15,662 results on '"INDUCTION"'
Search Results
2. The Role of Discourse in Mathematical Inquiry.
- Author
-
McGuire, Mary and Harshman, Karen
- Abstract
This paper describes a collaborative research study designed to determine whether well-constructed tasks in mathematical inquiry with the use of methods in dialogic discourse help students' retention of mathematical concepts in probability. The paper reports on an experiment conducted in two classrooms in an urban school district in the Pacific Northwest. One teacher taught a fourth and fifth grade combination in a low-income area, and the other taught a second grade class in a middle to high socioeconomic status area. Over a period of 2 weeks, students were introduced to activities using penny flips and spinners designed to engage students in inductive learning methods on probability. Dialogic discourse was used to engage students in conversations about the results of their activities. Interviews were conducted of observers regarding their perceptions of student learning from the discourse in which the students engaged. Two weeks later, students responded to a written prompt asking them to describe the activities and what they had learned from them. Responses varied in depth and specificity, but all students responded positively to at least one of the concepts taught using the vocabulary of probability indicating that the activity or the discourse of a combination of the two resulted in retention of a portion of the mathematical concepts presented to the children. (Author/SLD)
- Published
- 2002
3. Assessment of Rowan University's Peer Referral Orientation Staff.
- Author
-
Majnerick, Troy G.
- Abstract
An evaluation was conducted of the Peer Referral Orientation Staff (PROS) program of Rowan University to determine the extent to which the program is meeting its desired goals and objectives and achieving the desired effects for the students it serves. PROS members are currently enrolled students who are trained to offer personal, social, and educational services to their peers. A survey was constructed to assess the program's effectiveness, and this survey was administered to a convenience sample of 30 undergraduate students. Most students (63%) thought that the PROS significantly helped incoming students understand the purpose of higher education and that the program assisted them in developing positive relationships with faculty, administration, peers, and other individuals in the community. Students generally agreed that the program helped them develop familiarity with the physical surroundings of the university and that it introduced academic, advising, and registration procedures at Rowan University well. The qualitative analysis of student comments showed that there are areas the PROS program needs to address to assist students after the initial orientation period. Room for improvement is noted for the first year experience in particular. (Contains 13 figures, 1 table, and 13 references.) (SLD)
- Published
- 2002
4. Rising Political Consciousness: Transformational Learning in Malaysia.
- Author
-
Kamis, Mazalan and Muhamad, Mazanah
- Abstract
As part of a larger study (not discussed) ten educated Malaysian citizens were interviewed to find whether their rising political consciousness, over a ten year period (1988-1999), indicated that their transformation was influenced by their culture. The subjects were between 35-45 years old, married, with an average of four children. All were members of the majority race, Malay, and held membership in the United Malay National Organization (UMNO), the biggest political party in Malaysia. The following research questions guided the study: (1) What triggered individuals' transformations; and (2) what facilitated the transformation to occur. Voting patterns in the 10th General Election of 1999 were meshed with interview transcripts. Two themes emerged from the data: (1) abhorrence of political scandals and upheavals during 1998-1999; and (2) a feeling of being intellectually demeaned (lied to by government authorities). The major conclusion of the study was that a political event could trigger perspective shifts and lead to transformative learning if it is perceived as threatening to the communal cultural identity. A second conclusion was that the methods used to handle the national crisis were against participants' cultural and religious values. The researchers believe the study contributed to theoretical explanations of transformative learning among individuals in a developing country. (The bibliography lists 28 references). (AJ)
- Published
- 2002
5. Effects of Multiple Sources of Information on Induction in Young Children.
- Author
-
Lo, Yafen and Sloutsky, Vladimir M.
- Abstract
This study considered differences in induction of biological properties between children and preadolescents based on differences in stimuli processing in these two groups. Two experiments tested predictions that young children, but not preadolescents, base their inductive inference on aggregating information from different sources rather than relying on a single source of information. In both experiments, children and preadolescents from three age ranges were presented with an inductive task involving triads of animal pictures: a target baby animal, and two test animals, one a "neighbor" animal who "played with the baby," and an animal who "gave birth to the baby." In experiment 1, linguistic labels were fully crossed with relationship information, whereas in experiment 2 perceptual similarity information was fully crossed with relationship and labeling information. Findings indicated that while 10- and 11-year-olds relied exclusively on inheritance (when target animal shared only inheritance information and not labeling with the mother) across experiments, 4- and 5-year-olds relied on an aggregate of multiple sources of information, and 7- and 8-year-olds fell between these 2 extremes. In addition, while the relative weight of inheritance on inferences increased with age, the weights of other information sources decreased. The results support the hypotheses suggesting that between 8 and 10 years of age, children undergo a developmental shift from a holistic feature-integration induction to knowledge-based induction based on a single most predictive source. (Contains 20 references.) (HTH)
