46,544 results on '"Reaction Time"'
Search Results
2. The brain hierarchically represents the past and future during multistep anticipation
- Author
-
Tarder-Stoll, Hannah, Baldassano, Christopher, and Aly, Mariam
- Subjects
Biological Psychology ,Information and Computing Sciences ,Psychology ,Neurosciences ,1.2 Psychological and socioeconomic processes ,1.1 Normal biological development and functioning ,Mental health ,Brain ,Hippocampus ,Humans ,Magnetic Resonance Imaging ,Brain Mapping ,Learning ,Reaction Time ,Adult ,Female ,Male ,Young Adult ,Anticipation ,Psychological ,Virtual Reality - Abstract
Memory for temporal structure enables both planning of future events and retrospection of past events. We investigated how the brain flexibly represents extended temporal sequences into the past and future during anticipation. Participants learned sequences of environments in immersive virtual reality. Pairs of sequences had the same environments in a different order, enabling context-specific learning. During fMRI, participants anticipated upcoming environments multiple steps into the future in a given sequence. Temporal structure was represented in the hippocampus and across higher-order visual regions (1) bidirectionally, with graded representations into the past and future and (2) hierarchically, with further events into the past and future represented in successively more anterior brain regions. In hippocampus, these bidirectional representations were context-specific, and suppression of far-away environments predicted response time costs in anticipation. Together, this work sheds light on how we flexibly represent sequential structure to enable planning over multiple timescales.
- Published
- 2024
3. Behavioral and neural measures of confidence using a novel auditory pitch identification task.
- Author
-
Tang, Tamara, Samaha, Jason, and Peters, Megan
- Subjects
Humans ,Male ,Female ,Adult ,Electroencephalography ,Reaction Time ,Young Adult ,Pitch Perception ,Acoustic Stimulation ,Metacognition ,Auditory Perception - Abstract
Observers can discriminate between correct versus incorrect perceptual decisions with feelings of confidence. The centro-parietal positivity build-up rate (CPP slope) has been suggested as a likely neural signature of accumulated evidence, which may guide both perceptual performance and confidence. However, CPP slope also covaries with reaction time, which also covaries with confidence in previous studies, and performance and confidence typically covary; thus, CPP slope may index signatures of perceptual performance rather than confidence per se. Moreover, perceptual metacognition-including neural correlates-has largely been studied in vision, with few exceptions. Thus, we lack understanding of domain-general neural signatures of perceptual metacognition outside vision. Here we designed a novel auditory pitch identification task and collected behavior with simultaneous 32-channel EEG in healthy adults. Participants saw two tone labels which varied in tonal distance on each trial (e.g., C vs D, C vs F), then heard a single auditory tone; they identified which label was correct and rated confidence. We found that pitch identification confidence varied with tonal distance, but performance, metacognitive sensitivity (trial-by-trial covariation of confidence with accuracy), and reaction time did not. Interestingly, however, while CPP slope covaried with performance and reaction time, it did not significantly covary with confidence. We interpret these results to mean that CPP slope is likely a signature of first-order perceptual processing and not confidence-specific signals or computations in auditory tasks. Our novel pitch identification task offers a valuable method to examine the neural correlates of auditory and domain-general perceptual confidence.
- Published
- 2024
4. Polygenic association between attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder liability and cognitive impairments
- Author
-
Vainieri, Isabella, Martin, Joanna, Rommel, Anna-Sophie, Asherson, Philip, Banaschewski, Tobias, Buitelaar, Jan, Cormand, Bru, Crosbie, Jennifer, Faraone, Stephen V, Franke, Barbara, Loo, Sandra K, Miranda, Ana, Manor, Iris, Oades, Robert D, Purves, Kirstin L, Ramos-Quiroga, J Antoni, Ribasés, Marta, Roeyers, Herbert, Rothenberger, Aribert, Schachar, Russell, Sergeant, Joseph, Steinhausen, Hans-Christoph, Vuijk, Pieter J, Doyle, Alysa E, and Kuntsi, Jonna
- Subjects
Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) ,Genetics ,Prevention ,Human Genome ,Mental Health ,2.1 Biological and endogenous factors ,Aetiology ,Adolescent ,Adult ,Child ,Humans ,Young Adult ,Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity ,Cognitive Dysfunction ,Genome-Wide Association Study ,Phenotype ,Reaction Time ,Case-Control Studies ,ADHD ,attention ,cognition ,inhibition ,polygenic risk scores ,reaction time variability ,Neurosciences ,Public Health and Health Services ,Psychology ,Psychiatry - Abstract
BackgroundA recent genome-wide association study (GWAS) identified 12 independent loci significantly associated with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Polygenic risk scores (PRS), derived from the GWAS, can be used to assess genetic overlap between ADHD and other traits. Using ADHD samples from several international sites, we derived PRS for ADHD from the recent GWAS to test whether genetic variants that contribute to ADHD also influence two cognitive functions that show strong association with ADHD: attention regulation and response inhibition, captured by reaction time variability (RTV) and commission errors (CE).MethodsThe discovery GWAS included 19 099 ADHD cases and 34 194 control participants. The combined target sample included 845 people with ADHD (age: 8-40 years). RTV and CE were available from reaction time and response inhibition tasks. ADHD PRS were calculated from the GWAS using a leave-one-study-out approach. Regression analyses were run to investigate whether ADHD PRS were associated with CE and RTV. Results across sites were combined via random effect meta-analyses.ResultsWhen combining the studies in meta-analyses, results were significant for RTV (R2 = 0.011, β = 0.088, p = 0.02) but not for CE (R2 = 0.011, β = 0.013, p = 0.732). No significant association was found between ADHD PRS and RTV or CE in any sample individually (p > 0.10).ConclusionsWe detected a significant association between PRS for ADHD and RTV (but not CE) in individuals with ADHD, suggesting that common genetic risk variants for ADHD influence attention regulation.
- Published
- 2022
5. Action video game play facilitates learning to learn.
- Author
-
Zhang, Ru-Yuan, Chopin, Adrien, Shibata, Kengo, Lu, Zhong-Lin, Jaeggi, Susanne, Buschkuehl, Martin, Green, C, and Bavelier, Daphne
- Subjects
Adult ,Attention ,Cognition ,Humans ,Learning ,Middle Aged ,Reaction Time ,Task Performance and Analysis ,Video Games ,Visual Perception ,Young Adult - Abstract
Previous work has demonstrated that action video game training produces enhancements in a wide range of cognitive abilities. Here we evaluate a possible mechanism by which such breadth of enhancement could be attained: that action game training enhances learning rates in new tasks (i.e., learning to learn). In an initial controlled intervention study, we show that individuals who were trained on action video games subsequently exhibited faster learning in the two cognitive domains that we tested, perception and working memory, as compared to individuals who trained on non-action games. We further confirmed the causal effect of action video game play on learning ability in a pre-registered follow-up study that included a larger number of participants, blinding, and measurements of participant expectations. Together, this work highlights enhanced learning speed for novel tasks as a mechanism through which action video game interventions may broadly improve task performance in the cognitive domain.
