44 results on '"Fuat Balcı"'
Search Results
2. Metric error monitoring for a cleaner record of timing
- Author
-
Ece Yallak and Fuat Balcı
- Subjects
Judgment ,Behavioral Neuroscience ,Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,Time Perception ,Humans ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Prospective Studies ,Metacognition ,Feedback - Abstract
A recent line of research has shown that humans can keep track of the direction and magnitude of their timing errors without relying on feedback. But these studies tested temporal error monitoring explicitly by interrogating participants regarding their errors, which might have inadvertently primed the prospective coupling between the first-order timing and second-order metacognitive judgments. The current study utilized an indirect way of testing temporal error awareness while providing a strong objective incentive for maximizing the accuracy of first-order timing performance. In two experiments, participants were asked to maximize the average proximity of their time reproduction to the target by accepting or rejecting their time reproduction depending on the subjective judgment of their proximity to the target time interval. We found that participants more frequently opted out of a trial with a larger distance between their reproductions and the target time interval in both directions, forming a positive quadratic relationship with reproduced time. Resultantly, timing precision was lower in trials that participants opted out of. Our results provide new evidence in support of the temporal error-monitoring performance of human participants. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2022 APA, all rights reserved).
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
3. Mice are Near Optimal Timers
- Author
-
Ezgi Gür, Alihan Erdağı, and Fuat Balcı
- Subjects
Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Applied Psychology - Abstract
Many conventional interval timing tasks do not contain asymmetric cost (loss) functions and thereby favor high temporal accuracy. On the other hand, asymmetric cost functions that differentially penalize/reinforce the early or late responses result in adaptive biases (shift) in timed responses due to timing uncertainty. Consequently, optimal performance in these tasks entails the normative parametrization of adaptive timing biases by the level of timing uncertainty. Differential reinforcement of response duration (DRRD) is one of these tasks that require mice to actively respond (e.g., continuously depressing a lever) for a minimum amount of time to be reinforced. The active production of a time interval by mice in DRRD differentiates this task from the differential reinforcement of low rates of responding (DRL) task as a passive waiting task that was used in earlier studies to investigate the optimality of adaptive biases in timing behavior. We tested 21 Th-Cre male mice (9 weeks old) in a DRRD task with a minimum requirement of 2 s. Mean response durations were positively biased (longer than the minimum requirement), and the extent of bias was predicted by the level of endogenous timing uncertainty. Mice nearly maximized the reward rate in this task. These results contribute to the accumulating evidence supporting optimal temporal risk assessment in non-human animals.
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
4. Dynamics of retrospective timing: A big data approach
- Author
-
Fuat Balcı, Hüseyin Ünübol, Simon Grondin, Gökben Hızlı Sayar, Virginie van Wassenhove, and Marc Wittmann
- Subjects
Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology - Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
5. The timing database: An open-access, live repository for interval timing studies
- Author
-
Turaç Aydoğan, Hakan Karşılar, Yalçın Akın Duyan, Başak Akdoğan, Alessia Baccarani, Renaud Brochard, Benjamin De Corte, Jonathon D. Crystal, Bilgehan Çavdaroğlu, Charles Randy Gallistel, Simon Grondin, Ezgi Gür, Quentin Hallez, Joost de Jong, Leendert van Maanen, Matthew Matell, Nandakumar S. Narayanan, Ezgi Özoğlu, Tutku Öztel, Argiro Vatakis, David Freestone, Fuat Balcı, and Duyan, Yalçin Akin
- Subjects
Big data ,Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,Experimental psychology ,Time perception ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Psychology (miscellaneous) ,Interval timing ,General Psychology ,Secondary analysis - Abstract
Interval timing refers to the ability to perceive and remember intervals in the seconds to minutes range. Our contemporary understanding of interval timing is derived from relatively small-scale, isolated studies that investigate a limited range of intervals with a small sample size, usually based on a single task. Consequently, the conclusions drawn from individual studies are not readily generalizable to other tasks, conditions, and task parameters. The current paper presents a live database that presents raw data from interval timing studies (currently composed of 68 datasets from eight different tasks incorporating various interval and temporal order judgments) with an online graphical user interface to easily select, compile, and download the data organized in a standard format. The Timing Database aims to promote and cultivate key and novel analyses of our timing ability by making published and future datasets accessible as open-source resources for the entire research community. In the current paper, we showcase the use of the database by testing various core ideas based on data compiled across studies (i.e., temporal accuracy, scalar property, location of the point of subjective equality, malleability of timing precision). The Timing Database will serve as the repository for interval timing studies through the submission of new datasets. 36595180
- Published
- 2023
6. EXPRESS: Humans can monitor trial-based but not global timing errors: Evidence for relative judgments in temporal error monitoring
- Author
-
Tutku Öztel and Fuat Balcı
- Subjects
Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology ,Physiology ,Physiology (medical) ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,General Medicine ,General Psychology - Abstract
Humans can monitor the magnitude and direction of their temporal errors in individual trials. Based on the predictions of our model of temporal error monitoring that rely on a relative comparison of internal clock readings, we predict that participants would monitor their timing errors in individual trials, but not the direction of their global timing errors without external feedback. One study has indeed found that accurate self-monitoring of average timing biases required external feedback with directional information. The current study investigates how different sources of feedback (i.e., internal or external) affect performance in the self-monitoring of average timing bias. Four groups of participants were tested in a temporal reproduction task. Participants in the self-evaluation condition evaluated the direction and size of their time reproduction errors in individual trials. In the accurate feedback condition, participants received explicit trial-based feedback regarding the direction of their error while participants in the partially accurate feedback condition received trial-based feedback according to the accuracy of short-long judgements of another participant in the self-evaluation condition. Participants in the control condition reproduced only the target duration without making any judgements regarding their reproduction performance or receiving any external feedback about it. Results showed that while participants accurately monitor timing errors in individual trials, in none of the experimental conditions were they more accurate than the chance level in terms of evaluating the direction of their average temporal bias. We discuss these results in terms of the temporal error monitoring model introduced by Akdoğan and Balcı. Thus, our findings suggest that external directional feedback does not have any informational value for global temporal bias judgements above and beyond internal self-monitoring.
- Published
- 2022
7. The role of time estimation in decreased impatience in intertemporal choice
- Author
-
Fuat Balcı, Yossi Zana, Camila S. Agostino, and Peter Claessens
- Subjects
Behavioral Neuroscience ,Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology ,Time estimation ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,Economics, Econometrics and Finance (miscellaneous) ,Econometrics ,Business, Management and Accounting (miscellaneous) ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Intertemporal choice ,Psychology ,Applied Psychology - Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
8. Mice make temporal inferences about novel locations based on previously learned spatiotemporal contingencies
- Author
-
Fuat Balcı, Ezgi Gür, Yalçın Akın Duyan, and Balci, Fuat
- Subjects
Mice ,Peak interval procedure ,Bayesian averaging ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Conditioning ,Interval timing ,Ecology, Evolution, Behavior and Systematics - Abstract
Animals learn multiple spatiotemporal contingencies and organize their anticipatory responses accordingly. The representational/computational capacity that underlies such spatiotemporally guided behaviors is not fully understood. To this end, we investigated whether mice make temporal inferences of novel locations based on previously learned spatiotemporal contingencies. We trained 18 C57BL/6J mice to anticipate reward after three different intervals at three different locations and tested their temporal expectations of a reward at five locations simultaneously, including two locations that were not previously associated with reward delivery but adjacent to the previously trained locations. If mice made spatiotemporal inferences, they were expected to interpolate between duration pairs associated with previously reinforced hoppers surrounding the novel hopper. We found that the maximal response rate at the novel locations indeed fell between the two intervals reinforced at the surrounding hoppers. We argue that this pattern of responding might be underlain by spatially constrained Bayesian computations. WOS:000884947700001 2-s2.0-85142197643 Science Citation Index Expanded Q1 Article Uluslararasi isbirligi ile yapilmayan - HAYIR 2022 YÖK - 2022-23 Kasim
- Published
- 2022
9. Stimulus-Induced Temporal Illusions: When Awareness Is Mesmerized by Time
- Author
-
Tutku Öztel and Fuat Balcı
- Subjects
Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Stimulus (physiology) ,Time perception ,Psychology ,Neuroscience ,Applied Psychology - Abstract
Our subjective experience of time intervals is susceptible to the effects of the various properties of the timed stimuli/events (e.g., motion, size, affect). For instance, subjective time is considerably lengthened when observing faster and shortened when observing slower walking animations. Such effects on perceived time have been investigated widely in the field. What we do not know based on these studies is if participants are aware of these sorts of stimulus-induced timing illusions. Thus, the current study, using confidence ratings, investigated whether the participants are aware of their largely biased time perception induced by the observed walking speed in a temporal bisection task. After each categorization of a probe interval as ‘short’ or ‘long’, we asked participants to rate their confidence level regarding their categorization. We reasoned that if participants were aware of their biased time perception, the temporal modulation of confidence ratings regarding their categorization performance would not change between different walking speed conditions. We found that confidence ratings closely tracked shifts in the psychometric functions suggesting that participants were not aware of the stimulus-induced warping of perceived time. We replicated these findings in a second experiment. Our results show that human participants are not aware of the stimulus-induced temporal illusions they experience.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
10. Numerical averaging in mice
- Author
-
Fuat Balcı, Ezgi Gür, and Yalçın Akın Duyan
- Subjects
0106 biological sciences ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Lever ,business.product_category ,05 social sciences ,Cue integration ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Numerosity adaptation effect ,Stimulus (physiology) ,Fixed consecutive number ,Audiology ,010603 evolutionary biology ,01 natural sciences ,medicine ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,050102 behavioral science & comparative psychology ,Stimulus control ,business ,Ecology, Evolution, Behavior and Systematics ,Mathematics - Abstract
