of responses (16.5% at set size 8 in the best model). We find strong evidence against all 6 documented models. Our results demonstrate the value of factorial model comparison in working memory. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
20. The Positional-Specificity Effect Reveals a Passive-Trace Contribution to Visual Short-Term Memory.
- Author
-
Postle, Bradley R., Awh, Edward, Serences, John T., Sutterer, David W., and D’Esposito, Mark
- Subjects
- *
VISUAL memory , *DECISION making , *HUMAN behavior , *FUNCTIONAL magnetic resonance imaging , *AVERSIVE stimuli , *COGNITIVE neuroscience , *COGNITIVE psychology - Abstract
The positional-specificity effect refers to enhanced performance in visual short-term memory (VSTM) when the recognition probe is presented at the same location as had been the sample, even though location is irrelevant to the match/nonmatch decision. We investigated the mechanisms underlying this effect with behavioral and fMRI studies of object change-detection performance. To test whether the positional-specificity effect is a direct consequence of active storage in VSTM, we varied memory load, reasoning that it should be observed for all objects presented in a sub-span array of items. The results, however, indicated that although robust with a memory load of 1, the positional-specificity effect was restricted to the second of two sequentially presented sample stimuli in a load-of-2 experiment. An additional behavioral experiment showed that this disruption wasn’t due to the increased load per se, because actively processing a second object – in the absence of a storage requirement – also eliminated the effect. These behavioral findings suggest that, during tests of object memory, position-related information is not actively stored in VSTM, but may be retained in a passive tag that marks the most recent site of selection. The fMRI data were consistent with this interpretation, failing to find location-specific bias in sustained delay-period activity, but revealing an enhanced response to recognition probes that matched the location of that trial’s sample stimulus. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
21. Inter-electrode correlations measured with EEG predict individual differences in cognitive ability.
- Author
-
Hakim, Nicole, Awh, Edward, Vogel, Edward K., and Rosenberg, Monica D.
- Subjects
- *
COGNITIVE ability , *INDIVIDUAL differences , *FLUID intelligence , *BIOMEDICAL signal processing , *SHORT-term memory , *COGNITION - Abstract
Human brains share a broadly similar functional organization with consequential individual variation. This duality in brain function has primarily been observed when using techniques that consider the spatial organization of the brain, such as MRI. Here, we ask whether these common and unique signals of cognition are also present in temporally sensitive but spatially insensitive neural signals. To address this question, we compiled electroencephalogram (EEG) data from individuals of both sexes while they performed multiple working memory tasks at two different data-collection sites (n = 171 and 165). Results revealed that trial-averaged EEG activity exhibited inter-electrode correlations that were stable within individuals and unique across individuals. Furthermore, models based on these inter-electrode correlations generalized across datasets to predict participants' working memory capacity and general fluid intelligence. Thus, inter-electrode correlation patterns measured with EEG provide a signature of working memory and fluid intelligence in humans and a new framework for characterizing individual differences in cognitive abilities. • Individuals show stable and unique inter-electrode correlations of EEG activity • Inter-electrode correlation patterns can be used to identify individuals • Models based on these patterns predict working memory and fluid intelligence • Common and unique signals of cognition are present in sparse EEG activity Hakim et al. use EEG-based signals to identify individuals from a group and predict individual working memory capacity and general fluid intelligence. The work provides evidence that common and unique signals of cognition are present in spatially sparse EEG activity. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
22. Electroencephalogram Decoding Reveals Distinct Processes for Directing Spatial Attention and Encoding Into Working Memory.
- Author
-
Jones, Henry M., Diaz, Gisella K., Ngiam, William X. Q., and Awh, Edward
- Subjects
- *
SHORT-term memory , *SPATIAL memory , *VISUAL memory , *ELECTROENCEPHALOGRAPHY , *ATTENTION , *ADULTS - Abstract
Past work reveals a tight relationship between spatial attention and storage in visual working memory. But is spatially attending an item tantamount to working memory encoding? Here, we tracked electroencephalography (EEG) signatures of spatial attention and working memory encoding while independently manipulating the number of memory items and the spatial extent of attention in two studies of adults (N = 39; N = 33). Neural measures of spatial attention tracked the position and size of the attended area independent of the number of individuated items encoded into working memory. At the same time, multivariate decoding of the number of items stored in working memory was insensitive to variations in the breadth and position of spatial attention. Finally, representational similarity analyses provided converging evidence for a pure load signal that is insensitive to the spatial extent of the stored items. Thus, although spatial attention is a persistent partner of visual working memory, it is functionally dissociable from the selection and maintenance of individuated representations in working memory. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
23. Working memory and fluid intelligence: Capacity, attention control, and secondary memory retrieval.
- Author
-
Unsworth, Nash, Fukuda, Keisuke, Awh, Edward, and Vogel, Edward K.