- Published
- 2021
6. History Modulates Early Sensory Processing of Salient Distractors.
- Author
-
Adam, Kirsten CS and Serences, John T
- Subjects
Clinical Research ,Eye Disease and Disorders of Vision ,Behavioral and Social Science ,Underpinning research ,1.1 Normal biological development and functioning ,Mental health ,Adult ,Attention ,Brain ,Female ,Humans ,Magnetic Resonance Imaging ,Male ,Neuropsychological Tests ,Pattern Recognition ,Visual ,Reaction Time ,Visual Cortex ,Visual Perception ,Young Adult ,attentional selection ,fMRI ,priority ,salience ,visual search ,Medical and Health Sciences ,Psychology and Cognitive Sciences ,Neurology & Neurosurgery - Abstract
To find important objects, we must focus on our goals, ignore distractions, and take our changing environment into account. This is formalized in models of visual search whereby goal-driven, stimulus-driven, and history-driven factors are integrated into a priority map that guides attention. Stimulus history robustly influences where attention is allocated even when the physical stimulus is the same: when a salient distractor is repeated over time, it captures attention less effectively. A key open question is how we come to ignore salient distractors when they are repeated. Goal-driven accounts propose that we use an active, expectation-driven mechanism to attenuate the distractor signal (e.g., predictive coding), whereas stimulus-driven accounts propose that the distractor signal is attenuated because of passive changes to neural activity and inter-item competition (e.g., adaptation). To test these competing accounts, we measured item-specific fMRI responses in human visual cortex during a visual search task where trial history was manipulated (colors unpredictably switched or were repeated). Consistent with a stimulus-driven account of history-based distractor suppression, we found that repeated singleton distractors were suppressed starting in V1, and distractor suppression did not increase in later visual areas. In contrast, we observed signatures of goal-driven target enhancement that were absent in V1, increased across visual areas, and were not modulated by stimulus history. Our data suggest that stimulus history does not alter goal-driven expectations, but rather modulates canonically stimulus-driven sensory responses to contribute to a temporally integrated representation of priority.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT Visual search refers to our ability to find what we are looking for in a cluttered visual world (e.g., finding your keys). To perform visual search, we must integrate information about our goals (e.g., "find the red keychain"), the environment (e.g., salient items capture your attention), and changes to the environment (i.e., stimulus history). Although stimulus history impacts behavior, the neural mechanisms that mediate history-driven effects remain debated. Here, we leveraged fMRI and multivariate analysis techniques to measure history-driven changes to the neural representation of items during visual search. We found that stimulus history influenced the representation of a salient "pop-out" distractor starting in V1, suggesting that stimulus history operates via modulations of early sensory processing rather than goal-driven expectations.
- Published
- 2021
7. Closed-loop Neurofeedback of Alpha Synchrony during Goal-directed Attention
- Author
-
Mishra, Jyoti, Lowenstein, Mira, Campusano, Richard, Hu, Yihan, Diaz-Delgado, Juan, Ayyoub, Jacqueline, Jain, Rajat, and Gazzaley, Adam
- Subjects
Basic Behavioral and Social Science ,Clinical Research ,Behavioral and Social Science ,Brain Disorders ,Mind and Body ,Clinical Trials and Supportive Activities ,Pediatric ,Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) ,Neurosciences ,Mental Health ,1.1 Normal biological development and functioning ,Underpinning research ,Neurological ,Mental health ,Adult ,Alpha Rhythm ,Attention ,Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity ,Cerebral Cortex ,Child ,Double-Blind Method ,Female ,Goals ,Humans ,Male ,Neurofeedback ,Neuronal Plasticity ,Reaction Time ,cognitive neurofeedback ,brain computer interface ,synchrony ,dorsal attention network ,Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder ,anticipation ,alpha suppression ,neuroplasticity ,attention deficit hyperactivity disorder ,attention ,closed-loop ,Medical and Health Sciences ,Psychology and Cognitive Sciences ,Neurology & Neurosurgery - Abstract
α Oscillations in sensory cortex, under frontal control, desynchronize during attentive preparation. Here, in a selective attention study with simultaneous EEG in humans of either sex, we first demonstrate that diminished anticipatory α synchrony between the mid-frontal region of the dorsal attention network and ventral visual sensory cortex [frontal-sensory synchrony (FSS)] significantly correlates with greater task performance. Then, in a double-blind, randomized controlled study in healthy adults, we implement closed-loop neurofeedback (NF) of the anticipatory α FSS signal over 10 d of training. We refer to this closed-loop experimental approach of rapid NF integrated within a cognitive task as cognitive NF (cNF). We show that cNF results in significant trial-by-trial modulation of the anticipatory α FSS measure during training, concomitant plasticity of stimulus-evoked α/θ responses, as well as transfer of benefits to response time (RT) improvements on a standard test of sustained attention. In a third study, we implement cNF training in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), replicating trial-by-trial modulation of the anticipatory α FSS signal as well as significant improvement of sustained attention RTs. These first findings demonstrate the basic mechanisms and translational utility of rapid cognitive-task-integrated NF.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT When humans prepare to attend to incoming sensory information, neural oscillations in the α band (8-14 Hz) undergo desynchronization under the control of prefrontal cortex. Here, in an attention study with electroencephalography, we first show that frontal-sensory synchrony (FSS) of α oscillations during attentive preparation significantly correlates with task performance. Then, in a randomized controlled study in healthy adults, we show that neurofeedback (NF) training of this α FSS signal within the attention task is feasible. We show that this rapid cognitive NF (cNF) approach engenders plasticity of stimulus-evoked neural responses, and improves performance on a standard test of sustained attention. In a final study, we implement cNF in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), replicating the improvement of sustained attention found in adults.
- Published
- 2021
8. Cognitive and Neural Correlates of Loneliness and Wisdom during Emotional Bias.
- Author
-
Grennan, Gillian, Balasubramani, Pragathi Priyadharsini, Alim, Fahad, Zafar-Khan, Mariam, Lee, Ellen E, Jeste, Dilip V, and Mishra, Jyoti
- Subjects
Basic Behavioral and Social Science ,Behavioral and Social Science ,Neurosciences ,Mental Health ,Mind and Body ,Underpinning research ,1.2 Psychological and socioeconomic processes ,Mental health ,Adolescent ,Adult ,Aged ,Aged ,80 and over ,Cognition ,Electroencephalography ,Emotions ,Female ,Happiness ,Humans ,Loneliness ,Male ,Middle Aged ,Photic Stimulation ,Reaction Time ,Thinking ,Young Adult ,emotion bias ,temporo-parietal junction ,insula ,EEG ,happiness ,social threat ,Psychology ,Cognitive Sciences ,Experimental Psychology - Abstract
Loneliness and wisdom have opposing impacts on health and well-being, yet their neuro-cognitive bases have never been simultaneously investigated. In this study of 147 healthy human subjects sampled across the adult lifespan, we simultaneously studied the cognitive and neural correlates of loneliness and wisdom in the context of an emotion bias task. Aligned with the social threat framework of loneliness, we found that loneliness was associated with reduced speed of processing when angry emotional stimuli were presented to bias cognition. In contrast, we found that wisdom was associated with greater speed of processing when happy emotions biased cognition. Source models of electroencephalographic data showed that loneliness was specifically associated with enhanced angry stimulus-driven theta activity in the left transverse temporal region of interest, which is located in the area of the temporoparietal junction (TPJ), while wisdom was specifically related to increased TPJ theta activity during happy stimulus processing. Additionally, enhanced attentiveness to threatening stimuli for lonelier individuals was observed as greater beta activity in left superior parietal cortex, while wisdom significantly related to enhanced happy stimulus-evoked alpha activity in the left insula. Our results demonstrate emotion-context driven modulations in cognitive neural circuits by loneliness versus wisdom.
- Published
- 2021
9. The Goal-Dependence of Level-1 and Level-2 Visual Perspective Calculation
- Author
-
Todd, Andrew R, Cameron, C Daryl, and Simpson, Austin J
- Subjects
Biological Psychology ,Cognitive and Computational Psychology ,Psychology ,Neurosciences ,Adult ,Goals ,Humans ,Reaction Time ,Visual Perception ,automaticity ,implicit mentalizing ,perspective taking ,process dissociation ,theory of mind ,Cognitive Sciences ,Experimental Psychology ,Cognitive and computational psychology - Abstract
Does tracking another agent's visual perspective depend on having a goal-albeit a remote one-to do so? In 5 experiments using indirect measures of visual perspective taking with a cartoon avatar, we examined whether and how adult perceivers' processing goals shape the incidental tracking of what objects the avatar sees (Level-1 perspective taking) and how the avatar sees those objects (Level-2 perspective taking). Process dissociation analyses, which aim to isolate calculation of the avatar's perspective as the process of focal interest, revealed that both Level-1 and Level-2 perspective calculation were consistently weaker when the avatar's perspective was less relevant for participants' own processing goals. This pattern of goal-dependent perspective tracking was also evident in behavioral analyses of interference from the avatar's differing perspective when reporting one's own perspective (i.e., altercentric interference). These results suggest that, although Level-1 and Level-2 visual perspective calculation may operate unintentionally, both also appear to depend on perceivers' processing goals. More generally, these findings advance understanding of processes underlying visual perspective taking and the conditional automaticity with which those processes operate. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2021 APA, all rights reserved).