- Published
- 2001
6. Markedness in Intercultural Discourse: A Study of Chinese EFL Students' Discourse Patterns.
- Author
-
Chen, Jianping
- Abstract
One of the major issues in intercultural discourse studies is the variation of discourse patterns across cultures. It has been reported that there appears to be a Western preference for a deductive pattern and a Chinese preference for an inductive pattern in discourse. However, it has also been pointed out that there is nothing inherently Chinese or Western in either of these patterns, because both are used in all societies. The purpose of this study is to address the variations between Chinese and British/American preference for the inductive versus deductive patterns of discourse through a description of Chinese English-as-a-Foreign-Language (EFL) students and native speakers of English towards these patterns. The intent of this investigation is to provide a basis for further research on the Chinese versus Western preference for discourse patterns from a markedness point of view and the development of an explanatory model of markedness for intercultural discourse. The study was conducted through a content analysis of the discourse structures of 363 English language essays written by Chinese EFL students and an investigation of the evaluative attitudes of Chinese and Western informants towards different discourse styles and structures. A statistical analysis of the results confirms that Chinese EFL students generally prefer an inductive pattern in written discourse in English. Included are 15 tables and figures; three appendices--Description of the Structural Types of the Essays, The Questionnaire of the Study, Sample Copies of the Original Essays; and 112 English and Chinese language references. (KFT)
- Published
- 2000
7. A Conceptualization of Mixed Methods: A Need for Inductive/Deductive Approach to Conducting Research.
- Author
-
Newman, Isadore
- Abstract
This paper provides examples of how one can use the research issue and the relationships between qualitative and quantitative research as a frame for instructing students and judging the quality of research. The emphasis is on validity estimates, also called legitimization techniques, with attention to the idea of a qualitative-quantitative research continuum. It is not the technique that makes something quantitative or qualitative, but rather the intention of its use. The question is posed whether the research is testing hypotheses, helping to develop hypotheses, or describing the data. Criteria have been developed by Y. Lincoln and E. Guba (1985) for the validity of qualitative research so that it can be evaluated in a way similar to the more common evaluation of quantitative research. In fact, much of all research is a blend of quantitative and qualitative approaches. The more aware researchers are of this fact, the more likely it is that standards of good and effective research can be established. (Contains 21 references.) (SLD)
- Published
- 2000
8. Reading and the Structure of the English Language.
- Author
-
Ediger, Marlow
- Abstract
Understanding the structure of the English language can assist learners in getting the feeling for what comes next sequentially in oral or silent reading. Noting that the structure of knowledge movement during the 1960s-1970s emphasized the selection of what is relevant and important to teach and that pupils learn ideas inductively, this paper suggests some enjoyable ways to have pupils learn key ideas in English as it relates to reading instruction. The paper states, that to emphasize the structure of knowledge approach in learning, the teacher should: have an excellent knowledge of major generalizations in sentence patterns; sequence learning opportunities in which individuals experience the enactive (objects and items), the iconic (semi-concrete materials of instruction), and the symbolic (abstract ideas); appraise pupils to ascertain how many of these structural ideas are being attained by pupils in a spiral curriculum; and use enactive, iconic, and symbolic materials in inductive approaches to learning. The paper then focuses on a detailed discussion of sentence patterns, including procedures to use in evaluating pupil achievement pertaining to knowledge and skills in the structure or patterns of sentences. It concludes that teachers need to spend more time emphasizing to pupils how learning the structure of language is practical in reading and writing across the curriculum. (NKA)
- Published
- 1999
9. Using the Science Misconceptions Research To Address Science Teaching Misconceptions.
- Author
-
Weber, Suzanne
- Abstract
This study describes how the lesson plans of preservice teachers differed from the inductive learning cycle planning model, and relates these differences to persistent naive conceptions about effective science pedagogy held by preservice teachers. Strategies based on the science misconceptions literature that methods instructors can use to encourage the understanding and use of inductive learning cycle instruction by beginning teachers are suggested. The study concludes that preservice teachers majoring in secondary science education like and prefer traditional teaching methods while elementary and middle school preservice teachers find traditional methods boring and ineffective. (Contains 14 references.) (YDS)
- Published
- 1999
10. Dynamic Assessment of Inductive Reasoning: A Test of Induced versus Imposed Mediation.
- Author
-
Dillon, Ronna F.
- Abstract
The importance of dynamic testing procedures for understanding reasoning and its underlying cognitive processes as well as for increasing validity is investigated. An experiment tested the premise that African American and White college students demonstrate equivalent inductive reasoning ability when tested under induced mediation procedures, where they are encouraged to structure their own thought processes. Subject verbalization procedures foster induced mediation, while experimenter verbalization foster imposed mediation. Participants (77 White and 45 African American undergraduates) performed better under the two dynamic conditions than under standard procedures. In addition, an interaction was found between ethnic group membership and testing condition. White participants performed equally well under either dynamic condition, while African American participants performed better under subject-directed, induced mediation than under experimenter-directed imposed mediation. As hypothesized, no differences are found between African American and White college students under induced mediation procedures. Also, validity data are consistent with earlier findings that point to the importance of considering the role of learner attributes in testing for elucidating intelligence or ability. Previous reports of White college undergraduates outperforming their African American peers on tests of general intellectual ability are due to a greater incompatibility between demands of the testing situation and preferred processing tactics for African American participants than for White participants. (Contains 1 table and 38 references.) (Author/SLD)
- Published
- 1998
11. Integrating Elements of Inquiry into the Flow of Middle Level Teaching.
- Author
-
Flick, Lawrence B.