Rodents can be trained to associate different durations with different stimuli (e.g., light/sound). When the associated stimuli are presented together, maximal responding is observed around the average of individual durations (akin to averaging). The current study investigated whether mice can also average independently trained numerosities. Mice were initially trained to make 10 or 20 lever presses on a single (run) lever to obtain a reward and each fixed-ratio schedule was signaled either with an auditory or visual stimulus. Then, mice were trained to press another lever to obtain the reward after they responded on the run lever for the minimum number of presses [Fixed Consecutive Number (FCN)-10 or -20 trials] signaled by the corresponding discriminative stimulus. Following this training, FCN trials with the compound stimulus were introduced to test the counting behavior of mice when they encountered conflicting information regarding the number of responses required to obtain the reward. Our results showed that the numbers of responses on these compound test trials were around the average of the number of responses in FCN-10 and FCN-20 trials particularly when the auditory stimulus was associated with a fewer number of required responses. The counting strategy explained the behavior of the majority of the mice in the FCN-Compound test trials (as opposed to the timing strategy). The number of responses in FCN-Compound trials was accounted for equally well by the arithmetic, geometric, and Bayesian averages of the number of responses observed in FCN-10 and FCN-20 trials.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
11. Temporal error monitoring with directional error magnitude judgements: a robust phenomenon with no effect of being watched
- Author
-
Tutku Öztel, Terry Eskenazi, and Fuat Balcı
- Subjects
Adult ,Computer science ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Speech recognition ,Emotions ,Metacognition ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,General Medicine ,Conformity ,Self Concept ,Self-Control ,Direct measure ,Judgment ,Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,Social Conformity ,Phenomenon ,Task Performance and Analysis ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,Humans ,media_common - Abstract
A key aspect of metacognition is the ability to monitor performance. A recent line of work has shown that error-monitoring ability captures both the magnitude and direction of timing errors, thereby pointing at the metric composition of error monitoring [e.g., Akdoğan and Balcı (J Exp Psychol https://dx.doi.org/10.1037/xge0000265 , 2017)]. These studies, however, primarily used a composite variable that combined isolated measures of ordinal confidence ratings (as a proxy for error magnitude judgement) and "shorter/longer than the target" judgements. In two experiments we tested temporal error monitoring (TEM) performance with a more direct measure of directional error magnitude rating on a continuum. The second aim of this study is to test if TEM performance is modulated by the feeling of being watched that was previously shown to influence metacognitive-like monitoring processes. We predicted that being watched would improve TEM performance, particularly in participants with high timing precision (a proxy for high task mastery), and disrupt TEM performance in participants with low timing precision (a proxy for low task mastery). In both experiments, we found strong evidence for TEM ability. However, we did not find any reliable effect of the social stimulus on TEM performance. In short, our results demonstrate that metric error monitoring is a robust metacognitive phenomenon, which is not sensitive to social influence.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
12. Count-based decision-making in mice: numerosity vs. stimulus control
- Author
-
Fuat Balcı, Ezgi Gür, and PINAR TOPTAS
- Subjects
Mice ,Reinforcement Schedule ,Time Perception ,Animals ,Learning ,Conditioning, Operant ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Reinforcement, Psychology ,Ecology, Evolution, Behavior and Systematics ,Probability - Abstract
Numerical and temporal control of behavior is ubiquitous across many species of animals. Recent studies showed that in the presence of reliable discriminative stimuli, mice ignore temporal relations and probabilistic information but when discriminative stimuli become non-informative, the same mice can spontaneously start relying on previously experienced time intervals and probabilities. Similar dynamics do not readily generalize to counting behavior since the response-outcome contingency functions differ when reinforcement depends on the number vs. timing of responding. In the current study, mice (N = 32) learned to press two different levers 10 (few) or 20 (many) times, while the active lever was signaled by a light stimulus. The probability of the few/many trials was manipulated between groups. During testing, the informative value of light stimulus was eliminated by signaling both few- and many-levers. In a quarter of training trials, mice ignored the discriminative stimulus and adopted a numerical decision strategy (starting to respond on the few-option and then switching to the many-option in many trials) that was sensitive to probabilistic information. The frequency but not the probability-sensitive parametrization of switching behavior changed when the discriminative stimulus became non-informative in testing. These findings suggest that there is a relatively strong representational control over counting behavior even in conditions that afford strong stimulus control.
- Published
- 2022
13. Elucidating the Common Basis for Task‐Dependent Differential Manifestations of Category Advantage: A Decision Theoretic Approach
- Author
-
Seda Akbiyik, Tilbe Göksun, and Fuat Balcı
- Subjects
Artificial Intelligence ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,Reaction Time ,Humans ,Linguistics ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Color Perception - Abstract
Cross-category hues are differentiated easier than otherwise equidistant hues that belong to the same linguistic category. This effect is typically manifested through both accuracy and response time gains in tasks with a memory component, whereas only response times are affected when there is no memory component. This raises the question of whether there is a common generative process underlying the differential behavioral manifestations of category advantage in color perception. For instance, within the framework of noisy evidence accumulation models, changes in accuracy can be readily attributed to an increase in the efficacy of perceptual evidence integration (after controlling for threshold setting), whereas changes in response time can also be attributed to shorter nondecisional delays (e.g., due to facilitated signal detection). To address the latent decision processes underlying category advantage across different behavioral demands, we introduce a decision-theoretic perspective (i.e., diffusion decision model) to categorical color perception in three complementary experiments. In Experiment 1, we collected data from a binary color naming task (1) to determine the green-blue boundary in our sample and (2) to trace how parameter estimates of interest in the model output change as a function of color typicality. In Experiments 2 and 3, we used same-different task paradigms (with and without a memory component, respectively) and traced the category advantage in color discrimination in two parameters of the diffusion decision model: nondecision time and drift rate. An increase in drift rate predominantly characterized the category advantage in both tasks. Our results show that improved efficiency in perceptual evidence integration is a common driving force behind different manifestations of category advantage.
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
14. Editorial to the Special Issue on Temporal Illusions
- Author
-
Argiro Vatakis and Fuat Balcı
- Subjects
Cognitive science ,Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Time perception ,Applied Psychology - Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
15. Symbolism overshadows the effect of physical size in supra-second temporal illusions
- Author
-
Hakan Karşılar and Fuat Balcı
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Symbolism ,Linguistics and Language ,Adolescent ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Stimulus (physiology) ,Language and Linguistics ,Numeral system ,Young Adult ,Perception ,Humans ,Rectangle ,Mathematics ,media_common ,Point (typography) ,Numerosity adaptation effect ,Mathematical Concepts ,Time perception ,Representational systems ,Illusions ,Sensory Systems ,Time Perception ,Female ,Photic Stimulation ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
The perception of quantities has been suggested to rely on shared, magnitude-based representational systems that preserve metric properties. As such, different quantifiable dimensions that can characterize any given stimulus (e.g., size, speed, or numerosity) have been shown to modulate the perceived duration of these stimuli-a finding that has been attributed to cross-modal interaction among the quantity representations. However, these results are typically based on the isolated effects of a single stimulus dimension, leaving their potential combined effects uncharted. In the present study we aimed to investigate the joint effects of numerical magnitude and physical size on perceived time. In four complementary experiments, participants categorized six durations as "short" or "long," which were presented through combinations of Hindu-Arabic numerals in three font sizes, as well as with simple shapes (rectangles) and unfamiliar symbols (Klingon letters), the sizes of which corresponded to the font sizes of the Hindu-Arabic numerals. Our results showed temporal underestimation for the smallest numeral in the set (3), with no effects of font size on perceived duration. The perceived durations were longest for the physically smallest geometric stimuli (i.e., a rectangle), and the font size of symbol-like stimuli (i.e., Klingon letters) was not found to have an effect on perceived time. Finally, presenting only one numeral (6) instead of the rectangle once again eliminated the relationship between physical size and perceived time, suggesting an overshadowing of physical-size-based influences on temporal choice behavior, presumably by perceived symbolism. Our results point at the complex nature of the interaction between different magnitude representations.
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
16. Numerical error monitoring
- Author
-
Yalçın Akın Duyan and Fuat Balcı
- Subjects
Mixed model ,Magnitude (mathematics) ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Models, Psychological ,050105 experimental psychology ,Judgment ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,Linear regression ,Statistics ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Sequence ,05 social sciences ,Numerosity adaptation effect ,Fixed effects model ,Random effects model ,Degree (music) ,Acoustic Stimulation ,Auditory Perception ,Linear Models ,Metacognition ,Psychology ,Mathematics ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery - Abstract
Error monitoring has recently been discovered to have informationally rich foundations in the timing domain. Based on the common properties of magnitude-based representations, we hypothesized that judgments on the direction and the magnitude of errors would also reflect their objective counterparts in the numerosity domain. In two experiments, we presented fast sequences of "beeps" with random interstimulus intervals and asked participants to stop the sequence when they thought the target count (7, 11, or 19) had been reached. Participants then judged how close to the target they stopped the sequence, and whether their response undershot or overshot the target. Individual linear regression fits as well as the linear mixed model with a fixed effect of reproduced numerosity on confidence ratings, and participants as independent random effects on the intercept and the slope, revealed significant positive slopes for all the target numerosities. Our results suggest that humans can keep track of the direction and degree of errors in the estimation of discrete quantities, pointing at a numerical-error-monitoring ability.