- Published
- 2021
10. Reward-driven attention alters perceived salience
- Author
-
Qin, Nan, Gu, Ruolei, Xue, Jingming, Chen, Chuansheng, and Zhang, Mingxia
- Subjects
Clinical Research ,Neurosciences ,Eye Disease and Disorders of Vision ,Basic Behavioral and Social Science ,Behavioral and Social Science ,2.3 Psychological ,social and economic factors ,Aetiology ,Mental health ,Adolescent ,Adult ,Attention ,Female ,Humans ,Male ,Reaction Time ,Reward ,Visual Perception ,Young Adult ,reward-driven attention ,low-level ,visual perception ,perceived contrast ,Medical and Health Sciences ,Psychology and Cognitive Sciences ,Experimental Psychology - Abstract
Many studies have revealed that reward-associated features capture attention. Neurophysiological evidence further suggests that this reward-driven attention effect modulates visual processes by enhancing low-level visual salience. However, no behavioral study to date has directly examined whether reward-driven attention changes how people see. Combining the two-phase paradigm with a psychophysical method, the current study found that compared with nonsalient cues associated with lower reward, the nonsalient cues associated with higher reward captured more attention, and increased the perceived contrast of the subsequent stimuli. This is the first direct behavioral evidence of the effect of reward-driven attention on low-level visual perception.
- Published
- 2021
11. Automated recognition of spontaneous facial expression in individuals with autism spectrum disorder: parsing response variability
- Author
-
Bangerter, Abigail, Chatterjee, Meenakshi, Manfredonia, Joseph, Manyakov, Nikolay V, Ness, Seth, Boice, Matthew A, Skalkin, Andrew, Goodwin, Matthew S, Dawson, Geraldine, Hendren, Robert, Leventhal, Bennett, Shic, Frederick, and Pandina, Gahan
- Subjects
Biomedical and Clinical Sciences ,Biological Psychology ,Clinical Sciences ,Neurosciences ,Psychology ,Autism ,Brain Disorders ,Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities (IDD) ,Mental Health ,Mental health ,Adolescent ,Adult ,Algorithms ,Autism Spectrum Disorder ,Case-Control Studies ,Child ,Child ,Preschool ,Clinical Trials as Topic ,Emotions ,Facial Expression ,Female ,Humans ,Male ,Middle Aged ,Models ,Theoretical ,Multicenter Studies as Topic ,Photic Stimulation ,Reaction Time ,Recognition ,Psychology ,Young Adult ,Autism spectrum disorder ,Emotional regulation ,Facial expression ,Impulsive behavior ,Clinical sciences ,Biological psychology - Abstract
BackgroundReduction or differences in facial expression are a core diagnostic feature of autism spectrum disorder (ASD), yet evidence regarding the extent of this discrepancy is limited and inconsistent. Use of automated facial expression detection technology enables accurate and efficient tracking of facial expressions that has potential to identify individual response differences.MethodsChildren and adults with ASD (N = 124) and typically developing (TD, N = 41) were shown short clips of "funny videos." Using automated facial analysis software, we investigated differences between ASD and TD groups and within the ASD group in evidence of facial action unit (AU) activation related to the expression of positive facial expression, in particular, a smile.ResultsIndividuals with ASD on average showed less evidence of facial AUs (AU12, AU6) relating to positive facial expression, compared to the TD group (p < .05, r = - 0.17). Using Gaussian mixture model for clustering, we identified two distinct distributions within the ASD group, which were then compared to the TD group. One subgroup (n = 35), termed "over-responsive," expressed more intense positive facial expressions in response to the videos than the TD group (p < .001, r = 0.31). The second subgroup (n = 89), ("under-responsive"), displayed fewer, less intense positive facial expressions in response to videos than the TD group (p < .001; r = - 0.36). The over-responsive subgroup differed from the under-responsive subgroup in age and caregiver-reported impulsivity (p < .05, r = 0.21). Reduced expression in the under-responsive, but not the over-responsive group, was related to caregiver-reported social withdrawal (p < .01, r = - 0.3).LimitationsThis exploratory study does not account for multiple comparisons, and future work will have to ascertain the strength and reproducibility of all results. Reduced displays of positive facial expressions do not mean individuals with ASD do not experience positive emotions.ConclusionsIndividuals with ASD differed from the TD group in their facial expressions of positive emotion in response to "funny videos." Identification of subgroups based on response may help in parsing heterogeneity in ASD and enable targeting of treatment based on subtypes.Trial registrationClinicalTrials.gov, NCT02299700. Registration date: November 24, 2014.
- Published
- 2020
12. Adaptiveness in proactive control engagement in children and adults
- Author
-
Chevalier, Nicolas, Meaney, Julie Anne, Traut, Hilary Joy, and Munakata, Yuko
- Subjects
Clinical Research ,Pediatric ,Adolescent ,Adult ,Child ,Evoked Potentials ,Executive Function ,Female ,Humans ,Male ,Reaction Time ,Reproducibility of Results ,Young Adult ,Cognitive control ,Executive function ,Proactive control ,Event-related potentials ,Pupillometry ,Children ,Clinical Sciences ,Neurosciences ,Cognitive Sciences - Abstract
Age-related progress in cognitive control reflects more frequent engagement of proactive control during childhood. As proactive preparation for an upcoming task is adaptive only when the task can be reliably predicted, progress in proactive control engagement may rely on more efficient use of contextual cue reliability. Developmental progress may also reflect increasing efficiency in how proactive control is engaged, making this control mode more advantageous with age. To address these possibilities, 6-year-olds, 9-year-olds, and adults completed three versions of a cued task-switching paradigm in which contextual cue reliability was manipulated. When contextual cues were reliable (but not unreliable or uninformative), all age groups showed greater pupil dilation and a more pronounced (pre)cue-locked posterior positivity associated with faster response times, suggesting adaptive engagement of proactive task selection. However, adults additionally showed a larger contingent negative variation (CNV) predicting a further reduction in response times with reliable cues, suggesting motor preparation in adults but not children. Thus, early developing use of contextual cue reliability promotes adaptiveness in proactive control engagement from early childhood; yet, less efficient motor preparation in children makes this control mode overall less advantageous in childhood than adulthood.
- Published
- 2020
13. Decline in kidney function over the course of adulthood and cognitive function in midlife.
- Author
-
Sedaghat, Sanaz, Sorond, Farzaneh, Yaffe, Kristine, Sidney, Stephen, Kramer, Holly J, Jacobs, David R, Launer, Lenore J, and Carnethon, Mercedes R
- Subjects
Biomedical and Clinical Sciences ,Neurosciences ,Clinical Sciences ,Behavioral and Social Science ,Cardiovascular ,Aging ,Clinical Research ,Kidney Disease ,Renal and urogenital ,Good Health and Well Being ,Adult ,Albumins ,Cardiovascular Diseases ,Cognition ,Cognitive Dysfunction ,Creatinine ,Executive Function ,Female ,Glomerular Filtration Rate ,Humans ,Kidney Failure ,Chronic ,Kidney Function Tests ,Longitudinal Studies ,Male ,Middle Aged ,Neuropsychological Tests ,Prospective Studies ,Reaction Time ,Risk Factors ,Young Adult ,Cognitive Sciences ,Neurology & Neurosurgery ,Clinical sciences - Abstract
ObjectiveTo test the hypothesis that end-stage renal disease (ESRD) risk exposure during young adulthood is related to worse cognitive performance in midlife.MethodsWe included 2,604 participants from the population-based Coronary Artery Risk Development in Young Adults (CARDIA) Study (mean age 35 years, 54% women, 45% Black). Estimated glomerular filtration rate and albumin-to-creatinine ratio were measured every 5 years at year (Y) 10 through Y30. At each visit, moderate/high risk of ESRD according to the Kidney Disease: Improving Global Outcomes guidelines (estimated glomerular filtration rate 30 mg/g) was defined, totaled over examinations, and categorized into 0 episodes, 1 episode, and >1 episodes of ESRD risk. At Y30, participants underwent global and multidomain cognitive assessment. We used analysis of covariance to assess the association of ESRD risk categories with cognitive function, controlling for cardiovascular risk factors.ResultsOver the course of 20 years, 427 participants (16% of the study population) had ≥1 episodes of ESRD risk exposure. Individuals with more risk episodes had lower composite cognitive function (p < 0.001), psychomotor speed (p < 0.001), and executive function (p = 0.007). All these associations were independent of sociodemographic status and cardiovascular risk factors.ConclusionsIn this population-based longitudinal study, we show that episodes of decline in kidney function over the young-adulthood course are associated with worse cognitive performance at midlife. Preserving kidney function in young age needs to be investigated as a potential strategy to preserve cognitive function in midlife.