- Subjects
- *
SHORT-term memory , *FLUID intelligence , *ATTENTION control , *MEMORIZATION , *RECOLLECTION (Psychology) , *MEMORY , *PSYCHOLOGY - Abstract
Highlights: [•] The relation between working memory and fluid intelligence was examined. [•] Capacity, attention control, and secondary memory composed working memory. [•] The three factors mediated the working memory-fluid intelligence relation. [•] Each factor accounted for shared and unique variance in fluid intelligence. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
24. Selection and Storage of Perceptual Groups Is Constrained by a Discrete Resource in Working Memory.
- Author
-
Anderson, David E., Vogel, Edward K., and Awh, Edward
- Subjects
- *
SHORT-term memory , *INDIVIDUAL differences , *ELECTROPHYSIOLOGY , *EXPERIMENTAL psychology - Abstract
Perceptual grouping can lead observers to perceive a multielement scene as a smaller number of hierarchical units. Past work has shown that grouping enables more elements to be stored in visual working memory (WM). Although this may appear to contradict so-called discrete resource models that argue for fixed item limits in WM storage, it is also possible that grouping reduces the effective number of "items" in the display. To test this hypothesis, we examined how mnemonic resolution declined as the number of items to be stored increased. Discrete resource models predict that precision will reach a stable plateau at relatively early set sizes, because no further items can be stored once putative item limits are exceeded. Thus, we examined whether the precision by set size function was bilinear when storage was enhanced via perceptual grouping. In line with the hypothesis that each perceptual group counted as a single "item," precision still reached a clear plateau at a set size determined by the number of stored groups. Moreover, the maximum number of elements stored was doubled, and electrophysiological measures showed that selection and storage-related neural responses were the same for a single element and a multielement perceptual group. Thus, perceptual grouping allows more elements to be held in working memory while storage is still constrained by a discrete item limit. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
25. Discrete Resource Allocation in Visual Working Memory.
- Author
-
Barton, Brian, Ester, Edward F., and Awh, Edward
- Subjects
- *
RESOURCE allocation , *MEMORY , *SHORT-term memory , *RECOLLECTION (Psychology) , *INTELLECT - Abstract
Are resources in visual working memory allocated in a continuous or a discrete fashion? On one hand, flexible resource models suggest that capacity is determined by a central resource pool that can be flexibly divided such that items of greater complexity receive a larger share of resources. On the other hand, if capacity in working memory is defined in terms of discrete storage "slots," then observers may be able to determine which items are assigned to a slot but not how resources are divided between stored items. To test these predictions, the authors manipulated the relative complexity of the items to be stored while holding the number items constant. Although mnemonic resolution declined when set size increased (Experiment 1), resolution for a given item was unaffected by large variations in the complexity of the other items to be stored when set size was held constant (Experiments 2-4). Thus, resources in visual working memory are distributed in a discrete slot-based fashion, even when interitem variations in complexity motivate an asymmetrical division of resources across items. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2009
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
26. Alpha-Band Activity Reveals Spontaneous Representations of Spatial Position in Visual Working Memory.
- Author
-
Foster, Joshua J., Bsales, Emma M., Jaffe, Russell J., and Awh, Edward
- Subjects
- *
VISUAL memory , *ELECTROENCEPHALOGRAPHY , *SHORT-term memory , *LONG-term memory , *OSCILLATIONS - Abstract
Summary An emerging view suggests that spatial position is an integral component of working memory (WM), such that non-spatial features are bound to locations regardless of whether space is relevant [ 1, 2 ]. For instance, past work has shown that stimulus position is spontaneously remembered when non-spatial features are stored. Item recognition is enhanced when memoranda appear at the same location where they were encoded [ 3–5 ], and accessing non-spatial information elicits shifts of spatial attention to the original position of the stimulus [ 6, 7 ]. However, these findings do not establish that a persistent, active representation of stimulus position is maintained in WM because similar effects have also been documented following storage in long-term memory [ 8, 9 ]. Here we show that the spatial position of the memorandum is actively coded by persistent neural activity during a non-spatial WM task. We used a spatial encoding model in conjunction with electroencephalogram (EEG) measurements of oscillatory alpha-band (8–12 Hz) activity to track active representations of spatial position. The position of the stimulus varied trial to trial but was wholly irrelevant to the tasks. We nevertheless observed active neural representations of the original stimulus position that persisted throughout the retention interval. Further experiments established that these spatial representations are dependent on the volitional storage of non-spatial features rather than being a lingering effect of sensory energy or initial encoding demands. These findings provide strong evidence that online spatial representations are spontaneously maintained in WM—regardless of task relevance—during the storage of non-spatial features. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.