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
17. Effect of Acute Physical Activity on Interval Timing
- Author
-
Ceyda Sayalı, Ezgi Uslu, Fuat Balcı, Resit Canbeyli, and Melisa Menceloğlu
- Subjects
Rest (physics) ,Movement (music) ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,05 social sciences ,Physical activity ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Biology ,050105 experimental psychology ,03 medical and health sciences ,Interval (music) ,Subjective time ,0302 clinical medicine ,Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology ,Duration (music) ,Sample size determination ,Statistics ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Treadmill ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Applied Psychology - Abstract
Timing is an integral part of physical activities. Walking as a routine form of physical activity might affect interval timing primarily in two different ways within the pacemaker–accumulator timing-theoretic framework: (1) by increasing the speed of the pacemaker due to its physiological effects; (2) by decreasing attention to time and consequently slowing the rate of temporal integration by serving as a secondary task. In order to elucidate the effect of movement on subjective time, in two different experiments we employed a temporal reproduction task conducted on the treadmill under four different encoding–decoding conditions: (1) encoding and reproducing (decoding) the duration while standing (rest); (2) encoding the duration at rest and reproducing it while moving: (3) both encoding and reproducing the duration while moving; and (4) encoding the duration while moving and reproducing it at rest. In the first experiment, participants were tested either in the 4 or the 8 km/h movement condition, whereas in the second experiment a larger sample was tested only in the 4 km/h movement condition. Data were de-trended to control for long-term performance drifts. In Experiment 1, overall durations encoded at rest and reproduced during motion were under-reproduced whereas durations encoded during motion and reproduced at rest were over-reproduced only in the 8 km/h condition. In Experiment 2, the same results were observed in the 4 km/h condition with a larger sample size. These effects on timing behavior provide support for the clock speed-driven effect of movement and contradicts the predictions of attention-based mediation.
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
18. The modulatory role of pre-SMA in speed-accuracy tradeoff
- Author
-
Yusuf Ozgur Cakmak, Hale Yapici Eser, Alexander T. Sack, Fuat Balcı, Dilara Berkay, Cognition, and RS: FPN CN 4
- Subjects
Male ,STIMULATION ,NEURAL BASIS ,medicine.medical_treatment ,Motion Perception ,Task (project management) ,Behavioral Neuroscience ,0302 clinical medicine ,Theta burst stimulation ,Neural Pathways ,DIFFUSION-MODEL ,media_common ,Drift diffusion model ,SUBTHALAMIC NUCLEUS ,05 social sciences ,Motor Cortex ,Speed accuracy tradeoff ,SMA ,Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation ,PERCEPTUAL DECISION-MAKING ,Female ,Psychology ,RESPONSE-INHIBITION ,Adult ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Models, Neurological ,Posterior parietal cortex ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Sensory system ,Motor Activity ,050105 experimental psychology ,03 medical and health sciences ,Young Adult ,PARIETAL CORTEX ,Neuroimaging ,Control theory ,Perception ,medicine ,Journal Article ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,REACTION-TIME ,Presupplementary motor area ,Corpus Striatum ,FRONTO-BASAL-GANGLIA ,Transcranial magnetic stimulation ,Brain stimulation ,TASK ,Decision making ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Psychomotor Performance - Abstract
Many perceptual decisions are inevitably subject to the tradeoff between speed and accuracy of choices (SAT). Sequential sampling models attribute this ubiquitous relation to random noise in the sensory evidence accumulation process, and assume that SAT is adaptively modulated by altering the decision thresholds at which the level of integrated evidence should reach for making a choice. Although, neuroimaging studies have shown a relationship between right presupplementary motor area (pre-SMA) activity and threshold setting, only a limited number of brain stimulation studies aimed at establishing the causal link, results of which were inconsistent. Additionally, these studies were limited in scope as they only examined the effect of pre-SMA activity unidirectionally through experimentally inhibiting the neural activity in this region. The current study aims to investigate the predictions of the striatal theory of SAT by experimentally assessing the modulatory effect of right pre-SMA on threshold setting bi-directionally. To this end, we applied both offline inhibition and excitation to the right pre-SMA utilizing transcranial magnetic stimulation in a within-subjects design and tested participants on a Random Dot Motion Task. Decision thresholds were estimated using the Hierarchical Drift Diffusion Model. Findings of our planned comparisons showed that right pre-SMA inhibition leads to significantly higher, whereas right pre-SMA excitation leads to significantly lower thresholds without showing any effects on the evidence integration process itself.
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
19. Spontaneous integration of temporal information: implications for representational/computational capacity of animals
- Author
-
Yalçın Akın Duyan, Ezgi Gür, and Fuat Balcı
- Subjects
Cognitive science ,Time Factors ,Behavior, Animal ,Psychological research ,05 social sciences ,Behavioural sciences ,Flexibility (personality) ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Cognition ,Choice Behavior ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,Ascription ,Conditioning, Psychological ,Animals ,Learning ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,050102 behavioral science & comparative psychology ,Representation (mathematics) ,Association (psychology) ,Psychology ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Ecology, Evolution, Behavior and Systematics ,Associative property - Abstract
How do animals adapt their behaviors to changing conditions? This question relates to the debate between associative versus representational/computational approaches in cognitive science. An influential line of research that has significantly shaped the conceptual development of animal learning over decades has primarily focused on the role of associative dynamics with little-to-no ascription of representational/combinatorial capacities. The common assumption of these models is that behavioral adjustments are incremental and they result from updating of associations based on actions and their outcomes, without encoding the critical information serving as the determinant(s) of such contingencies (e.g., time in interval schedules, number in ratio schedules). On the other hand, an independent line of research provides evidence for behavioral phenomena that cannot be readily accounted for by the conventional associationist approach. In this paper, we will review different sets of findings particularly in the area of interval timing that suggest the ability of animals to make swift spontaneous computations on subjective quantities and incorporate them into their behavior. Findings of these studies constitute empirical challenges for the associationist approaches to behavioral flexibility. We argue that interval timing is a fertile ground for the formulation of critical tests of different theoretical approaches to animal behavior.
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
20. The Confidence Database
- Author
-
Alan L. F. Lee, Polina Arbuzova, Torin K. Clark, Nadia Haddara, Damian P. Birney, Tricia X. F. Seow, Gerit Pfuhl, Tzu Yu Hsu, Caroline Peters, Jason Samaha, Maxine T. Sherman, David Soto, Maël Lebreton, Michael Pereira, Manuel Rausch, Ji Won Bang, Fuat Balcı, Gabriel Weindel, Karolina M. Lempert, Sabina Gherman, Antonio Martin, Denis O'Hora, Ariel Zylberberg, Başak Akdoğan, Peter D. Kvam, Matt Jaquiery, Gabriel Reyes, Eleanor R. Palser, Marcin Koculak, Audrey Mazancieux, Joshua Calder-Travis, Jeroen J.A. van Boxtel, Andrey Chetverikov, Yalçın Akın Duyan, Chen Song, Liang Luo, Borysław Paulewicz, Medha Shekhar, Vincent de Gardelle, Saeedeh Sadeghi, Kit S. Double, Karen Davranche, Christina Koß, Nathan Faivre, Troy C. Dildine, Sze Chai Kwok, Marios G. Philiastides, Indrit Bègue, Marion Rouault, Kobe Desender, Marta Siedlecka, Zuzanna Skóra, Lauren Y. Atlas, Fernanda Prieto, Xinming Xu, Justin Kantner, Jiwon Yeon, Brian Maniscalco, David Aguilar-Lleyda, Futing Zou, Timothy F. Brady, Xiao Hu, Mahiko Konishi, Julian Matthews, Sai Sun, Sébastien Massoni, William T. Adler, Shuo Wang, Rachel N. Denison, Samuel Recht, Jérôme Sackur, Thibault Gajdos, Kaitlyn Fallow, Michał Wierzchoń, Daniel M. Merfeld, Chien Ming Lo, Elisa Filevich, Iñaki Iturrate, Marine Hainguerlot, Christoph T. Weidemann, Qun Ye, Regan M. Gallagher, Dobromir Rahnev, Georgia Institute of Technology [Atlanta], Universitaetsklinikum Hamburg-Eppendorf = University Medical Center Hamburg-Eppendorf [Hamburg] (UKE), Universiteit Gent = Ghent University [Belgium] (UGENT), Lingnan Normal University, Department of Psychology and Center for Neural Science, New York University, New York, NY, USA, Centre d'économie de la Sorbonne (CES), Université Paris 1 Panthéon-Sorbonne (UP1)-Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), Columbia University [New York], Bernstein Center for Computational Neuroscience [Berlin], Humboldt-Universität zu Berlin, National Institutes of Health [Bethesda] (NIH), New York University School of Medicine (NYU), New York University School of Medicine, NYU System (NYU)-NYU System (NYU), Geneva University Hospitals and University of Geneva, School of Psychology, The University of Sydney, Department of Psychology (University of California San Diego), University of California [San Diego] (UC San Diego), University of California-University of California, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK, Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud university [Nijmegen], Smead Aerospace Engineering Sciences, Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), Aix Marseille Université (AMU), Department of Education, University of Oxford, Applied Economics, Cognitive Psychology, Koç University, Laboratoire de psychologie cognitive (LPC), Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS)-Aix Marseille Université (AMU), Laboratoire de Psychologie et NeuroCognition (LPNC ), Université Savoie Mont Blanc (USMB [Université de Savoie] [Université de Chambéry])-Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS)-Université Grenoble Alpes (UGA), Department of Psychology, University of Victoria, School of Psychology, University of Queensland, University of Queensland [Brisbane], Department of Experimental & Applied Psychology, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam (VUPSY), Paris School of Economics (PSE), École des Ponts ParisTech (ENPC)-École normale supérieure - Paris (ENS Paris), Université Paris sciences et lettres (PSL)-Université Paris sciences et lettres (PSL)-Université Paris 1 Panthéon-Sorbonne (UP1)-Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS)-École des hautes études en sciences sociales (EHESS)-Institut National de Recherche pour l’Agriculture, l’Alimentation et l’Environnement (INRAE), Institute of Neuroscience and Psychology, University of Glasgow, Glasgow, United Kingdom, The Feinstein Institute for Medical Research, Erasmus University Rotterdam, Graduate Institute of Mind, Brain and Consciousness, Taipei