- Published
- 2020
14. Sustained attention and vigilance deficits associated with HIV and a history of methamphetamine dependence
- Author
-
Pocuca, Nina, Young, Jared W, MacQueen, David A, Letendre, Scott, Heaton, Robert K, Geyer, Mark A, Perry, William, Grant, Igor, Minassian, Arpi, and Center, the Translational Methamphetamine AIDS Research
- Subjects
Medical Microbiology ,Biomedical and Clinical Sciences ,Immunology ,Brain Disorders ,Methamphetamine ,Substance Misuse ,Infectious Diseases ,HIV/AIDS ,Neurosciences ,Drug Abuse (NIDA only) ,Clinical Research ,6.1 Pharmaceuticals ,Evaluation of treatments and therapeutic interventions ,Infection ,Good Health and Well Being ,Adult ,Amphetamine-Related Disorders ,Attention ,Central Nervous System Stimulants ,Cognition ,Female ,HIV Infections ,Humans ,Male ,Middle Aged ,Neurocognitive Disorders ,Reaction Time ,Viral Load ,Young Adult ,Human immunodeficiency virus ,Sustained attention ,Vigilance ,Continuous performance test ,Translational Methamphetamine AIDS Research Center ,Medical and Health Sciences ,Psychology and Cognitive Sciences ,Substance Abuse ,Biochemistry and cell biology ,Pharmacology and pharmaceutical sciences ,Epidemiology - Abstract
BackgroundHuman immunodeficiency virus (HIV)-associated neurocognitive disorders persist in the era of antiretroviral therapy. One factor that is elevated among persons with HIV (PWH) and independently associated with neurocognitive impairment is methamphetamine dependence (METH). Such dependence may further increase cognitive impairment among PWH, by delaying HIV diagnosis (and thus, antiretroviral therapy initiation), which has been posited to account for persistent cognitive impairment among PWH, despite subsequent treatment-related viral load suppression (VLS;
- Published
- 2020
15. People with schizophrenia do not show the normal benefits of social versus nonsocial attentional cues.
- Author
-
Catalano, Lauren T, Green, Michael F, Wynn, Jonathan K, and Lee, Junghee
- Subjects
Humans ,Social Behavior ,Motivation ,Cues ,Social Perception ,Attention ,Reaction Time ,Neuropsychological Tests ,Schizophrenic Psychology ,Fixation ,Ocular ,Adult ,Middle Aged ,Female ,Male ,Young Adult ,Behavioral and Social Science ,Schizophrenia ,Mental Health ,Brain Disorders ,Clinical Research ,Serious Mental Illness ,Mental health ,schizophrenia ,social orienting ,gaze cueing ,social attention ,social motivation ,Neurosciences ,Psychology ,Cognitive Sciences ,Experimental Psychology - Abstract
ObjectiveSchizophrenia is associated with impairments in social motivation. Social attention has been proposed as an underlying mechanism for social motivation. However, studies in schizophrenia have rarely examined social attention, and none of these studies examined the effects with rapidly presented stimuli.MethodThe current study examined whether individuals with schizophrenia have reduced social attention and whether reduced social attention was related to social motivation deficits (measured with the Clinical Assessment Interview for Negative Symptoms) and decreased social functioning (Role Functioning Scale). Thirty-seven outpatients with schizophrenia and 29 healthy participants completed a gaze cueing task with directional social cues (eye gaze) and nonsocial cues (arrows) at varying stimulus onset asynchronies.ResultsAs predicted, schizophrenia participants had reduced social attention relative to nonsocial attention, compared with healthy participants. Healthy participants were quicker to respond to social cues than nonsocial cues, but schizophrenia participants did not exhibit this same pattern. Schizophrenia participants showed higher accuracy when targets appeared in the same location as a directional cue (i.e., congruency) for nonsocial, but not social, cues. Contrary to expectations, reduced social attention was not significantly correlated with clinically rated social motivation deficits or decreased social functioning in the schizophrenia group.ConclusionThese findings provide evidence for social attention deficits in schizophrenia, but without a clear mapping of its influence on social motivation. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights reserved).
- Published
- 2020
16. Interplay between persistent activity and activity-silent dynamics in the prefrontal cortex underlies serial biases in working memory
- Author
-
Barbosa, Joao, Stein, Heike, Martinez, Rebecca L, Galan-Gadea, Adrià, Li, Sihai, Dalmau, Josep, Adam, Kirsten CS, Valls-Solé, Josep, Constantinidis, Christos, and Compte, Albert
- Subjects
Biomedical and Clinical Sciences ,Biological Psychology ,Cognitive and Computational Psychology ,Neurosciences ,Psychology ,Brain Disorders ,1.1 Normal biological development and functioning ,Underpinning research ,1.2 Psychological and socioeconomic processes ,Neurological ,Adolescent ,Adult ,Animals ,Electroencephalography ,Female ,Humans ,Macaca mulatta ,Male ,Memory ,Short-Term ,Neurons ,Prefrontal Cortex ,Reaction Time ,Saccades ,Spatial Memory ,Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation ,Young Adult ,Cognitive Sciences ,Neurology & Neurosurgery ,Biological psychology - Abstract
Persistent neuronal spiking has long been considered the mechanism underlying working memory, but recent proposals argue for alternative 'activity-silent' substrates. Using monkey and human electrophysiology data, we show here that attractor dynamics that control neural spiking during mnemonic periods interact with activity-silent mechanisms in the prefrontal cortex (PFC). This interaction allows memory reactivations, which enhance serial biases in spatial working memory. Stimulus information was not decodable between trials, but remained present in activity-silent traces inferred from spiking synchrony in the PFC. Just before the new stimulus, this latent trace was reignited into activity that recapitulated the previous stimulus representation. Importantly, the reactivation strength correlated with the strength of serial biases in both monkeys and humans, as predicted by a computational model that integrates activity-based and activity-silent mechanisms. Finally, single-pulse transcranial magnetic stimulation applied to the human PFC between successive trials enhanced serial biases, thus demonstrating the causal role of prefrontal reactivations in determining working-memory behavior.
- Published
- 2020
17. Neurocomputational Changes in Inhibitory Control Associated With Prolonged Exposure Therapy
- Author
-
Harlé, Katia M, Spadoni, Andrea D, Norman, Sonya B, and Simmons, Alan N
- Subjects
Clinical Research ,Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD) ,Anxiety Disorders ,Behavioral and Social Science ,Neurosciences ,Mental Health ,Brain Disorders ,Mental health ,Adult ,Bayes Theorem ,Humans ,Implosive Therapy ,Inhibition ,Psychological ,Magnetic Resonance Imaging ,Male ,Reaction Time ,Stress Disorders ,Post-Traumatic ,Veterans ,Psychology ,Psychiatry - Abstract
Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is associated with inhibitory control dysfunction that extends beyond difficulties inhibiting trauma-related intrusions. Inhibitory learning has been proposed as a potential mechanism of change underlying the effectiveness of extinction-based therapies such as prolonged exposure (PE), a first-line treatment for PTSD. To identify neurocognitive markers of change in inhibitory learning associated with PE, we applied a Bayesian learning model to the analysis of neuroimaging data collected during an inhibitory control task, both before and after PE treatment. Veterans (N = 20) with combat-related PTSD completed a stop-signal task (SST) while undergoing fMRI at time points immediately before and after PE treatment. Participants exhibited a small, significant improvement in performance on the SST, as demonstrated by longer reaction times and improved inhibition accuracy. Amplitude of neural activation associated with a signed prediction error (SPE; i.e., the discrepancy between actual outcome and model-based expectation of needing to stop) in the right caudate decreased from baseline to posttreatment assessment. Change in model-based activation was modulated by performance accuracy, with a decrease in positive SPE activation observed on successful trials, d = 0.79, and a reduction in negative SPE activation on error trials, d = 0.74. The decrease in SPE-related activation on successful stop trials was correlated with PTSD symptom reduction. These results are consistent with the notion that PE may help broadly strengthen inhibitory learning and the development of more accurate model-based predictions, which may thus facilitate change in cognitions in response to trauma-related cues and help reduce PTSD symptoms.