- Abstract
This paper is a part of a research program whose purpose it is to design instruction for scaffolding classroom inquiry in middle school classrooms. Scaffolding is a dynamic process, reflecting teacher adjustments based on student responses. Even though a computer, textbook, or laboratory materials may serve as proxy for a "teacher", arguably the most important source of scaffolding in a classroom is the flesh and blood teacher. The teacher decides, consciously or unconsciously, how and when to use a computer, textbook, or laboratory materials. The actions of the teacher are also the primary mediator of the scaffolding effects of other classroom materials. The purpose of inquiry-based instruction is to prompt focused effort on a specific problem. The effort includes recall and application of relevant knowledge and implementation of procedures for generating, analyzing, and interpreting data. Mental effort for inquiry requires evaluation of the fit among a problem statement, recalled knowledge, and evidence either empirical or theoretical. This type of thinking requires self-monitoring of one's understanding of the problem and processes undertaking to solve it. Middle level students are just beginning to show the capabilities for the kinds of thought necessary for scientific inquiry. However, most of the evidence for these thinking skills is clinically based (Keating, 1990). Classroom-based reasoning situations do not afford the time, focus, and cues for employing critical thinking. Cognitive scaffolding is necessary to support student thinking necessary for benefiting from inquiry-based instruction. Cognitive scaffolding has been defined as what a teacher does when working with a student "to solve a problem, carry out a task, or achieve a goal which would be beyond his unassisted efforts: (Wood, Bruner, & Ross, 1976, p.90). As a psychological construct, it refers to a cognitive orientation in the teacher to select and structure tasks, communicate purpose, and hold expectations with the intention of guiding student work at the limits of their independent capabilities. Scaffolding also refers to an affective orientation in the teacher that is sensitive to variations in student cognitive and affective capabilities that results in adjusting elements of the task or context that promotes continued student effort. For scaffolding to work, however, there must be a complementary cognitive and affective orientation in the student. The student must be willing to apply existing knowledge and make active use of the teacher or other resources to leverage that knowledge toward a learning goal. Student affect must be willing to accept less than immediate gratification and persevere in the task. (Contains 17 references.) (Author/YDS)
- Published
- 1998
12. Components of Self-Regulation during Within- and Between-Domain Problem-Solving Performance.
- Author
-
Phye, Gary D.
- Abstract
Self-regulation components linked to academic problem-solving were studied. Cognitive process instruction on analogy problems was given to 63 undergraduates on a Monday. On Wednesday, problem-solving performance within the inductive reasoning domain was assessed with 30 Remote Associate Test (RAT) problems. On Friday, problem-solving performance between the inductive and deductive problem-solving task of 20 categorical syllogism problems was assessed. Results indicate that both within- and between-domain problem-solving performance is influenced by knowledge acquired prior to practice. In both cases, this influence is indirect, being mediated by the learner's ability to monitor working memory during practice. Self-efficacy assessed during practice is directly linked to within-domain, but not between-domain problem solving. On the other hand, eductive mental activity directly influenced between-domain, but not within-domain problem-solving performance in this study. Exploratory path analysis can provide insight into the relative roles played by selected cognitive-motivational components of self-regulation when learners are required to adopt to changing demands during problem solving. (Contains one figure, three tables, and four references.) (SLD)
- Published
- 1998
13. Effects of Inductive Multimedia Programs Including Graphs on Creation of Linear Function and Variable Conceptualization.
- Author
-
Johari, Abbas
- Abstract
This study examined the effects of an inductive multimedia program, including graphs, on subjects' ability to create linear functions and conceptualize variables from word problems. Subjects were 98 undergraduate students in two sections of a computer literacy course. Students' achievements were assessed via pre- and posttests, which were parallel to the instructional themes stated in the treatment programs. Students were randomly assigned to one of two treatment groups to view a version of a self-paced program. Students, regardless of treatment, scored significantly higher on posttest than pretest on both function construction and variable conceptualization. These results may have been influenced by instructional strategies, including: inquiry mathematical thinking, schema training, linked representational systems, and coordinate graph tutorial teaching. Students receiving instruction via the inductive table-and-graph program scored significantly higher on the function construction of the posttest than did students receiving the table-only treatment. Results suggest the use of inductive multimedia program treatments that incorporate many strategies including inquiry learning from data, tutorial, schema, and core representational systems for the problem of translation, specifically creation of linear function. Data specifically suggest that inductive multimedia programs that include the coordinate graph tutorial strategy have a significant effect on the function construction tasks. (Contains 101 references.) (Author/AEF)
- Published
- 1998
14. 'Just Do It': An Inductive, Experiential Method for Teaching Qualitative Data Analysis.
- Author
-
Wolfer, Terry A.
- Abstract
Novices often struggle to learn qualitative data analysis, and benefit from intensive assistance. For teaching analysis more efficiently in the classroom setting, the workshop described in this paper explained and demonstrated an inductive, experiential method. The method introduces grounded theory analysis and helps students to identify, elaborate, and systematize their own approaches. The workshop included selected, abbreviated portions of the instructional process, and provided sample teaching materials. The method centers on a semistructured data analysis exercise that consists of two rounds of activity. Each round includes analysis of the same interview transcript, written reflection during the analytic process, and a guided class discussion. The exercise provides some explicit guidance, but the instructions are deliberately ambiguous at some points to force student choices. The first round provides preliminary experience with data analysis, and the second round builds on the initial learning experience as it elaborates and qualifies the grounded theory method of analysis. Three appendixes present a section of the course syllabus, an inductive analysis exercise, and sample discussion questions. (Contains 10 references.) (SLD)