Medical University, Taipei, Taiwan, Capital Normal University [Beijing], California State University [Northridge] (CSUN), Uniwersytet Jagielloński w Krakowie = Jagiellonian University (UJ), Laboratoire de sciences cognitives et psycholinguistique (LSCP), Département d'Etudes Cognitives - ENS Paris (DEC), École normale supérieure - Paris (ENS Paris), Université Paris sciences et lettres (PSL)-Université Paris sciences et lettres (PSL)-École normale supérieure - Paris (ENS Paris), Université Paris sciences et lettres (PSL)-Université Paris sciences et lettres (PSL)-École des hautes études en sciences sociales (EHESS)-Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), Department of Psychology, University of Florida, Key Laboratory of Brain Functional Genomics (MOE & STCSM), School of Psychology and Cognitive Science, East China Normal University, Shanghai, China, Shanghai Key Laboratory of Magnetic Resonance, School of Physics and Materials Science, East China Normal University, Swiss Center for Affective Science, University of Pennsylvania [Philadelphia], Collaborative Innovation Center of Assessment toward Basic Education Quality, Beijing (ENCICABEQ), Department of Bioengineering, University of California, Bureau d'Économie Théorique et Appliquée (BETA), Université de Strasbourg (UNISTRA)-Université de Lorraine (UL)-Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS)-Institut National de Recherche pour l’Agriculture, l’Alimentation et l’Environnement (INRAE), School of Psychological Sciences, Monash University, Ohio State University [Columbus] (OSU), National University of Ireland [Galway] (NUI Galway), University of California [San Francisco] (UCSF), University of California, University College of London [London] (UCL), SWPS University of Social Sciences and Humanities, Partenaires INRAE, Ecole Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne (EPFL), University of Glasgow, The Arctic University of Norway (UiT), Universidad del Desarrollo, Catholic University of Eichstätt-Ingolstadt (KU), Laboratoire des systèmes perceptifs (LSP), Université Paris sciences et lettres (PSL)-Université Paris sciences et lettres (PSL)-Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), Université Paris sciences et lettres (PSL)-Université Paris sciences et lettres (PSL), Laboratoire d'informatique de l'École polytechnique [Palaiseau] (LIX), Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS)-École polytechnique (X), Cornell University [New York], University of California [Santa Cruz] (UCSC), Trinity College Dublin, Sackler Ctr Consciousness Sci, Brighton, E Sussex, University of Sussex, Cardiff University, Basque Ctr Cognit Brain & Language, San Sebastian, Basque Fdn Sci, Ikerbasque, Bilbao, CALTECH, Div Biol, Pasadena, CALTECH, Div Computat & Neural Syst, Pasadena, Monash University [Melbourne], University of Canberra, West Virginia University [Morgantown], Swansea University, East China Normal University [Shangaï] (ECNU), University of Rochester [USA], The organization of the Confidence Database was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health under award number R56MH119189 to D.R., ANR-16-CE28-0002,ImpactMeta,impact de la métacognition sur le comportement(2016), ANR-16-ASTR-0014,MetaStress,Impact du stress sur la décision et la métacognition: applications en aéronautique(2016), Balcı, Fuat (ORCID 0000-0003-3390-9352 & YÖK ID 51269), Duyan, Yalçın Akın, Kvam, Peter D., Rahnev, Dobromir, Desender, Kobe, Lee, Alan L. F., Adler, William T., Aguilar-Lleyda, David, Akdoğan, Başak, Arbuzova, Polina, Atlas, Lauren Y., Bang, Ji Won, Bègue, Indrit, Birney, Damian P, Brady, Timothy F., Calder-Travis, Joshua, Chetverikov, Andrey, Clark, Torin K., Davranche, Karen, Denison, Rachel N., Dildine, Troy C., Double, Kit S., Faivre, Nathan, Fallow, Kaitlyn, Filevich, Elisa, Gajdos, Thibault, Gallagher, Regan M., de Gardelle, Vincent, Gherman, Sabina, Haddara, Nadia, Hainguerlot, Marine, Hsu, Tzu-Yu, Hu, Xiao, Iturrate, Iñaki, Jaquiery, Matt, Kantner, Justin, Koculak, Marcin, Konishi, Mahiko, Koß, Christina, Kwok, Sze Chai, Lebreton, Maël, Lempert, Karolina M., Ming Lo, Chien, Luo, Liang, Maniscalco, Brian, Martin, Antonio, Massoni, Sébastien, Matthews, Julian, Mazancieux, Audrey, Merfeld, Daniel M., O’Hora, Denis, Palser, Eleanor R., Paulewicz, Borysław, Pereira, Michael, Peters, Caroline, Philiastides, Marios G., Pfuhl, Gerit, Prieto, Fernanda, Rausch, Manuel, Recht, Samuel, Reyes, Gabriel, Rouault, Marion, Sackur, Jérôme, Sadeghi, Saeedeh, Samaha, Jason, Seow, Tricia X. F., Shekhar, Medha, Sherman, Maxine T., Siedlecka, Marta, Skóra, Zuzanna, Song, Chen, Soto, David, Sun, Sai, van Boxtel, Jeroen J. A., Wang, Shuo, Weidemann, Christoph T., Weindel, Gabriel, Wierzchoń, Michał, Xu, Xinming, Ye, Qun, Yeon, Jiwon, Zou, Futing, Zylberberg, Ariel, College of Social Sciences and Humanities, Department of Psychology, PSL Univ, Ecole Normale Super, Lab Syst Percetifs, Dept Etud Cognit,CNRS, PSL Univ, Ecole Normale Super, INSERM, Dept Etud Cognit,CNRS,EHESS, École polytechnique (X)-Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), Universiteit Gent = Ghent University (UGENT), Humboldt University Of Berlin, Department of Psychology [Univ California San Diego] (Psycho - UC San Diego), University of California (UC)-University of California (UC), Radboud University [Nijmegen], University of Colorado [Boulder], Aix Marseille Université (AMU)-Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), Université Paris 1 Panthéon-Sorbonne (UP1)-École normale supérieure - Paris (ENS-PSL), Université Paris sciences et lettres (PSL)-Université Paris sciences et lettres (PSL)-École des hautes études en sciences sociales (EHESS)-École des Ponts ParisTech (ENPC)-Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS)-Institut National de Recherche pour l’Agriculture, l’Alimentation et l’Environnement (INRAE), École normale supérieure - Paris (ENS-PSL), Université Paris sciences et lettres (PSL)-Université Paris sciences et lettres (PSL)-École normale supérieure - Paris (ENS-PSL), University of Florida [Gainesville] (UF), University of Pennsylvania, AgroParisTech-Université de Strasbourg (UNISTRA)-Université de Haute-Alsace (UHA) Mulhouse - Colmar (Université de Haute-Alsace (UHA))-Université de Lorraine (UL)-Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS)-Institut National de Recherche pour l’Agriculture, l’Alimentation et l’Environnement (INRAE), University of California [San Francisco] (UC San Francisco), University of California (UC), SWPS University of Social Sciences and Humanities (SWPS), The Arctic University of Norway [Tromsø, Norway] (UiT), University of California [Santa Cruz] (UC Santa Cruz), and Ikerbasque - Basque Foundation for Science
- Subjects
Databases, Factual ,SIGNAL-DETECTION ,Computer science ,Datasets as Topic ,Social Sciences ,Confidence ,INSIGHT ,computer.software_genre ,Signal-detection ,Choice ,Availability ,Recognition ,Insight ,Choice Behavior ,[SCCO]Cognitive science ,Behavioral Neuroscience ,ddc:616.89 ,Mental Processes ,0302 clinical medicine ,Software ,insight ,Task Performance and Analysis ,Psychology ,choice ,media_common ,0303 health sciences ,Thesaurus (information retrieval) ,Psychology, Biological ,Database ,Psychology, Experimental ,Cognition ,[SHS.ECO]Humanities and Social Sciences/Economics and Finance ,16. Peace & justice ,CHOICE ,Multidisciplinary Sciences ,ddc:128.37 ,[SCCO.PSYC]Cognitive science/Psychology ,VDP::Samfunnsvitenskap: 200::Psykologi: 260 ,Science & Technology - Other Topics ,recognition ,Life Sciences & Biomedicine ,Adult ,Psychometrics ,Social Psychology ,VDP::Social science: 200::Psychology: 260 ,media_common.quotation_subject ,availability ,MEDLINE ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Article ,Databases ,03 medical and health sciences ,Perception ,Reaction Time ,Humans ,Set (psychology) ,Factual ,030304 developmental biology ,Science & Technology ,business.industry ,Extramural ,AVAILABILITY ,Neurosciences ,RECOGNITION ,signal-detection ,Psychology, biological ,Multidisciplinary sciences ,Psychology, experimental ,ddc:616.8 ,Study ,Neurosciences & Neurology ,Metacognition ,business ,computer ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery - Abstract
Understanding how people rate their confidence is critical for the characterization of a wide range of perceptual, memory, motor and cognitive processes. To enable the continued exploration of these processes, we created a large database of confidence studies spanning a broad set of paradigms, participant populations and fields of study. The data from each study are structured in a common, easy-to-use format that can be easily imported and analysed using multiple software packages. Each dataset is accompanied by an explanation regarding the nature of the collected data. At the time of publication, the Confidence Database (which is available at ) contained 145 datasets with data from more than 8,700 participants and almost 4 million trials. The database will remain open for new submissions indefinitely and is expected to continue to grow. Here we show the usefulness of this large collection of datasets in four different analyses that provide precise estimations of several foundational confidence-related effects., NIH National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH)
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
21. Monitoring line length reproduction errors
- Author
-
Yalçın Akın Duyan, Fuat Balcı, Duyan, Yalçın Akın, Balcı, Fuat (ORCID 0000-0003-3390-9352 & YÖK ID 51269), Koç University Research Center for Translational Medicine (KUTTAM) / Koç Üniversitesi Translasyonel Tıp Araştırma Merkezi (KUTTAM), Graduate School of Social Sciences and Humanities, College of Social Sciences and Humanities, and Department of Psychology
- Subjects
Adult ,05 social sciences ,Line length ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Error monitoring ,Line reproduction ,Magnitude representations ,Confidence judgments ,Mathematical Concepts ,050105 experimental psychology ,Psychology, experimental ,03 medical and health sciences ,Executive Function ,Young Adult ,0302 clinical medicine ,Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,Space Perception ,Statistics ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,Visual Perception ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Psychology ,Metacognition ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Size Perception - Abstract
Previous work revealed that humans can keep track of the direction and degree of errors in their temporal and numerical reproductions/estimations. Given the behavioral and psychophysical commonalities to various magnitudes and the implication of an overlapping neuroanatomical locus for their representation, we hypothesized that participants would capture the direction of errors and confidence ratings would track the magnitude of errors in line-length reproductions. In two experiments, participants reproduced various target lengths as accurately as possible, and reported the direction of their errors and provided confidence ratings for their reproductions. The isolated analysis of these two second-order judgments showed that participants can correctly report the direction of errors in their line-length reproductions and subjective confidence decreases as the magnitude of errors increases. These results show that humans can robustly keep track of the direction of errors in their line-length reproductions and their subjective confidence corroborates the magnitude of these errors., Turkish Academy of Sciences (Turkish Academy of Sciences (TÜBA)-GEBiP) The Young Scientist Award Programme; Science Academy of Turkey (BAGEP) The Young Scientist Award Programme