- Published
- 2020
18. The architecture of working memory: Features from multiple remembered objects produce parallel, coactive guidance of attention in visual search.
- Author
-
Bahle, Brett, Thayer, Daniel, Mordkoff, J, and Hollingworth, Andrew
- Subjects
Adolescent ,Adult ,Attention ,Cues ,Female ,Humans ,Male ,Memory ,Short-Term ,Mental Recall ,Neuropsychological Tests ,Reaction Time ,Visual Perception ,Young Adult - Abstract
Theories of working memory (WM) differ in their claims about the number of items that can be maintained in a state that directly interacts with other, ongoing cognitive operations (termed the focus of attention). A similar debate has arisen in the literature on visual working memory (VWM), focused on the number of items that can simultaneously interact with attentional priority. In 3 experiments, we used a redundancy-gain paradigm to provide a comprehensive test of the latter question. Participants searched for 2 cued features (e.g., a color and a shape) within a search array. The cued feature values changed on a trial-by-trial basis, requiring VWM. The target (when present) could match 1 of the cued features (single-target trials) or both cued features (redundant-target trials). We tested whether response time distributions contained a substantial proportion of trials with redundant-target responses that were faster than predicted by 2 independent guidance processes operating in parallel (i.e., violations of the race-model inequality). Violations are consistent with a coactive architecture in which both cued values guide attention in parallel and sum on the priority map. Robust violations were observed in all cases predicted by the hypothesis that multiple items in VWM can guide attention simultaneously, and these results were inconsistent with the hypothesis that guidance is limited to a single item simultaneously. When considered in the larger context of the literature on VWM and attention, the present results are consistent with a model of WM architecture in which the focus of attention can maintain multiple, independent representations. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights reserved).
- Published
- 2020
19. Increased Influence of a Previously Attended Feature in People With Schizophrenia
- Author
-
Leonard, Carly J, Robinson, Benjamin M, Hahn, Britta, Gold, James M, and Luck, Steven J
- Subjects
Biological Psychology ,Cognitive and Computational Psychology ,Psychology ,Clinical Research ,Mental Health ,Behavioral and Social Science ,Schizophrenia ,Brain Disorders ,Neurosciences ,Mental health ,Adult ,Attention ,Cognition ,Female ,Humans ,Male ,Memory ,Short-Term ,Middle Aged ,Neuropsychological Tests ,Reaction Time ,Schizophrenic Psychology ,Visual Perception ,priming ,attention ,schizophrenia ,Cognitive Sciences ,Clinical Psychology ,Applied and developmental psychology ,Clinical and health psychology ,Cognitive and computational psychology - Abstract
Everyday functioning requires the appropriate allocation of visual attention, which is achieved through multiple mechanisms of attentional guidance. Traditional theories have focused on top-down and bottom-up factors, but implicit learning from recent experience ("selection history") also has a substantial impact on attentional allocation. The present experiment examined the influence of intertrial priming on attentional guidance in people with schizophrenia and matched control subjects. Participants searched for a color pop-out target, which switched randomly between a red target among blue distractors and a blue target among red distractors. We found that performance on the current trial was more influenced by the previous-trial target color in people with schizophrenia than in control subjects. Moreover, this implicit priming effect was greater in individuals with lower working memory capacity (as measured in a separate task). These results suggest that intertrial priming has an exaggerated impact on attentional guidance in people with schizophrenia and that this is associated with other aspects of impaired cognition. Overall, these results are consistent with the hyperfocusing hypothesis, which proposes that a single underlying attentional abnormality may explain a range of atypical effects across perception, attention, and cognition in schizophrenia. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights reserved).
- Published
- 2020
20. People with schizophrenia show enhanced cognitive costs of maintaining a single item in working memory
- Author
-
Gold, James M, Bansal, Sonia, Gaspar, John M, Chen, Shuo, Robinson, Benjamin M, Hahn, Britta, and Luck, Steven J
- Subjects
Biological Psychology ,Cognitive and Computational Psychology ,Biomedical and Clinical Sciences ,Psychology ,Schizophrenia ,Neurosciences ,Mental Health ,Behavioral and Social Science ,Brain Disorders ,Clinical Research ,2.1 Biological and endogenous factors ,Aetiology ,Mental health ,Adult ,Attention ,Case-Control Studies ,Cognition ,Cognitive Dysfunction ,Female ,Humans ,Male ,Memory Disorders ,Memory ,Short-Term ,Middle Aged ,Photic Stimulation ,Reaction Time ,Schizophrenic Psychology ,Young Adult ,Cognitive impairment ,reaction time ,working memory ,Public Health and Health Services ,Psychiatry ,Clinical sciences ,Biological psychology ,Clinical and health psychology - Abstract
BackgroundWorking memory (WM) deficits are seen as a core deficit in schizophrenia, implicated in the broad cognitive impairment seen in the illness. Here we examine the impact of WM storage of a single item on the operation of other cognitive systems.MethodsWe studied 37 healthy controls (HCS) and 43 people with schizophrenia (PSZ). Each trial consisted of a sequence of two potential target stimuli, T1 and T2. T1 was a letter presented for 100 ms. After delays of 100-800 ms, T2 was presented. T2 was a 1 or a 2 and required a speeded response. In one condition, subjects were instructed to ignore T1 but respond to T2. In another condition, they were required to report T1 after making their speeded response to T2 (i.e. to make a speeded T2 response while holding T1 in WM).ResultsPSZ were dramatically slowed at responding to T2 when T1 was held in WM. A repeated measures ANOVA yielded main effects of group, delay, and condition with a group by condition interaction (p's < 0.001). Across delays, the slowing of the T2 response when required to hold T1 in memory, relative to ignoring T1, was nearly 3 times higher in PSZ than HCS (633 v. 219 ms).ConclusionsWhereas previous studies have focused on reduced storage capacity, the present study found that PSZ are impaired at performing tasks while they are successfully maintaining a single item in WM. This may play a role in the broad cognitive impairment seen in PSZ.
- Published
- 2020
21. The Confidence Database.
- Author
-
Rahnev, Dobromir, Desender, Kobe, Lee, Alan, Adler, William, Aguilar-Lleyda, David, Akdoğan, Başak, Arbuzova, Polina, Atlas, Lauren, Balcı, Fuat, Bang, Ji, Bègue, Indrit, Birney, Damian, Brady, Timothy, Calder-Travis, Joshua, Chetverikov, Andrey, Clark, Torin, Davranche, Karen, Denison, Rachel, Dildine, Troy, Double, Kit, Duyan, Yalçın, Faivre, Nathan, Fallow, Kaitlyn, Filevich, Elisa, Gajdos, Thibault, Gallagher, Regan, de Gardelle, Vincent, Gherman, Sabina, Haddara, Nadia, Hainguerlot, Marine, Hsu, Tzu-Yu, Hu, Xiao, Iturrate, Iñaki, Jaquiery, Matt, Kantner, Justin, Koculak, Marcin, Konishi, Mahiko, Koß, Christina, Kvam, Peter, Kwok, Sze, Lebreton, Maël, Lempert, Karolina, Ming Lo, Chien, Luo, Liang, Maniscalco, Brian, Martin, Antonio, Massoni, Sébastien, Matthews, Julian, Mazancieux, Audrey, Merfeld, Daniel, OHora, Denis, Palser, Eleanor, Paulewicz, Borysław, Pereira, Michael, Peters, Caroline, Philiastides, Marios, Pfuhl, Gerit, Prieto, Fernanda, Rausch, Manuel, Recht, Samuel, Reyes, Gabriel, Rouault, Marion, Sackur, Jérôme, Sadeghi, Saeedeh, Samaha, Jason, Seow, Tricia, Shekhar, Medha, Sherman, Maxine, Siedlecka, Marta, Skóra, Zuzanna, Song, Chen, Soto, David, Sun, Sai, van Boxtel, Jeroen, Wang, Shuo, Weidemann, Christoph, Weindel, Gabriel, Wierzchoń, Michał, Xu, Xinming, Ye, Qun, Yeon, Jiwon, Zou, Futing, and Zylberberg, Ariel
- Subjects
Adult ,Choice Behavior ,Databases ,Factual ,Datasets as Topic ,Humans ,Mental Processes ,Metacognition ,Psychometrics ,Reaction Time ,Task Performance and Analysis - Abstract
Understanding how people rate their confidence is critical for the characterization of a wide range of perceptual, memory, motor and cognitive processes. To enable the continued exploration of these processes, we created a large database of confidence studies spanning a broad set of paradigms, participant populations and fields of study. The data from each study are structured in a common, easy-to-use format that can be easily imported and analysed using multiple software packages. Each dataset is accompanied by an explanation regarding the nature of the collected data. At the time of publication, the Confidence Database (which is available at https://osf.io/s46pr/) contained 145 datasets with data from more than 8,700 participants and almost 4 million trials. The database will remain open for new submissions indefinitely and is expected to continue to grow. Here we show the usefulness of this large collection of datasets in four different analyses that provide precise estimations of several foundational confidence-related effects.