- Published
- 1998
15. Accelerating Intelligence Development through an Inductive Reasoning Training.
- Author
-
Tomic, Welko and Kingma, Johannes
- Abstract
This study investigated the effects of an inductive reasoning training program on children's performance on intelligence test tasks, the range of transfer, the long-term effects of training over 4 months, and the effectiveness of group training. Participating were 47 third-grade children of average ability. The 23 children randomly assigned to the training condition received a 3-week course in inductive reasoning in ten 30-minute sessions. The training program consisted of six forms of inductive reasoning tasks: (1) generalization; (2) discrimination; (3) cross-classification; (4) recognizing relations; (5) discriminating relations; and (6) system formation. Abstract material was used in 15 percent of the tasks. The remaining 85 percent consisted of either concrete material, such as blocks or picture and figure problems from the children's everyday life. Children were trained in groups of three or four, by six researchers who were not involved in administering the pre- or posttests. Control group classes completed the regular school curriculum. The Raven Coloured Progressive Matrices Test and arithmetic tasks were used as a pretest, immediate posttest, and posttest four months later. Results indicated that there was a significant, positive training effect on children's performance on inductive reasoning tasks. Far-far transfer was also observed, because the children were able to solve arithmetic problems involving relations between numbers and their common attributes which related to inductive reasoning, in which they had received no training. (Contains 43 references.) (Author/KB)
- Published
- 1997
16. The Return of Dangerous Dan: Further Adventures in Recreational Mathematics.
- Author
-
Malmstrom, Jay A.
- Abstract
This paper describes six tricks on different mathematical concepts for mathematics classrooms. The mathematical concepts emphasized in these activities include arithmetic, modular arithmetic, limit cycles, graph theory, pairings, combinatorics, cyclic groups, induction, and sequences. (ASK)
- Published
- 1997
17. Emergence of Abductive Reasoning in Mathematical Problem Solving.
- Author
-
Cifarelli, Victor
- Abstract
This paper examines the novel problem solving actions of a pair of college students. The analysis highlights the role of the solvers' inferential processes including abductions, deductions, and inductions as structuring resources that contribute to both their understanding of the problems they face and the emerging novelty that constitutes their viable solution activity. The purpose of this research is to clarify the processes by which learners construct new knowledge in mathematical problem solving situations, with particular focus on instances where the learner's emerging abductions help to facilitate development of novel problem solution activity. Findings indicate that: (1) abduction is characterized as an ongoing, sense-making process that constitutes the problem solver's source of ideas as to how to proceed when unexpected problems occur; (2) problem solvers' abductions aided their novel explorations, serving to organize and structure their subsequent solution activity; and (3) novelty demonstrated by the problem solvers through their abductive inferences suggests the need to rethink views toward teaching problem solving. Contains 23 references. (DDR)
- Published
- 1997
18. Assessing Grammar Teaching Methods Using a Metacognitive Framework.
- Author
-
Burkhalter, Nancy
- Abstract
A study examined 3 grammar teaching methods to understand why some methods may carry over into writing better than others. E. Bialystok and E. B. Ryan's (1985) metacognitive model of language skills was adapted to plot traditional grammar, sentence combining, and the functional/inductive approach according to the amount of analyzed knowledge and cognitive control each method requires to raise metalinguistic awareness. In so doing, the cognitive demands asked of various kinds of writers by each method can be ascertained. Research results from several studies were analyzed. Results indicated that traditional grammar is not being blended into students' writing because it requires a great deal of cognitive control and analyzed knowledge to deploy. On the other hand, neither sentence combining nor the functional/inductive approach hamstring students with multiple terms and abstract concepts to memorize. Rather, writers focus their attention on some aspect of sentence error and correction by using implicit knowledge, thus keeping the value on the analyzed knowledge axis low. Findings suggest that the highly analyzed system of traditional grammar has had limited success in carrying over into writing because: (1) its goal is to shape mental representations, necessitating an extended period of time to acquire the system; (2) control is possible only after the writer knows the system; and (3) overemphasis on that skill can undermine the production side of things. (Contains 3 figures and 37 references; notes are appended.) (Author/CR)
- Published
- 1997
19. Issues in the Teaching of Grammar in a Foreign Language.
- Author
-
Weatherford, H. Jarold
- Abstract
A number of issues in classroom second language instruction, particularly as they relate to grammar instruction, are considered in the context of recent research and theory. Discussion begins with a review of the nature and role of second language grammar instruction. Following this, these issues are explored: whether the native or target language should be used in explaining or discussing grammar; whether the inductive or deductive approach should be adopted; whether students can read basic grammar rules on their own, or need teacher intervention; whether grammar should occupy a central or more subordinate role in the classroom; how error correction should be used; whether students enjoy grammar and consider it necessary; and what constitutes grammar. The paper concludes that grammar is a necessary component of second language instruction, not to be either the primary focus of instruction or relegated to a status of unimportance, but viewed as a tool for development of communicative competence. Contains 31 references. (MSE)
- Published
- 1997
20. A Logic Programming Testbed for Inductive Thought and Specification.
- Author
-
Neff, Norman D.