- Published
- 2020
22. Metric error monitoring: Another generalized mechanism for magnitude representations?
- Author
-
Fuat Balcı and Ece Yallak
- Subjects
Linguistics and Language ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Magnitude (mathematics) ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,050105 experimental psychology ,Language and Linguistics ,Feedback ,Task (project management) ,Judgment ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,Perception ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Point (geometry) ,Generalizability theory ,Sensitivity (control systems) ,media_common ,business.industry ,05 social sciences ,Pattern recognition ,Numerosity adaptation effect ,Metric (mathematics) ,Artificial intelligence ,Metacognition ,Psychology ,business ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery - Abstract
Error monitoring refers to the ability to monitor one's own task performance without explicit feedback. This ability is studied typically in two-alternative forced-choice (2AFC) paradigms. Recent research showed that humans can also keep track of the magnitude and direction of errors in different magnitude domains (e.g., numerosity, duration, length). Based on the evidence that suggests a shared mechanism for magnitude representations, we aimed to investigate whether metric error monitoring ability is commonly governed across different magnitude domains. Participants reproduced/estimated temporal, numerical, and spatial magnitudes after which they rated their confidence regarding first order task performance and judged the direction of their reproduction/estimation errors. Participants were also tested in a 2AFC perceptual decision task and provided confidence ratings regarding their decisions. Results showed that variability in reproductions/estimations and metric error monitoring ability, as measured by combining confidence and error direction judgements, were positively related across temporal, spatial, and numerical domains. Metacognitive sensitivity in these metric domains was also positively associated with each other but not with metacognitive sensitivity in the 2AFC perceptual decision task. In conclusion, the current findings point at a general metric error monitoring ability that is shared across different metric domains with limited generalizability to perceptual decision-making.
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
23. Individual differences in long-range time representation
- Author
-
Yossi Zana, Marcelo S. Caetano, Peter Claessens, Fuat Balcı, and Camila S. Agostino
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Linguistics and Language ,Individuality ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Intertemporal choice ,050105 experimental psychology ,Language and Linguistics ,Normal distribution ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,Statistics ,Linear regression ,Psychophysics ,Range (statistics) ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Statistical dispersion ,Power function ,Mathematics ,05 social sciences ,Linear model ,Models, Theoretical ,Sensory Systems ,Time Perception ,Linear Models ,Female ,Time preference ,Social psychology ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery - Abstract
On the basis of experimental data, long-range time representation has been proposed to follow a highly compressed power function, which has been hypothesized to explain the time inconsistency found in financial discount rate preferences. The aim of this study was to evaluate how well linear and power function models explain empirical data from individual participants tested in different procedural settings. The line paradigm was used in five different procedural variations with 35 adult participants. Data aggregated over the participants showed that fitted linear functions explained more than 98% of the variance in all procedures. A linear regression fit also outperformed a power model fit for the aggregated data. An individual-participant-based analysis showed better fits of a linear model to the data of 14 participants; better fits of a power function with an exponent β > 1 to the data of 12 participants; and better fits of a power function with β < 1 to the data of the remaining nine participants. Of the 35 volunteers, the null hypothesis β = 1 was rejected for 20. The dispersion of the individual β values was approximated well by a normal distribution. These results suggest that, on average, humans perceive long-range time intervals not in a highly compressed, biased manner, but rather in a linear pattern. However, individuals differ considerably in their subjective time scales. This contribution sheds new light on the average and individual psychophysical functions of long-range time representation, and suggests that any attribution of deviation from exponential discount rates in intertemporal choice to the compressed nature of subjective time must entail the characterization of subjective time on an individual-participant basis.
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
24. Mice optimize timed decisions about probabilistic outcomes under deadlines
- Author
-
Fuat Balcı and Ezgi Gür
- Subjects
Male ,Mathematical optimization ,Schedule ,Time Factors ,Computer science ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Decision Making ,Reward-based selection ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,050105 experimental psychology ,Task (project management) ,Mice ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,Inhibitory control ,Animals ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Function (engineering) ,Ecology, Evolution, Behavior and Systematics ,Response inhibition ,Probability ,media_common ,Behavior, Animal ,05 social sciences ,Uncertainty ,Probabilistic logic ,Maximization ,Mice, Inbred C57BL ,Conditioning, Operant ,Social psychology ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery - Abstract
Optimal performance in temporal decisions requires the integration of timing uncertainty with environmental statistics such as probability or cost functions. Reward maximization under response deadlines constitutes one of the most stringent examples of these problems. The current study investigated whether and how mice can optimize their timing behavior in a complex experimental setting under a response deadline in which reward maximization required the integration of timing uncertainty with a geometrically increasing probability/decreasing cost function. Mice optimized their performance under seconds-long response deadlines when the underlying function was reward probability but approached this level of performance when the underlying function was reward cost, only under the assumption of logarithmically scaled subjective costs. The same subjects were then tested in a timed response inhibition task characterized by response rules that conflicted with the initial task, not responding earlier than a schedule as opposed to not missing the deadline. Irrespective of original test groups, mice optimized the timing of their inhibitory control in the second experiment. These results provide strong support for the ubiquity of optimal temporal risk assessment in mice.
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
25. Temporal Expectation Indexed by Pupillary Response
- Author
-
Fuat Balcı, Hedderik van Rijn, and Başak Akdoğan
- Subjects
medicine.medical_specialty ,Artificial neural network ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,05 social sciences ,Pupil size ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Stimulus (physiology) ,Audiology ,050105 experimental psychology ,Developmental psychology ,03 medical and health sciences ,Autonomic nervous system ,0302 clinical medicine ,Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology ,Pupillary response ,medicine ,Delay Duration ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Predictability ,Psychology ,Temporal information ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Applied Psychology - Abstract
Forming temporal expectations plays an instrumental role for the optimization of behavior and allocation of attentional resources. Although the effects of temporal expectations on visual attention are well-established, the question of whether temporal predictions modulate the behavioral outputs of the autonomic nervous system such as the pupillary response remains unanswered. Therefore, this study aimed to obtain an online measure of pupil size while human participants were asked to differentiate between visual targets presented after varying time intervals since trial onset. Specifically, we manipulated temporal predictability in the presentation of target stimuli consisting of letters which appeared after either a short or long delay duration (1.5 vs. 3 s) in the majority of trials (75%) within different test blocks. In the remaining trials (25%), no target stimulus was present to investigate the trajectory of preparatory pupillary response under a low level of temporal uncertainty. The results revealed that the rate of preparatory pupillary response was contingent upon the time of target appearance such that pupils dilated at a higher rate when the targets were expected to appear after a shorter as compared to a longer delay period irrespective of target presence. The finding that pupil size can track temporal regularities and exhibit differential preparatory response between different delay conditions points to the existence of a distributed neural network subserving temporal information processing which is crucial for cognitive functioning and goal-directed behavior.
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
26. The effects of payoff manipulations on temporal bisection performance
- Author
-
Başak Akdoğan and Fuat Balcı
- Subjects
Male ,Signal Detection, Psychological ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Stimulus (physiology) ,Choice Behavior ,050105 experimental psychology ,Discrimination Learning ,Young Adult ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,Reward ,Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,Reaction Time ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Detection theory ,Discrimination learning ,Motivation ,05 social sciences ,Stochastic game ,General Medicine ,Maximization ,Time perception ,Response bias ,Categorization ,Time Perception ,Female ,Psychology ,Social psychology ,Psychomotor Performance ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
There is growing evidence that alterations in reward rates modify timing behavior demonstrating the role of motivational factors in interval timing behavior. This study aimed to investigate the effects of manipulations of rewards and penalties on temporal bisection performance in humans. Participants were trained to classify experienced time intervals as short or long based on the reference durations. Two groups of participants were tested under three different bias conditions in which either the relative reward magnitude or penalty associated with correct or incorrect categorizations of short and long reference durations was manipulated. Participants adapted their choice behavior (i.e., psychometric functions shifted) based on these payoff manipulations in directions predicted by reward maximization. The signal detection theory-based analysis of the data revealed that payoff contingencies affected the response bias parameter (B″) without altering participants' sensitivity (A') to temporal distances. Finally, the response time (RT) analysis showed that short categorization RTs increased, whereas long categorization RTs decreased as a function of stimulus durations. However, overall RTs did not exhibit any modulation in response to payoff manipulations. Taken together, this study provides additional support for the effects of motivational variables on temporal decision-making.