- Published
- 2020
22. Flexible weighting of target features based on distractor context
- Author
-
Lee, Jeongmi and Geng, Joy J
- Subjects
Biological Psychology ,Psychology ,Adult ,Attentional Bias ,Color Perception ,Cues ,Female ,Humans ,Learning ,Male ,Orientation ,Spatial ,Probability ,Reaction Time ,Visual Perception ,Young Adult ,Attention ,Selective ,Theoretical and Computational Models ,Visual search ,Attention: Selective ,Attention ,Cognitive Sciences ,Experimental Psychology ,Biological psychology ,Cognitive and computational psychology - Abstract
Models of attention posit that attentional priority is established by summing the saliency and relevancy signals from feature-selective maps. The dimension-weighting account further hypothesizes that information from each feature-selective map is weighted based on expectations of how informative each dimension will be. In the current studies, we investigated the question of whether attentional biases to the features of a conjunction target (color and orientation) differ when one dimension is expected to be more diagnostic of the target. In a series of color-orientation conjunction search tasks, observers saw an exact cue for the upcoming target, while the probability of distractors sharing a target feature in each dimension was manipulated. In one context, distractors were more likely to share the target color, and in another, distractors were more likely to share the target orientation. The results indicated that despite an overall bias toward color, attentional priority to each target feature was flexibly adjusted according to distractor context: RT and accuracy performance was better when the diagnostic feature was expected than unexpected. This occurred both when the distractor context was learned implicitly and explicitly. These results suggest that feature-based enhancement can occur selectively for the dimension expected to be most informative in distinguishing the target from distractors.
- Published
- 2020
23. The influence of nicotine metabolic rate on working memory over 6 hours of abstinence from nicotine
- Author
-
Nardone, Natalie, Shahid, Marian, Strasser, Andrew A, Dempsey, Delia A, and Benowitz, Neal L
- Subjects
Pharmacology and Pharmaceutical Sciences ,Biomedical and Clinical Sciences ,Behavioral and Social Science ,Substance Misuse ,Tobacco ,Clinical Research ,Tobacco Smoke and Health ,Prevention ,Drug Abuse (NIDA only) ,Neurosciences ,Basic Behavioral and Social Science ,Good Health and Well Being ,Adult ,Cigarette Smoking ,Cognition ,Female ,Humans ,Magnetic Resonance Spectroscopy ,Male ,Memory ,Short-Term ,Middle Aged ,Nicotine ,Prospective Studies ,Reaction Time ,Smoking Cessation ,Time Factors ,Young Adult ,Nicotine metabolite ratio ,NMR ,Smoking ,Working memory ,Neurology & Neurosurgery ,Pharmacology and pharmaceutical sciences - Abstract
BackgroundA faster rate of nicotine metabolism has been associated with smoking more cigarettes, greater nicotine withdrawal symptoms, and lower smoking quit rates. However, the association between nicotine metabolic rate (NMR) and cognitive functioning during withdrawal has not been determined.MethodsWe compared cognitive function in 121 fast or slow nicotine metabolizers after smoking, and at 3 and 6 h of nicotine abstinence. Cognitive functioning was assessed using N-back working memory tests with outcomes of accuracy and processing speed. Participants smoked two cigarettes and then abstained from smoking for 6 h. N-back tests were administered after smoking (0 h) and at 3 and 6 h of nicotine abstinence.ResultsAn effect of processing speed was found over time on the 2-back, in that participants had significantly longer average reaction times when the stimuli presented did not match the target letter. NMR was not significantly associated with the processing speed change over time. Within-race differences in working memory were evident in that Caucasian fast metabolizers had significantly poorer accuracy and processing speed.ConclusionsMinimal change in working memory over 6 h of nicotine abstinence was observed. Overall, NMR was not significantly associated with the change in processing speed, however Caucasian fast metabolizers displayed poorer accuracy and processing speed at discrete time points.
- Published
- 2020
24. Decomposing the constituent oscillatory dynamics underlying mismatch negativity generation in schizophrenia: Distinct relationships to clinical and cognitive functioning
- Author
-
Hochberger, WC, Joshi, YB, Zhang, W, Thomas, ML, investigators, the Consortium of Genomics in Schizophrenia, Braff, DL, Swerdlow, NR, and Light, GA
- Subjects
Pharmacology and Pharmaceutical Sciences ,Medical Physiology ,Biomedical and Clinical Sciences ,Serious Mental Illness ,Neurosciences ,Behavioral and Social Science ,Schizophrenia ,Mental Health ,Brain Disorders ,Clinical Research ,Mental health ,Acoustic Stimulation ,Adult ,Brain ,Cognition ,Electroencephalography ,Evoked Potentials ,Female ,Humans ,Male ,Middle Aged ,Neuropsychological Tests ,Reaction Time ,Mismatch negativity ,Early auditory information processing ,COGS-2 ,Consortium of Genomics in Schizophrenia (COGS) investigators ,Medical and Health Sciences ,Psychology and Cognitive Sciences ,Experimental Psychology ,Medical physiology - Abstract
Abnormalities in early auditory information processing (EAIP) contribute to higher-order deficits in cognition and psychosocial functioning in schizophrenia. A passive auditory oddball paradigm is commonly used to evoke event-related potential (ERP) measures of EAIP reflecting auditory sensory registration and deviance detection, including mismatch negativity (MMN) and P3a responses. MMN and P3a have been extensively studied in healthy subjects and neuropsychiatric patient populations and are increasingly used as translational biomarkers in the development of novel therapeutics. Despite widespread use, relatively few studies have examined the constituent oscillatory elements and the extent to which sensory registration and deviance detection represent distinct or intercorrelated processes. This study aimed to determine the factor structure and clinical correlates of these oscillatory measures in schizophrenia patients (n = 706) and healthy comparison subjects (n = 615) who underwent clinical, cognitive, and functional characterization and EEG testing via their participation in the Consortium of Genomics in Schizophrenia (COGS-2) study. Results revealed significant deficits in theta-band (4-7 Hz) evoked power and phase locking in patients. Exploratory factor analyses of both ERP and oscillatory measures revealed two dissociable factors reflecting sensory registration and deviance detection. While each factor shared a significant correlation with social cognition, the deviance detection factor had a unique relationship to multiple cognitive and clinical domains. Results support the continued advancement of functionally relevant oscillatory measures underlying EAIP in the development of precognitive therapeutics.
- Published
- 2019
25. Mild acute stress improves response speed without impairing accuracy or interference control in two selective attention tasks: Implications for theories of stress and cognition.