- Abstract
This paper describes applications of logic programming technology to the teaching of the inductive method in computer science and mathematics. It discusses the nature of inductive thought and its place in those fields of inquiry, arguing that a complete logic programming system for supporting inductive inference is not only feasible but necessary. A sample dialog from the Prologb system is included, along with an overview of the Prologb language and some details about classroom experiences using the system. (Author/BEW)
- Published
- 1995
21. Training in Inductive Reasoning.
- Author
-
Tomic, Welko
- Abstract
This study investigated the effects of K. J. Klauer's (1989) inductive reasoning training program of teaching children. Effects of training and the range of transfer of the training were assessed. The subjects were 34 third-grade Dutch children of average ability, matched on age, sex, and IQ. Children from the training condition (N=17) received one week training of five 30 minute sessions. Results demonstrated a significant, positive effect on training children's performance of inductive reasoning tasks. A near-far transfer was observed with children able to solve tasks not taught in training. These effects persisted for 4 months. Far-far transfer was not observed because children were not able to solve mathematics tasks that relate to inductive reasoning, for which they had received no training. Implications for training children within the context of regular schools and the range of transfer are discussed. (EH)
- Published
- 1994
22. Teaching Thinking across the Curriculum with the Concept Attainment Model.
- Author
-
Pritchard, Florence Fay
- Abstract
The Concept Attainment Model is described as a teaching approach that helps students develop skills for inductive and deductive thinking while learning subject matter in any field in a constructive and meaningful way. A definition and overview of the model are presented, with guidelines for using the concept-attainment approach to design and deliver instruction and to evaluate student learning of subject matter and thinking skills. The model is an instructional approach in which teachers guide students to derive an abstract, generic idea inductively using pattern recognition and categorizing skills, and then help them deductively apply the concept in new situations. The theory of concepts of Jerome Bruner is the source of the model, which was transformed into a teaching model by B. Joyce and M. Weil (1972, 1980, 1986). Planning for teaching with the concept attainment model involves: (1) identifying a significant and definable concept; (2) analyzing its essential and defining features; and (3) designing exemplars from which the concept can be derived. The model offers teachers a method for teaching thinking across the curriculum using the subject matter of disciplines they teach. Five figures illustrate the discussion. (Contains 10 references.) (SLD)
- Published
- 1994
23. Beyond Inductive and Deductive Reasoning: The Search for a Sense of Knowing.
- Author
-
Simon, Martin A.
- Abstract
In this paper it is argued that the characterization of mathematical justifications as inductive or deductive is incomplete. Promoting in classrooms the development of and discourse about a third type of reasoning, transformational reasoning, which considers the results of operations on a set of objects, may contribute to mathematics learning in significant ways. Topics discussed include: contrasting the presence and absence of transformational reasoning, defining transformational reasoning, transformational reasoning as a way of thinking, and implications. Contains 19 references. (MKR)
- Published
- 1994
24. Abduction? Deduction? Induction? Is There a Logic of Exploratory Data Analysis?
- Author
-
Yu, Chong Ho
- Abstract
The philosophical notions introduced by Charles Sanders Peirce (1839-1914) are helpful for researchers in understanding the nature of knowledge and reality. In the Peircean logical system, the logic of abduction and deduction contribute to our conceptual understanding of a phenomenon, while the logic of induction adds quantitative details to the conceptual knowledge. Although Peirce justified the validity of induction as a self-corrective process, he asserted that neither induction nor deduction can help us to unveil the internal structure of meaning. As exploratory data analysis performs the function of a model builder for confirmatory data analysis, abduction plays the role of explorer of viable paths to further inquiry. Thus, the logic of abduction fits well into exploratory data analysis. At the stage of abduction, the goal is to explore the data, find out a pattern, and suggest a plausible hypothesis; deduction is to refine the hypothesis based upon other plausible premises; and induction is the empirical substantiation. (Contains 55 references.) (Author)
- Published
- 1994
25. The Micro Inductive-Reasoning Unit within the Basic Speech Course.
- Author
-
Bohlken, Robert
- Abstract
An instructional approach to reasoning and critical thinking which fulfills behavioral objectives and retains a general educational perspective for the development of student knowledge and skills is the "micro inductive reasoning unit." In this approach neither subject matter nor propositions are prescribed. The teacher acts as facilitator in the implementation of the methods in the unit. The class begins the unit by trying to isolate problems which are relevant and immediate for class members. The class is divided into small groups of four to six students. Each group takes a topic from those that evolved and the members decide the best way to phrase a question that can be analyzed according to causes and solutions. Groups exchange questions and go through the process of problem-solution discussion. Students record consensus on the nature of the problem, causes, possible solutions, and advantages and disadvantages of each solution. The statement of the best solution becomes the proposition for debate. Each group divides into two groups of two or three members. One group now becomes the advocate of the proposition and argues for its adoption. The inductive reasoning unit in the basic speech course enables the fulfillment of legitimate behavioral and competitive argument. (Diagrams illustrating the micro-inductive approach and a chart describing parts of the unit are included.) (RS)
- Published
- 1993
26. Implicit Theories about Everyday Problem Solving.
- Author
-
Herbert, Margaret E. and Dionne, Jean-Paul
- Abstract
Mental models or implicit theories held by adults about everyday problem solving were studied. Research questions were posed to 12 male and 12 female adults, aged 25 to 60 years, from a wide range of educational and occupational orientations. Subjects were interviewed in pairs. Verbal Protocol Analysis was used to analyze the data from two distinct vantages: first through a content analysis of specific responses to the interview questions, and then through the frequency of responses obtained from the application of a coding grid developed for the study. A generic model was derived and applied to recuperate and elaborate the emerging mental models. The integrated framework on induction proposed by J. H. Holland and others (1986) was adopted as a conceptual framework for the study by virtue of its use for ill-defined problems and its application to social science research. Results identify the predominance of the interpersonal and emotional factors salient to this genre of problem solving. Individual differences, communication, and problem identification are highlighted as the most significant and difficult areas on which success was contingent. Degrees of self-confidence and habitual usage of a system of method were seen to be significant factors, often correlated with experience and education. Two tables and two figures illustrate the study. (Author/SLD)