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
27. Mice and rats fail to integrate exogenous timing noise into their time-based decisions
- Author
-
Fuat Balcı, Dilara Berkay, and David Freestone
- Subjects
Mathematical optimization ,Computer science ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Decision Making ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Time based ,050105 experimental psychology ,Task (project management) ,Normal distribution ,Mice ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,Reward ,Animals ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Set (psychology) ,Function (engineering) ,Ecology, Evolution, Behavior and Systematics ,media_common ,Communication ,business.industry ,05 social sciences ,Uncertainty ,Maximization ,Wait time ,Rats ,Noise ,Time Perception ,business ,psychological phenomena and processes ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery - Abstract
Endogenous timing uncertainty results in variability in time-based judgments. In many timing tasks, animals need to incorporate their level of endogenous timing uncertainty into their decisions in order to maximize the reward rate. Although animals have been shown to adopt such optimal behavioral strategies in time-based decisions, whether they can optimize their behavior under exogenous noise is an open question. In this study, we tested mice and rats in a task that required them to space their responses for a minimum duration (DRL task) in different task conditions. In one condition, the minimum wait time was fixed, whereas in other conditions minimum wait time was a Gaussian random variable. Although reward maximization entailed waiting longer with added exogenous timing variability, results indicated that both mice and rats became more impulsive and deviated from optimality with increasing levels of exogenous noise. We introduce a reward-rate-dependent sampling function to SET to account for optimal performance in noiseless and suboptimal performance in noisy environments.
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
28. Asymmetrical modulation of time perception by increase versus decrease in coherence of motion
- Author
-
Fuat Balcı and Hakan Karşılar
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Linguistics and Language ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Time Factors ,Adolescent ,Motion Perception ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Audiology ,Stimulus (physiology) ,050105 experimental psychology ,Language and Linguistics ,Young Adult ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,Saccades ,medicine ,Humans ,Attention ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Analysis of Variance ,Communication ,business.industry ,05 social sciences ,Eye movement ,Time perception ,Sensory Systems ,Saccadic masking ,Time Perception ,Female ,Psychology ,business ,Photic Stimulation ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery - Abstract
Stimulus properties are known to affect duration judgments. In this study, we tested the effect of motion coherence levels in randomly moving dots on the perceived duration of these stimuli. In Experiments 1 and 2 we tested participants on a temporal reproduction task, using stimuli with varying degrees of motion coherence as the to-be-timed stimuli. Our results in both experiments showed that increasing motion coherence from the encoded (i.e. the first) to the reproduced (i.e. the second) stimulus leads to longer reproduction times. These effects were primarily additive in nature, and their magnitude increased with the difference between the coherence levels in the encoding versus reproduction (decoding) phases. This effect was not mirrored when there was a decrease in motion coherence. Experiment 3 tested if the differential number of exploratory saccadic eye-movements during encoding and reproduction predicted these effects. The behavioral findings of Experiment 1 and 2 were replicated in the third experiment, and the change in the number of eye movements from encoding to reproduction predicted the reproduction time when there was an increase in motion coherence. These results are explained by the effect of attention on the latency to initiate temporal integration that is only manifested when there is an increase in the level of motion coherence.
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
29. Metric error monitoring in the numerical estimates
- Author
-
Fuat Balcı and Yalçın Akın Duyan
- Subjects
Adult ,Numerical error ,05 social sciences ,Numerical cognition ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Mathematical Concepts ,050105 experimental psychology ,Correlation ,Thinking ,03 medical and health sciences ,Executive Function ,Young Adult ,0302 clinical medicine ,Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,Pattern Recognition, Visual ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,Auditory stimuli ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Numerical estimation ,Psychology ,Algorithm ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery - Abstract
Recent studies have shown that participants can keep track of the magnitude and direction of their errors while reproducing target intervals (Akdogan & Balci, 2017) and producing numerosities with sequentially presented auditory stimuli (Duyan & Balci, 2018). Although the latter work demonstrated that error judgments were driven by the number rather than the total duration of sequential stimulus presentations, the number and duration of stimuli are inevitably correlated in sequential presentations. This correlation empirically limits the purity of the characterization of "numerical error monitoring". The current work expanded the scope of numerical error monitoring as a form of "metric error monitoring" to numerical estimation based on simultaneously presented array of stimuli to control for temporal correlates. Our results show that numerical error monitoring ability applies to magnitude estimation in these more controlled experimental scenarios underlining its ubiquitous nature.
- Published
- 2018
30. Mice can count and optimize count-based decisions
- Author
-
Fuat Balcı and Bilgehan Çavdaroğlu
- Subjects
Male ,Schedule ,Reinforcement Schedule ,business.product_category ,Decision Making ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Fixed consecutive number ,050105 experimental psychology ,Mice ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,Reward ,Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,Statistics ,Psychophysics ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,Animals ,Learning ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Reinforcement ,Lever ,Behavior, Animal ,05 social sciences ,Scalar (physics) ,Numerosity adaptation effect ,Maximization ,Conditioning, Operant ,Psychology ,business ,Reinforcement, Psychology ,psychological phenomena and processes ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery - Abstract
Previous studies showed that rats and pigeons can count their responses, and the resultant count-based judgments exhibit the scalar property (also known as Weber's Law), a psychophysical property that also characterizes interval-timing behavior. Animals were found to take a nearly normative account of these well-established endogenous uncertainty characteristics in their time-based decision-making. On the other hand, no study has yet tested the implications of scalar property of numerosity representations for reward-rate maximization in count-based decision-making. The current study tested mice on a task that required them to press one lever for a minimum number of times before pressing the second lever to collect the armed reward (fixed consecutive number schedule, FCN). Fewer than necessary number of responses reset the response count without reinforcement, whereas emitting responses at least for the minimum number of times reset the response counter with reinforcement. Each mouse was tested with three different FCN schedules (FCN10, FCN20, FCN40). The number of responses emitted on the first lever before pressing the second lever constituted the main unit of analysis. Our findings for the first time showed that mice count their responses with scalar property. We then defined the reward-rate maximizing numerical decision strategies in this task based on the subject-based estimates of the endogenous counting uncertainty. Our results showed that mice learn to maximize the reward-rate by incorporating the uncertainty in their numerosity judgments into their count-based decisions. Our findings extend the scope of optimal temporal risk-assessment to the domain of count-based decision-making.
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
31. Optimal response rates in humans and rats
- Author
-
Russell M. Church, Fuat Balcı, David Freestone, and Patrick Simen
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Adolescent ,Decision Making ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Models, Psychological ,Article ,Rats ,Task (project management) ,Rats, Sprague-Dawley ,Young Adult ,Reward ,Animals ,Humans ,Female ,Reinforcement ,Psychology ,Social psychology ,Photic Stimulation ,Psychomotor Performance ,Ecology, Evolution, Behavior and Systematics ,Probability ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
The analysis of response rates has been highly influential in psychology, giving rise to many prominent theories of learning. There is, however, growing interest in explaining response rates, not as a global response to associations or value, but as a decision about how to space responses in time. Recently, researchers have shown that humans and mice can time a single response optimally; that is, in a way that maximizes reward. Here, we use the well-established differential reinforcement of low rates (DRL) timing task to show that humans and rats come close to optimizing reinforcement rate, but respond systematically faster than they should.