- Author
-
Shields, Grant, Rivers, Andrew, Ramey, Michelle, Yonelinas, Andrew, and Trainor, Brian
- Subjects
Acute stress ,Attention ,Motor control ,Reaction time ,Response speed ,Stress severity ,Adult ,Attention ,Cognition ,Executive Function ,Female ,Humans ,Hydrocortisone ,Male ,Neuropsychological Tests ,Reaction Time ,Saliva ,Stress ,Psychological ,Young Adult - Abstract
Acute stress is generally thought to impair performance on tasks thought to rely on selective attention. This effect has been well established for moderate to severe stressors, but no study has examined how a mild stressor-the most common type of stressor-influences selective attention. In addition, no study to date has examined how stress influences the component processes involved in overall selective attention task performance, such as controlled attention, automatic attentional activation, decision-making, and motor abilities. To address these issues, we randomly assigned 107 participants to a mild acute stress or control condition. As expected, the mild acute stress condition showed a small but significant increase in cortisol relative to the control condition. Following the stressor, we assessed attention with two separate flanker tasks. One of these tasks was optimized to investigate component attentional processes using computational cognitive modeling, whereas the other task employed mouse-tracking to illustrate how response conflict unfolded over time. The results for both tasks showed that mild acute stress decreased response time (i.e., increased response speed) without influencing accuracy or interference control. Further, computational modeling and mouse-tracking analyses indicated that these effects were due to faster motor action execution time for chosen actions. Intriguingly, however, cortisol responses were unrelated to any of the observed effects of mild stress. These results have implications for theories of stress and cognition, and highlight the importance of considering motor processes in understanding the effects of stress on cognitive task performance.
- Published
- 2019
26. Human motor cortical beta bursts relate to movement planning and response errors.
- Author
-
Little, Simon, Bonaiuto, James, Barnes, Gareth, and Bestmann, Sven
- Subjects
Motor Cortex ,Humans ,Beta Rhythm ,Cortical Synchronization ,Magnetoencephalography ,Reaction Time ,Evoked Potentials ,Movement ,Adult ,Female ,Male ,2.1 Biological and endogenous factors ,Aetiology ,Biological Sciences ,Agricultural and Veterinary Sciences ,Medical and Health Sciences ,Developmental Biology - Abstract
Motor cortical beta activity (13-30 Hz) is a hallmark signature of healthy and pathological movement, but its behavioural relevance remains unclear. Using high-precision magnetoencephalography (MEG), we show that during the classical event-related desynchronisation (ERD) and event-related synchronisation (ERS) periods, motor cortical beta activity in individual trials (n > 12,000) is dominated by high amplitude, transient, and infrequent bursts. Beta burst probability closely matched the trial-averaged beta amplitude in both the pre- and post-movement periods, but individual bursts were spatially more focal than the classical ERS peak. Furthermore, prior to movement (ERD period), beta burst timing was related to the degree of motor preparation, with later bursts resulting in delayed response times. Following movement (ERS period), the first beta burst was delayed by approximately 100 milliseconds when an incorrect response was made. Overall, beta burst timing was a stronger predictor of single trial behaviour than beta burst rate or single trial beta amplitude. This transient nature of motor cortical beta provides new constraints for theories of its role in information processing within and across cortical circuits, and its functional relevance for behaviour in both healthy and pathological movement.
- Published
- 2019
27. Lower extremity long-latency reflexes differentiate walking function after stroke
- Author
-
Banks, Caitlin L, Little, Virginia L, Walker, Eric R, and Patten, Carolynn
- Subjects
Biomedical and Clinical Sciences ,Neurosciences ,Clinical Research ,Brain Disorders ,Stroke ,Rehabilitation ,Adult ,Aged ,Evoked Potentials ,Motor ,Female ,Humans ,Lower Extremity ,Male ,Middle Aged ,Muscle ,Skeletal ,Reaction Time ,Reflex ,Reflex ,Stretch ,Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation ,Walking ,Long-latency reflex ,Transcortical reflex ,Reciprocal inhibition ,Medical and Health Sciences ,Psychology and Cognitive Sciences ,Neurology & Neurosurgery - Abstract
The neural mechanisms of walking impairment after stroke are not well characterized. Specifically, there is a need for understanding the mechanisms of impaired plantarflexor power generation in late stance. Here, we investigated the association between two neurophysiologic markers, the long-latency reflex (LLR) response and dynamic facilitation of antagonist motor-evoked responses, and walking function. Fourteen individuals with chronic post-stroke hemiparesis and thirteen healthy controls performed both isometric and dynamic plantarflexion. Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) assessed supraspinal drive to the tibialis anterior. LLR activity was assessed during dynamic voluntary plantarflexion and individuals post-stroke were classified as either LLR present (LLR+) or absent (LLR-). All healthy controls and nine individuals post-stroke exhibited LLRs, while five did not. LLR+ individuals revealed higher clinical scores, walking speeds, and greater ankle plantarflexor power during walking compared to LLR- individuals. LLR- individuals exhibited exaggerated responses to TMS during dynamic plantarflexion relative to healthy controls. The LLR- subset revealed dysfunctional modulation of stretch responses and antagonist supraspinal drive relative to healthy controls and the higher functioning LLR+ individuals post-stroke. These abnormal physiologic responses allow for characterization of individuals post-stroke along a dimension that is clinically relevant and provides additional information beyond standard behavioral assessments. These findings provide an opportunity to distinguish among the heterogeneity of lower extremity motor impairments present following stroke by associating them with responses at the nervous system level.
- Published
- 2019
28. Mild acute stress improves response speed without impairing accuracy or interference control in two selective attention tasks: Implications for theories of stress and cognition.
- Author
-
Shields, Grant S, Rivers, Andrew M, Ramey, Michelle M, Trainor, Brian C, and Yonelinas, Andrew P
- Subjects
Saliva ,Humans ,Hydrocortisone ,Stress ,Psychological ,Cognition ,Attention ,Reaction Time ,Neuropsychological Tests ,Adult ,Female ,Male ,Young Adult ,Executive Function ,Acute stress ,Motor control ,Reaction time ,Response speed ,Stress severity ,Mental Health ,Basic Behavioral and Social Science ,Behavioral and Social Science ,Neurosciences ,Mind and Body ,Brain Disorders ,Clinical Research ,Medical and Health Sciences ,Psychology and Cognitive Sciences ,Psychiatry - Abstract
Acute stress is generally thought to impair performance on tasks thought to rely on selective attention. This effect has been well established for moderate to severe stressors, but no study has examined how a mild stressor-the most common type of stressor-influences selective attention. In addition, no study to date has examined how stress influences the component processes involved in overall selective attention task performance, such as controlled attention, automatic attentional activation, decision-making, and motor abilities. To address these issues, we randomly assigned 107 participants to a mild acute stress or control condition. As expected, the mild acute stress condition showed a small but significant increase in cortisol relative to the control condition. Following the stressor, we assessed attention with two separate flanker tasks. One of these tasks was optimized to investigate component attentional processes using computational cognitive modeling, whereas the other task employed mouse-tracking to illustrate how response conflict unfolded over time. The results for both tasks showed that mild acute stress decreased response time (i.e., increased response speed) without influencing accuracy or interference control. Further, computational modeling and mouse-tracking analyses indicated that these effects were due to faster motor action execution time for chosen actions. Intriguingly, however, cortisol responses were unrelated to any of the observed effects of mild stress. These results have implications for theories of stress and cognition, and highlight the importance of considering motor processes in understanding the effects of stress on cognitive task performance.
- Published
- 2019
29. A large-scale analysis of task switching practice effects across the lifespan
- Author
-
Steyvers, Mark, Hawkins, Guy E, Karayanidis, Frini, and Brown, Scott D
- Subjects
Behavioral and Social Science ,Aging ,Basic Behavioral and Social Science ,Clinical Research ,1.2 Psychological and socioeconomic processes ,Underpinning research ,1.1 Normal biological development and functioning ,Generic health relevance ,Mental health ,Adult ,Aged ,Aged ,80 and over ,Cognition ,Female ,Humans ,Male ,Middle Aged ,Psychomotor Performance ,Reaction Time ,task switching ,cognitive control ,practice effects ,Bayesian modeling ,aging effects - Abstract
An important feature of human cognition is the ability to flexibly and efficiently adapt behavior in response to continuously changing contextual demands. We leverage a large-scale dataset from Lumosity, an online cognitive-training platform, to investigate how cognitive processes involved in cued switching between tasks are affected by level of task practice across the adult lifespan. We develop a computational account of task switching that specifies the temporal dynamics of activating task-relevant representations and inhibiting task-irrelevant representations and how they vary with extended task practice across a number of age groups. Practice modulates the level of activation of the task-relevant representation and improves the rate at which this information becomes available, but has little effect on the task-irrelevant representation. While long-term practice improves performance across all age groups, it has a greater effect on older adults. Indeed, extensive task practice can make older individuals functionally similar to less-practiced younger individuals, especially for cognitive measures that focus on the rate at which task-relevant information becomes available.