- Published
- 1993
27. The Dark and Bloody Mystery: Building Basic Writers' Confidence.
- Author
-
Sledd, Robert
- Abstract
While the roots of students' fear of writing go deep, students fear most the surface of writing. They fear that a person's language indicates the state not only of the mind but of the soul--thus their writing can make them look stupid and morally depraved. This fear of error and lack of confidence prevent students from developing a command of the written language and results in superficial treatment of superficial ideas. Using an inductive approach to analyzing writing can help students conquer the fear of surface errors that prevents them from tackling complex ideas in their work. An inductive approach which involves class discussion that analyzes samples of strong and weak writing has several benefits: (1) disagreement about which revision the class prefers undermines the notion that writing is "right" or "wrong," good" or "bad"; (2) students get reinforcement of the idea that writing involves choices among multiple alternatives, not a binary choice between "right" and "wrong"; (3) describing differences among alternative revisions and the original dispells the notions that the mysteries of writing are inaccessible to students; (4) discussion and description of the paragraphs make it necessary to get concepts like subject, verb, dependent clause under control; (5) the frequency of surface errors in student writing decreases; and (6) students begin to do more substantive stylistic revision to their own writing with the result that the content of their essays also improves. (Handouts intended to aid students in analyzing their own own and others' writing are presented in four attached appendixes.) (SAM)
- Published
- 1993
28. Learning Processes and Learning Outcomes.
- Author
-
Armstrong Lab, Brooks AFB, TX. Human Resources Directorate. and Shute, Valerie J.
- Abstract
This paper summarizes our present knowledge and understanding of the processes and outcomes of learning. The basic idea about learning is that the outcomes of learning (e.g., propositional knowledge, procedural skills, and mental models) reflect differences in learning processes (e.g., encoding skills, attention allocation, and hypothesis generation). Additionally, learning outcomes reflect differences in conative processes, knowledge structures, and metacognitive skills, mediated by the learning processes. Against the background of a brief historical introduction, this article presents a research-based overview of the major categories of educationally relevant learning outcomes and of the underlying acquisition processes. In this perspective, different learning environments are discussed such as learning by direct instruction, drill and practice, and discovery. Finally, implications for the design of computerized instructional environments are indicated. (Contains 9 figures and 83 references.) (Author)
- Published
- 1992
29. Inductive Reasoning in the Secondary Classroom.
- Author
-
National Education Association, Washington, DC., Neubert, Gloria A., and Binko, James B.
- Abstract
It has been alleged that few American students can use their knowledge effectively in thinking and reasoning. This study urges teachers to give more attention to student abilities in analyzing, classifying, comparing, formulating hypotheses, and drawing conclusions--that is, thinking skills essential to reasoning processes. Designed to familiarize secondary classroom teachers with one model of instruction for developing students' reasoning abilities, this book gives practical assistance in learning how to use the inductive approach, a teaching approach that actively involves students in the use of their own reasoning while learning content area material. Each chapter of the book includes a section that asks readers to recall experience pertinent to the material in the chapter, a set of questions to answer as the material is read, and a series of activities designed to lead readers through the developmental stages of the learning process. Chapters of the book are: (1) What is the inductive approach? (2) Why use the inductive approach? (3) How to use the inductive approach for concept development; (4) How to use the inductive approach for principle formation; (5) Inductive reasoning in English; (6) Inductive reasoning in mathematics; (7) Inductive reasoning in science; and (8) Inductive reasoning in social studies. A 90-item bibliography is attached. (RS)
- Published
- 1992
30. A Comparison of Learning Environments: All That Glitters... Interim Technical Paper for Period January 1990-July 1991.
- Author
-
Armstrong Lab, Brooks AFB, TX. Human Resources Directorate. and Shute, Valerie J.
- Abstract
Aptitude-treatment interactions (ATIs) refer to the covariation between learner characteristic and instructional treatment in relation to some outcome measure. To systematically test for ATI, an intelligent tutoring system instructing in basic principles of electricity was chosen as a complex but controlled learning task. Two learning environments were created from this one tutor, differing only in feedback. In the rule-application environment, the system provided learners with relevant principles, and in the rule-induction environment, learners had to induce principles on their own. Subjects were 309 adult high school graduates approached through a temporary employment agency. The learner characteristic examined was exploratory behavior, a measure of on-line tool usage. It was hypothesized that exploratory learners would learn faster and better if they had been assigned to the inductive environment and fewer exploratory learners would benefit from the more structured application environment. Results show significant aptitude-treatment interactions in the data, confirming the hypothesis. Implications are discussed in relation to the design of intelligent tutoring systems. Eleven figures and six tables illustrate the discussion and present study findings. (Contains 30 references.) (SLD)
- Published
- 1992
31. The Induction of Rules from Analog, Mental Models.
- Author
-
Schwartz, Daniel L. and Black, John B.