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
32. Interval Timing, Dopamine, and Motivation
- Author
-
Fuat Balcı, Balcı, Fuat (ORCID 0000-0003-3390-9352 & YÖK ID 51269), College of Social Sciences and Humanities, and Department of Psychology
- Subjects
Cognitive Neuroscience ,Rightward shift ,Dopaminergic ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Developmental psychology ,Clock hypothesis ,Interval (music) ,Multidisciplinary sciences ,Science and technology ,Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology ,Motivation ,Interval timing ,Averaging artifact ,Peak interval procedure ,Clock speed ,Dopamine clock hypothesis ,Dopamine ,medicine ,Psychology ,Neuroscience ,Applied Psychology ,medicine.drug - Abstract
The dopamine clock hypothesis suggests that the dopamine level determines the speed of the hypothetical internal clock. However, dopaminergic function has also been implicated for motivation and thus the effect of dopaminergic manipulations on timing behavior might also be independently mediated by altered motivational state. Studies that investigated the effect of motivational manipulations on peak responding are reviewed in this paper. The majority of these studies show that a higher reward magnitude leads to a leftward shift, whereas reward devaluation leads to a rightward shift in the initiation of timed anticipatory behavior, typically in the absence of an effect on the timing of response termination. Similar behavioral effects are also present in a number of studies that investigated the effect of dopamine agonists and dopamine-related genetic factors on peak responding. These results can be readily accounted for by independent modulation of decision-thresholds for the initiation and termination of timed responding., FP7 Marie Curie; Scientific and Technological Research Council of Turkey (TÜBİTAK); The Science Academy BAGEP Grant
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
33. Are you early or late?: Temporal error monitoring
- Author
-
Başak Akdoğan and Fuat Balcı
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Adolescent ,Magnitude (mathematics) ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,050105 experimental psychology ,Discrimination Learning ,03 medical and health sciences ,Judgment ,Young Adult ,0302 clinical medicine ,Developmental Neuroscience ,Statistics ,Reaction Time ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Attention ,Discrimination learning ,General Psychology ,05 social sciences ,Response time ,Variance (accounting) ,Time perception ,Degree (music) ,Interval (music) ,Time Perception ,Female ,Psychology ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery - Abstract
Temporal judgments regarding a target interval typically produce a nearly normally distributed reproduction times centered on the target with substantial variance. This phenomenon indicates that the majority of our temporal judgments are deviations from the target times, which are assumed to originate from the underlying timing uncertainty. Although humans were found to adapt their decisions in response to timing uncertainty, we do not know if they can accurately judge the direction and degree of their temporal errors. In this study, we asked participants to reproduce durations as accurately as possible. After each reproduction, participants were asked to retrospectively rate their confidence in their temporal estimates and to judge if their response time was earlier or later than the target interval. The results revealed that human participants are aware of both the direction and magnitude of their timing errors, pointing at an informationally rich temporal error monitoring ability. We further show that a sequential diffusion process can account for the detection of direction of errors as well as the qualitative features of the relationship of objective temporal errors with subjective confidence ratings and associated response times. (PsycINFO Database Record
- Published
- 2017
34. Timescale Invariance in the Pacemaker-Accumulator Family of Timing Models
- Author
-
Fuat Balcı, Elliot Andrew Ludvig, Patrick Simen, Francois Rivest, and Peter R. Killeen
- Subjects
business.industry ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Scalar expectancy ,Interval (mathematics) ,Extension (predicate logic) ,Scale invariance ,Poisson distribution ,Accumulator (cryptography) ,symbols.namesake ,Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology ,symbols ,Artificial intelligence ,Psychology ,Set (psychology) ,business ,Algorithm ,Applied Psychology ,Generator (mathematics) - Abstract
Pacemaker-accumulator (PA) systems have been the most popular kind of timing model in the half-century since their introduction by Treisman (1963). Many alternative timing models have been designed predicated on different assumptions, though the dominant PA model during this period — Gibbon and Church’s Scalar Expectancy Theory (SET) — invokes most of them. As in Treisman, SET’s implementation assumes a fixed-rate clock-pulse generator and encodes durations by storing average pulse counts; unlike Treisman’s model, SET’s decision process invokes Weber’s law of magnitude-comparison to account for timescale-invariant temporal precision in animal behavior. This is one way to deal with the ‘Poisson timing’ issue, in which relative temporal precision increases for longer durations, contrafactually, in a simplified version of Treisman’s model. First, we review the fact that this problem does not afflict Treisman’s model itself due to a key assumption not shared by SET. Second, we develop a contrasting PA model, an extension of Killeen and Fetterman’s Behavioral Theory of Timing that accumulates Poisson pulses up to a fixed criterion level, with pulse rates adapting to time different intervals. Like Treisman’s model, this time-adaptive, opponent Poisson, drift–diffusion model accounts for timescale invariance without first assuming Weber’s law. It also makes new predictions about response times and learning speed and connects interval timing to the popular drift–diffusion model of perceptual decision making. With at least three different routes to timescale invariance, the PA model family can provide a more compelling account of timed behavior than may be generally appreciated.
- Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
35. IS MATCHING INNATE?
- Author
-
Fuat Balcı, Kimberly S. Carbone, E. B. Papachristos, Daniel A. Gottlieb, Adam Philip King, Charles R. Gallistel, and Matthew Szalecki
- Subjects
Instinct ,Matching (statistics) ,Behavior, Animal ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Investment (macroeconomics) ,Mice, Inbred C57BL ,Mice ,Behavioral Neuroscience ,Law of effect ,Behavioral dynamics ,Statistics ,Animals ,Learning ,Female ,Animal behavior ,Reinforcement, Psychology ,Research Articles ,Simulation ,Mathematics - Abstract
Experimentally naive mice matched the proportions of their temporal investments (visit durations) in two feeding hoppers to the proportions of the food income (pellets per unit session time) derived from them in three experiments that varied the coupling between the behavioral investment and food income, from no coupling to strict coupling. Matching was observed from the outset; it did not improve with training. When the numbers of pellets received were proportional to time invested, investment was unstable, swinging abruptly from sustained, almost complete investment in one hopper, to sustained, almost complete investment in the other—in the absence of appropriate local fluctuations in returns (pellets obtained per time invested). The abruptness of the swings strongly constrains possible models. We suggest that matching reflects an innate (unconditioned) program that matches the ratio of expected visit durations to the ratio between the current estimates of expected incomes. A model that processes the income stream looking for changes in the income and generates discontinuous income estimates when a change is detected is shown to account for salient features of the data.
- Published
- 2007
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
36. Obsessive compulsive features predict cautious decision strategies
- Author
-
Fuat Balcı and Ceyla Erhan
- Subjects
Psychotherapist ,Physiology ,05 social sciences ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,General Medicine ,050105 experimental psychology ,Developmental psychology ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology ,Obsessive compulsive ,Physiology (medical) ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Psychology ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,General Psychology - Abstract
Introduction: Obsessive compulsive disorder (OCD) is occasionally characterized by decision-making deficits. Compared to the isolated analysis of the choice and response times, characterizing decision outputs at the level of latent processes can be a more powerful approach in revealing differences, even in subclinical cases. We hypothesized that participants with higher obsessive compulsive (OC) features would set their decision thresholds higher and thus make more cautious decisions. Method: We used a perceptual two-alternative forced-choice (2AFC) task (dot motion discrimination) to test this hypothesis in a non-clinical sample ( N = 74). We fitted the data with the diffusion model and evaluated the optimality of decision outputs. We also conducted exploratory analyses to reveal which subscales best predicted the differences at the level of latent decision processes. Results: Higher OC total scores in Maudsley and Padua scales significantly predicted higher threshold settings (cautiousness). The follow-up exploratory analyses with subscale scores showed that checking and rumination tendencies predicted higher threshold settings whereas washing tendency predicted faster non-decision times. Conclusions: Our primary results showed that participants with higher degrees of OC features exhibit more cautious decision making. Our exploratory analyses also revealed distinctions based on different types of OC features in both controlled (cautiousness in decision making) and automatic (faster non-decision times) elements of the decision process.
- Published
- 2015
37. Probabilistic numerical discrimination in mice
- Author
-
Bilgehan Çavdaroğlu, Fuat Balcı, and Dilara Berkay
- Subjects
Male ,business.product_category ,Decision Making ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Machine learning ,computer.software_genre ,Frequency ,050105 experimental psychology ,Task (project management) ,Discrimination Learning ,03 medical and health sciences ,Mice ,0302 clinical medicine ,Statistics ,Animals ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Discrimination learning ,Ecology, Evolution, Behavior and Systematics ,Probability ,Lever ,business.industry ,05 social sciences ,Probabilistic logic ,Numerosity adaptation effect ,Mathematical Concepts ,Mice, Inbred C57BL ,Interval (music) ,Categorization ,Conditioning, Operant ,Artificial intelligence ,business ,Psychology ,computer ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery - Abstract
Previous studies showed that both human and non-human animals can discriminate between different quantities (i.e., time intervals, numerosities) with a limited level of precision due to their endogenous/representational uncertainty. In addition, other studies have shown that subjects can modulate their temporal categorization responses adaptively by incorporating information gathered regarding probabilistic contingencies into their time-based decisions. Despite the psychophysical similarities between the interval timing and nonverbal counting functions, the sensitivity of count-based decisions to probabilistic information remains an unanswered question. In the current study, we investigated whether exogenous probabilistic information can be integrated into numerosity-based judgments by mice. In the task employed in this study, reward was presented either after few (i.e., 10) or many (i.e., 20) lever presses, the last of which had to be emitted on the lever associated with the corresponding trial type. In order to investigate the effect of probabilistic information on performance in this task, we manipulated the relative frequency of different trial types across different experimental conditions. We evaluated the behavioral performance of the animals under models that differed in terms of their assumptions regarding the cost of responding (e.g., logarithmically increasing vs. no response cost). Our results showed for the first time that mice could adaptively modulate their count-based decisions based on the experienced probabilistic contingencies in directions predicted by optimality.
- Published
- 2015
38. Stimulus probability effects on temporal bisection performance of mice (Mus musculus)
- Author
-
Fuat Balcı and Başak Akdoğan
- Subjects
Male ,05 social sciences ,Decision Making ,Probabilistic logic ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Stimulus (physiology) ,Choice Behavior ,050105 experimental psychology ,Mice, Inbred C57BL ,03 medical and health sciences ,Mice ,0302 clinical medicine ,Categorization ,Statistics ,Time Perception ,Reaction Time ,Temporal similarity ,Animals ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Decision process ,Reinforcement ,Psychology ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Ecology, Evolution, Behavior and Systematics ,Probability - Abstract
In the temporal bisection task, participants classify experienced stimulus durations as short or long based on their temporal similarity to previously learned reference durations. Temporal decision making in this task should be influenced by the experienced probabilities of the reference durations for adaptiveness. In this study, we tested the temporal bisection performance of mice (Mus musculus) under different short and long reference duration probability conditions implemented across two experimental phases. In Phase 1, the proportion of reference durations (compared to probe durations) was 0.5, whereas in Phase 2 it was increased to 0.8 to further examine the adjustment of choice behavior with more frequent reference duration presentations (under higher reinforcement rate). Our findings suggest that mice developed adaptive biases in their choice behaviors. These adjustments in choice behavior were nearly optimal as the mice maximized their gain to a great extent which required them to monitor stimulus probabilities as well as the level of variability in their temporal judgments. We further found that short but not long categorization response times were sensitive to stimulus probability manipulations, which in turn suggests an asymmetry between short and long categorizations. Finally, we investigated the latent decision processes underlying the bias manifested in subjects' choice behavior within the diffusion model framework. Our results revealed that probabilistic information influenced the starting point and the rate of evidence accumulation process. Overall, the stimulus probability effects on choice behavior were modulated by the reinforcement rate. Our findings illustrate that mice can adapt their temporal behaviors with respect to the probabilistic contingencies in the environment.