- Abstract
This study investigated how people reason about simple mechanical devices and physical systems, and how reasoning methods and understanding of a device evolve over a period of exposure. Twelve students attending the Teachers College at Columbia University (New York) participated in the first of two experiments; and 10 students attending the same Teachers College participated in the second experiment and were randomly assigned to five dyads. The first experiment used a quantified protocol in which spontaneous hand movements were considered evidence of modeling; dramatic reductions in reaction time were evidence of rule induction. The second experiment organized subjects into problem-solving dyads. The inductive movement between analog models and number-based rules was documented for problems about gear movement. It was found that subjects rely initially upon an analog model until a satisfactory rule is induced. However, when a problem was introduced that led to failure of the rule, subjects returned to a model. Exophoric references were used as evidence that subjects were reasoning about models. Numeric expressions were taken as evidence of rule induction under the logic that the rule operates over numbers rather than gears. The strengths of each type of reasoning are discussed. Models are conceptualized as a quasi-empirical base from which to draw the basic patterns and relevant parameters for rule induction. Three figures are included. (TJH)
- Published
- 1990
32. The California Critical Thinking Skills Test--College Level. Technical Report #4. Interpreting the CCTST, Group Norms, and Sub-Scores.
- Author
-
Facione, Peter A.
- Abstract
Group norms are provided for the California Critical Thinking Skills Test (CCTST)--College Level, a standardized 34-item multiple-choice test designed to assess the core critical thinking skills associated with baccalaureate general education. The CCTST offers three subtests conceptualized in terms of a national Delphi study on critical thinking. These three subtests--analysis, evaluation, and inference--correlate strongly with each other and the overall CCTST when used as either a pretest or posttest. Subtests are also offered based on the more traditional division of reasoning into "deductive reasoning" and "inductive reasoning." These latter two subtests also correlate strongly with each other and the overall CCTST when used as either a pretest or posttest. Statistical analyses, correlations, and recommended percentile rankings for raw scores are presented in nine tables. These norms were developed on the basis of analyses of 1,673 test forms for representative samples of college students at a comprehensive urban state university during the 1989-90 school year. (SLD)
- Published
- 1990
33. Program Development and Evaluation: A Modeling Process.
- Author
-
Genesee Community Coll., Batavia, NY., Green, Donald W., and Corgiat, RayLene
- Abstract
A model of program development and evaluation was developed at Genesee Community College, utilizing a system theory/process of deductive and inductive reasoning to ensure coherence and continuity within the program. The model links activities to specific measurable outcomes. Evaluation checks and feedback are built in at various levels so that program changes can be made without redesigning the whole program. The model consists of the following six steps: (1) define the purpose of the course or program; (2) identify three to six general activities which will achieve the defined purpose; (3) break down each activity to three to six more specific activities, making sure that the outcomes remain consistent; (4) continue to breakdown the activities to new levels of specificity until all activities are clearly defined and understood; (5) identify all input and output at each level of activity; (6) determine relationships among the activities; and (7) identify constraints, such as attitudes, policies, and funding, which influence outcomes. The completed model should be checked to ensure that activities and outcomes are properly matched at each level and that all outcomes are clearly defined in measurable, behavioral terms. Graphic depictions of these steps are appended. (WJT)
- Published
- 1990
34. Keeping the Show on the Road: The Role of the Acting Headteacher.
- Author
-
Draper, Janet and McMichael, Paquita
- Abstract
Study of acting heads' situations, experience, and support in the position. Uncertainty on length of appointment, role boundaries, and freedom to innovate led to some dissatisfaction and unwillingness to apply for permanent positions. Findings also raise issues for the maintenance of school performance in times of change and uncertainty. (Contains 16 references.) (Authors/PKP)
- Published
- 2003
35. The Effects of Deductive and Inductive Instruction on the Acquisition of Direct Object Pronouns in French as a Second Language.
- Author
-
Erlam, Rosemary
- Abstract
Presents results from a recent study that isolated grammar instruction that is deductive as a variable and contrasted it with an instructional treatment that is inductive. The effectiveness of these two types of instruction was compared on measures of both comprehension and production. Highlights the difficulty of designing language measures that access implicit language knowledge. (Author/VWL)
- Published
- 2003
36. EAP or TEAP?
- Author
-
Todd, Richard Watson
- Abstract
Identifies six key approaches to the teaching of English for academic purposes (EAP): inductive learning, process syllabuses, learner autonomy, authenticity, technology, and team teaching. Reasons for the emphasis on these approaches are given. Concludes by arguing that a greater emphasis needs to be placed on methodology in EAP. (VWL)
- Published
- 2003
37. Brief Report: Pitocin Induction in Autistic and Nonautistic Individuals.
- Author
-
Gale, Susan, Ozonoff, Sally, and Lainhart, Janet
- Abstract
A study examined rates of labor induction using pitocin in 41 boys with autism and 25 matched controls with either typical development or mental retardation. There were no differences in pitocin induction rates as a function of either diagnostic group (autism vs. control) or IQ level (average vs. subaverage range). (Contains references.) (Author/CR)
- Published
- 2003
38. Content Effects in Self-Directed Inductive Learning.
- Author
-
Wilhelm, P. and Beishuizen, J. J.
- Abstract
Studied the effects of familiar and abstract task content on self-directed inductive learning with 2 groups of college students (n=50). As expected, learning outcome was higher in the group performing the concrete task. Discusses reasons for the content effect. (SLD)
- Published
- 2003
39. Pattern Induction by Infant Language Learners.
- Author
-
Saffran, Jenny R. and Thiessen, Erik D.
- Abstract
In three experiments, 9-month-olds were given the opportunity to induce specific phonological patterns from manipulated syllable structure, consonant voicing position, and segmental position. Infants were then familiarized with fluent speech containing words that either fit or violated these patterns. Subsequent testing revealed that infants rapidly extracted new phonological regularities and that this process was constrained such that some regularities were easier to acquire than others. (Author)
- Published
- 2003
40. The Debate Continues: Further Evidence of Discontinuity in Dewey's Philosophy.
- Author
-
Prawat, Richard S.
- Abstract
Responds to a critique of a thesis that Dewey underwent a dramatic midcareer change in his philosophy and that this change drew heavily on Pierce's metaphysics, offering additional evidence to support the claim that comparison of the 1910 and 1933 versions of "How We Think" reveals a major change in Dewey's views about inductionism. New evidence is also presented to support the claim that this shift occurred at midcareer. (SM)
- Published
- 2003
41. An Inductive Proof for an Identity involving (nk) and the Partial Sums of some Series.
- Author
-
Guo, Bai-Ni and Qi, Feng
- Abstract
In this note involving mathematical induction, an identity involving the combinatorial numbers and the partial sums of the harmonic and related series is verified. (Author)
- Published
- 2002
42. Doctoral Curriculum Studies in an Age of Shifting Boundaries.
- Author
-
Ansaldo, Jim and Goodman, Jesse
- Abstract
Describes faculty and student experiences involving efforts to restructure the curriculum-studies doctoral program at Indiana University in light of shifting and ambiguous field boundaries. (Contains 26 references.) ((PKP)
- Published
- 2002
43. Complexity of Geometric Inductive Reasoning Tasks: Contribution to the Understanding of Fluid Intelligence.
- Author
-
Primi, Ricardo
- Abstract
Created two geometric inductive reasoning matrix tests by manipulating four sources of complexity orthogonally. Results for 313 undergraduates show that fluid intelligence is most strongly associated with the part of the central executive component of working memory that is related to controlled attention processing and selective encoding. (SLD)
- Published
- 2002
44. What Does Galileo's Discovery of Jupiter's Moons Tell Us about the Process of Scientific Discovery?
- Author
-
Lawson, Anton E.
- Abstract
Given that hypothetico-deductive reasoning has played a role in other important scientific discoveries, asks the question whether it plays a role in all important scientific discoveries. Explores and rejects as viable alternatives possible alternative scientific methods such as Baconian induction and combinatorial analysis. Discusses the educational implications of this hypothetico-deductive view of science. (Author/MM)
- Published
- 2002
45. Information Architecture without Internal Theory: An Inductive Design Process.
- Author
-
Haverty, Marsha
- Abstract
Suggests that information architecture design is primarily an inductive process, partly because it lacks internal theory and partly because it is an activity that supports emergent phenomena (user experiences) from basic design components. Suggests a resemblance to Constructive Induction, a design process that locates the best representational framework and identifies a solution. (Author/LRW)
- Published
- 2002
46. Initial Inductive Learning in a Complex Computer Simulated Environment: The Role of Metacognitive Skills and Intellectual Ability.
- Author
-
Veenman, M. V. J., Prins, F. J., and Elshout, J. J.
- Abstract
Discusses conceptual knowledge acquisition using computer simulation and describes a study of undergraduates that examined the role of metacognitive skillfulness and intellectual ability during initial inductive learning with a complex computer simulation. Results showed that metacognitive skillfulness was positively related to learning behavior and to scores on qualitative tests. (Author/LRW)
- Published
- 2002
47. Re-constructing a Painting with Geometric Eyes.
- Author
-
Sinclair, Nathalie
- Abstract
Describes episodes from work with a small group of 8th grade students who were working independently on a geometry course. Uses Geometer's Sketchpad for the tasks. Discusses students' reasoning skills and their interpretation of the painting in terms of the mathematical properties and relationships. (KHR)
- Published
- 2002
48. Heuristic and Analytic Processing: Age Trends and Associations with Cognitive Ability and Cognitive Styles.
- Author
-
Kokis, Judite V., Macpherson, Robyn, Toplak, Maggie E., West, Richard F., and Stanovich, Keith E.
- Abstract
Examined developmental and individual differences in tendencies to favor analytic over heuristic responses in three tasks (inductive reasoning, deduction under belief bias conditions, probabilistic reasoning) in children varying in age and cognitive ability. Found significant increases in analytic responding with development on first two tasks. Cognitive ability related to analytic responding on all tasks. Cognitive style measures explained analytic responding variance after variance shared with cognitive ability had been controlled. (Author/KB)
- Published
- 2002
49. Inductive Reasoning in Zambia, Turkey, and the Netherlands Establishing Cross-Cultural Equivalence.
- Author
-
van de Vijver, Fons J. R.
- Abstract
Administered tasks of inductive reasoning to 704 Zambian, 877 Turkish, and 632 Dutch students from the highest 2 grades of primary and the lowest 2 grades of secondary school. Results show strong evidence for structural equivalence and partial evidence for measurement unit equivalence, but did not support full score equivalence. (SLD)
- Published
- 2002
50. Induction in a Modular Learner.
- Author
-
Carroll, Susanne E.
- Abstract
Presents a theory of inductive learning--Autonomous Induction Theory--a form of induction that takes place within the autonomous and modular representational systems of the language faculty. Argues that Autonomous Induction Theory is constrained enough to be taken seriously as a plausible approach to explaining second language acquisition. (Author/VWL)
- Published
- 2002
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.