- Published
- 2015
39. Cross-domain transfer of quantitative discriminations: Is it all a matter of proportion?
- Author
-
Charles R. Gallistel and Fuat Balcı
- Subjects
Models, Statistical ,Extramural ,Transfer, Psychology ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Scale factor ,Domain (mathematical analysis) ,Discrete quantity ,Cognition ,Discrimination, Psychological ,Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,Duration (music) ,Transfer (computing) ,Statistics ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,Range (statistics) ,Humans ,Psychology ,Scaling - Abstract
Meck and Church (1983) estimated a 5:1 scale factor relating the mental magnitudes representing number to the mental magnitudes representing duration. We repeated their experiment with human subjects. We obtained transfer regardless of the objective scaling between the ranges; a 5:1 scaling for number versus duration (measured in seconds) was not necessary. We obtained transfer even when the proportions between the endpoints of the number range were different. We conclude that, at least in human subjects, transfer from a discrimination based on continuous quantity (duration) to a discrimination based on discrete quantity (number) is mediated by the cross-domain comparability of within-domain proportions. The results of our second and third experiments also suggest that the subjects compare a probe with a criterion determined by the range of stimuli tested rather than by trial-specific referents, in accordance with the pseudologistic model of Killeen, Fetterman, and Bizo (1997).
- Published
- 2006
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
40. Optimal time discrimination
- Author
-
Emine Gurbuz, Zeynep Ceyda Sayalı, Filiz Çoşkun, Fuat Balcı, Coşkun, Filiz, Sayalı, Zeynep Ceyda, Gürbüz, Emine, Balcı, Fuat (ORCID 0000-0003-3390-9352 & YÖK ID 51269), College of Social Sciences and Humanities, and Department of Psychology
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Time Factors ,Physiology ,Computer science ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Stimulus (physiology) ,behavioral disciplines and activities ,Choice Behavior ,Judgment ,Young Adult ,Discrimination, Psychological ,Physiology (medical) ,Statistics ,Biological psychology ,Psychology ,Reaction Time ,Humans ,General Psychology ,Probability ,Spatial Memory ,business.industry ,Experimental psychology ,Decision-making ,Interval timing ,Optimality ,Response times ,Temporal bisection ,Pattern recognition ,General Medicine ,Maximization ,Time optimal ,Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology ,Categorization ,Time Perception ,Linear Models ,Female ,Artificial intelligence ,business ,psychological phenomena and processes ,Photic Stimulation - Abstract
In the temporal bisection task, participants categorize experienced stimulus durations as short or long based on their similarity to previously acquired reference durations. Reward maximization in this task requires integrating endogenous timing uncertainty as well as exogenous probabilities of the reference durations into temporal judgements. We tested human participants on the temporal bisection task with different short and long reference duration probabilities (exogenous probability) in two separate test sessions. Incorrect categorizations were not penalized in Experiment 1 but were penalized in Experiment 2, leading to different levels of stringency in the reward functions that participants tried to maximize. We evaluated the judgements within the framework of optimality. Our participants adapted their choice behaviour in a nearly optimal fashion and earned nearly the maximum possible expected gain they could attain given their level of endogenous timing uncertainty and exogenous probabilities in both experiments. These results point to the optimality of human temporal risk assessment in the temporal bisection task. The long categorization response times (RTs) were overall faster than short categorization RTs, and short but not long categorization RTs were modulated by reference duration probability manipulations. These observations suggested an asymmetry between short and long categorizations in the temporal bisection task., FP7 Marie Curie; Scientific and Technological Research Council of Turkey (TÜBİTAK) 1001; BAGEP Grant from Bilim Akademisi-The Science Academy, Turkey
- Published
- 2014
41. Decision processes in temporal discrimination
- Author
-
Patrick Simen and Fuat Balcı
- Subjects
Male ,Adolescent ,Computer science ,Decision Making ,Response time ,Parameterized complexity ,Poison control ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,General Medicine ,Stimulus (physiology) ,Young Adult ,Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,Categorization ,Reward ,Salient ,Time Perception ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,Reaction Time ,Humans ,Attention ,Female ,Timer ,Decision model ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
The processing dynamics underlying temporal decisions and the response times they generate have received little attention in the study of interval timing. In contrast, models of other simple forms of decision making have been extensively investigated using response times, leading to a substantial disconnect between temporal and non-temporal decision theories. An overarching decision-theoretic framework that encompasses existing, non-temporal decision models may, however, account both for interval timing itself and for time-based decision-making. We sought evidence for this framework in the temporal discrimination performance of humans tested on the temporal bisection task. In this task, participants retrospectively categorized experienced stimulus durations as short or long based on their perceived similarity to two, remembered reference durations and were rewarded only for correct categorization of these references. Our analysis of choice proportions and response times suggests that a two-stage, sequential diffusion process, parameterized to maximize earned rewards, can account for salient patterns of bisection performance. The first diffusion stage times intervals by accumulating an endogenously noisy clock signal; the second stage makes decisions about the first-stage temporal representation by accumulating first-stage evidence corrupted by endogenous noise. Reward-maximization requires that the second-stage accumulation rate and starting point be based on the state of the first-stage timer at the end of the stimulus duration, and that estimates of non-decision-related delays should decrease as a function of stimulus duration. Results are in accord with these predictions and thus support an extension of the drift-diffusion model of static decision making to the domain of interval timing and temporal decisions.
- Published
- 2013
42. Epistasis effects of dopamine genes on interval timing and reward magnitude in humans
- Author
-
Bilgehan Çavdaroğlu, Fuat Balcı, Martin Wiener, and H. Branch Coslett
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Reinforcement Schedule ,Genotype ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,Dopamine ,Statistics as Topic ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Striatum ,Catechol O-Methyltransferase ,Polymorphism, Single Nucleotide ,Behavioral Neuroscience ,Young Adult ,Reward ,Polymorphism (computer science) ,medicine ,Reaction Time ,Humans ,Prefrontal cortex ,Dopamine transporter ,Analysis of Variance ,Dopamine Plasma Membrane Transport Proteins ,Sex Characteristics ,biology ,Receptors, Dopamine D2 ,Epistasis, Genetic ,nervous system ,Time Perception ,biology.protein ,Epistasis ,Female ,Analysis of variance ,Animal studies ,Psychology ,Neuroscience ,medicine.drug - Abstract
We tested human participants on a modified peak procedure in order to investigate the relation between interval timing and reward processing, and examine the interaction of this relation with three different dopamine-related gene polymorphisms. These gene polymorphisms affected the expression of catechol-o-methyltransferase, which catabolizes synaptic dopamine primarily in the prefrontal cortex (COMT Val158Met polymorphism), D2 dopamine receptors primarily in the striatum (DRD2/ANKK1-Taq1a polymorphism), and dopamine transporters, which clear synaptic dopamine in the striatum (DAT 3' VNTR variant). The inclusion of these polymorphisms allowed us to investigate dissociable aspects of the dopamine system and their interaction with reward magnitude manipulations in shaping timed behavior. These genes were chosen for their roles in reward processing and cortico-striatal information processing that have been implicated for interval timing. Consistent with recent animal studies, human participants initiated their timed anticipatory responding earlier when expecting a larger reward in the absence of any changes in the timing of response termination or perceived time. This effect however was specific to two out of four evaluated COMT and DRD2 polymorphism combinations that lead to high prefrontal dopamine coupled with high D2 density and low prefrontal dopamine coupled with low D2 density. Larger rewards also decreased timing precision indices, some of which interacted with the COMT polymorphism. Furthermore, the COMT polymorphism that leads to higher prefrontal dopamine resulted in weaker manifestation of memory variability (relative to threshold variability) in timed behavior. There was no effect of DAT polymorphisms on any of the core behavioral measures. These results suggest that the reward modulates decision thresholds rather than clock speed, and that these effects are specific to COMT and DRD2 epistasis effects that presumably constitute a balanced prefrontal and striatal dopamine transmission.
- Published
- 2012
43. Acquisition of decision making criteria: reward rate ultimately beats accuracy
- Author
-
Andrew M. Saxe, Fuat Balcı, Patrick Simen, Jessica A. Hughes, Philip Holmes, Ritwik K. Niyogi, and Jonathan D. Cohen
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Linguistics and Language ,Adolescent ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Reward-based selection ,Decision Making ,Motion Perception ,Poison control ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Residual ,Language and Linguistics ,Article ,Young Adult ,Discrimination, Psychological ,Reward ,Perception ,Statistics ,Psychophysics ,Reaction Time ,Humans ,Attention ,media_common ,Motivation ,Two-alternative forced choice ,Uncertainty ,Coherence (statistics) ,Multiple-criteria decision analysis ,Sensory Systems ,Pattern Recognition, Visual ,Practice, Psychological ,Female ,Probability Learning ,Psychology ,Social psychology - Abstract
Speed–accuracy trade-offs strongly influence the rate of reward that can be earned in many decision-making tasks. Previous reports suggest that human participants often adopt suboptimal speed–accuracy trade-offs in single session, two-alternative forced-choice tasks. We investigated whether humans acquired optimal speed–accuracy trade-offs when extensively trained with multiple signal qualities. When performance was characterized in terms of decision time and accuracy, our participants eventually performed nearly optimally in the case of higher signal qualities. Rather than adopting decision criteria that were individually optimal for each signal quality, participants adopted a single threshold that was nearly optimal for most signal qualities. However, setting a single threshold for different coherence conditions resulted in only negligible decrements in the maximum possible reward rate. Finally, we tested two hypotheses regarding the possible sources of suboptimal performance: (1) favoring accuracy over reward rate and (2) misestimating the reward rate due to timing uncertainty. Our findings provide support for both hypotheses, but also for the hypothesis that participants can learn to approach optimality. We find specifically that an accuracy bias dominates early performance, but diminishes greatly with practice. The residual discrepancy between optimal and observed performance can be explained by an adaptive response to uncertainty in time estimation.
- Published
- 2011
44. Erratum
- Author
-
Fuat Balcı
- Subjects
Neuropsychology and Physiological Psychology ,Physiology ,Physiology (medical) ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,General Medicine ,General Psychology - Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.