- Published
- 2019
30. Sleep restriction impairs maximal jump performance and joint coordination in elite athletes
- Author
-
Mah, Cheri D, Sparks, Aaron J, Samaan, Michael A, Souza, Richard B, and Luke, Anthony
- Subjects
Allied Health and Rehabilitation Science ,Biomedical and Clinical Sciences ,Health Sciences ,Sports Science and Exercise ,Behavioral and Social Science ,Clinical Research ,Sleep Research ,Actigraphy ,Adult ,Athletes ,Athletic Performance ,Cross-Over Studies ,Humans ,Male ,Psychomotor Performance ,Reaction Time ,Sleep ,Athlete ,biomechanics ,drop vertical jump ,sleep restriction ,response time ,Human Movement and Sports Sciences ,Curriculum and Pedagogy ,Sport Sciences ,Clinical sciences ,Sports science and exercise ,Applied and developmental psychology - Abstract
The study objective was to examine the effects of three days of sleep restriction on maximal jump performance and joint coordination. Eleven elite cyclists obtained a one-week baseline of habitual sleep then restricted sleep to 4 h/night (SR) for three nights assessed through self-report and actigraphy. Pre and post-intervention measures were a box drop maximal vertical jump with 3D motion capture to assess physical performance and biomechanical changes, and Psychomotor Vigilance Task (PVT) assessed changes in response time. Associations between biomechanical, physical, and cognitive performance measures were assessed. Participants restricted reported sleep from 7.4 ± 0.5 h/night at baseline to 4.0 ± 0.2 h/night and actigraphy indicated 6.7 ± 0.7 to 3.7 ± 0.2 h/night. Following SR, jump height decreased (0.44 ± 0.09 vs. 0.42 ± 0.10 m, p = 0.02, g = 0.21). Hip sagittal/knee frontal (Δ15.5°, p = 0.04, g = 0.40) and hip frontal/knee frontal (Δ11.0°, p < 0.01, g = 0.44) plane coordination variability increased after SR. Hip sagittal/knee frontal plane coordination variability after SR was associated with increasingly slower PVT response time (r = 0.63, p = 0.03). These findings suggest SR for three days decreased maximal jump performance. SR increased joint coordination variability and was associated with greater impairment in response time. SR leads to deviations from preferred movement patterns, which may have implications for decrements in athlete performance and increased injury risk.
- Published
- 2019
31. The Quality of Response Time Data Inference: A Blinded, Collaborative Assessment of the Validity of Cognitive Models
- Author
-
Dutilh, Gilles, Annis, Jeffrey, Brown, Scott D, Cassey, Peter, Evans, Nathan J, Grasman, Raoul PPP, Hawkins, Guy E, Heathcote, Andrew, Holmes, William R, Krypotos, Angelos-Miltiadis, Kupitz, Colin N, Leite, Fábio P, Lerche, Veronika, Lin, Yi-Shin, Logan, Gordon D, Palmeri, Thomas J, Starns, Jeffrey J, Trueblood, Jennifer S, van Maanen, Leendert, van Ravenzwaaij, Don, Vandekerckhove, Joachim, Visser, Ingmar, Voss, Andreas, White, Corey N, Wiecki, Thomas V, Rieskamp, Jörg, and Donkin, Chris
- Subjects
Adult ,Cognition ,Female ,Humans ,Male ,Models ,Psychological ,Models ,Statistical ,Reaction Time ,Reproducibility of Results ,Single-Blind Method ,Validity ,Cognitive modeling ,Response Times ,Diffusion Model ,LBA ,Psychology ,Cognitive Sciences ,Experimental Psychology - Abstract
Most data analyses rely on models. To complement statistical models, psychologists have developed cognitive models, which translate observed variables into psychologically interesting constructs. Response time models, in particular, assume that response time and accuracy are the observed expression of latent variables including 1) ease of processing, 2) response caution, 3) response bias, and 4) non-decision time. Inferences about these psychological factors, hinge upon the validity of the models' parameters. Here, we use a blinded, collaborative approach to assess the validity of such model-based inferences. Seventeen teams of researchers analyzed the same 14 data sets. In each of these two-condition data sets, we manipulated properties of participants' behavior in a two-alternative forced choice task. The contributing teams were blind to the manipulations, and had to infer what aspect of behavior was changed using their method of choice. The contributors chose to employ a variety of models, estimation methods, and inference procedures. Our results show that, although conclusions were similar across different methods, these "modeler's degrees of freedom" did affect their inferences. Interestingly, many of the simpler approaches yielded as robust and accurate inferences as the more complex methods. We recommend that, in general, cognitive models become a typical analysis tool for response time data. In particular, we argue that the simpler models and procedures are sufficient for standard experimental designs. We finish by outlining situations in which more complicated models and methods may be necessary, and discuss potential pitfalls when interpreting the output from response time models.
- Published
- 2019
32. Examining HPA-axis functioning as a mediator of the relationship between depression and cognition across the adult lifespan
- Author
-
Karstens, Aimee James, Korzun, Inez, Avery, Erich T, Kassel, Michelle T, Keelan, Rachel, Kales, Helen, Abercrombie, Heather, Eisenlohr-Moul, Tory, Langenecker, Scott A, and Weisenbach, Sara
- Subjects
Biological Psychology ,Clinical and Health Psychology ,Psychology ,Neurosciences ,Aging ,Mental Health ,Depression ,Mind and Body ,Clinical Research ,Brain Disorders ,Behavioral and Social Science ,Mental health ,Adolescent ,Adult ,Aged ,Aged ,80 and over ,Cognition ,Depressive Disorder ,Major ,Executive Function ,Female ,Humans ,Hydrocortisone ,Hypothalamo-Hypophyseal System ,Longevity ,Male ,Memory ,Middle Aged ,Neuropsychological Tests ,Pituitary-Adrenal System ,Reaction Time ,Saliva ,Young Adult ,cortisol ,memory ,speeded processing ,cognition ,HPA-axis ,Cognitive Sciences ,Experimental Psychology - Abstract
Altered HPA-axis functioning is a hypothesized mechanism for worsened cognition in depression. The current study examines the indirect effects of depression on processing speed, executive functioning, and memory as a function of the HPA-axis. 38 individuals with a depression diagnosis and 50 healthy controls (HCs) aged 18-86 underwent neuropsychological testing and at-home diurnal salivary cortisol collection. Depression was assessed via structured clinical interviews and rating scales. Cognitive composite scores were derived from factor analysis. Daytime cortisol exposure was estimated using area under the curve (AUC). Depression was associated with higher cortisol levels and slower processing speed . A significant suppression effect of AUC was present on the relationship between depression and processing speed. Limitations include the cross-sectional design and limited sample heterogeneity. Though poorly modulated HPA-axis is one proposed mechanism of cognitive alterations in depression, our results did not support this conclusion for processing speed. Alternative mechanisms should be considered to inform interventions to target cognitive alterations in depression.
- Published
- 2019
33. Movement Improves the Quality of Temporal Perception and Decision Making
- Author
-
Wiener, Martin, Zhou, Weiwei, Bader, Farah, and Joiner, Wilsaan M
- Subjects
Biological Psychology ,Cognitive and Computational Psychology ,Psychology ,Behavioral and Social Science ,Clinical Research ,1.1 Normal biological development and functioning ,1.2 Psychological and socioeconomic processes ,Underpinning research ,Adult ,Decision Making ,Female ,Humans ,Male ,Psychomotor Performance ,Reaction Time ,Time Perception ,Young Adult ,decision-making ,movement ,perception ,time perception ,Neurosciences - Abstract
A critical aspect of behavior is that mobile organisms must be able to precisely determine where and when to move. A better understanding of the mechanisms underlying precise movement timing and action planning is therefore crucial to understanding how we interact with the world around us. Recent evidence suggests that our experience of time is directly and intrinsically computed within the motor system, consistent with the theory of embodied cognition. To investigate the role of the motor system, we tested human subjects (n = 40) on a novel task combining reaching and time estimation. In this task, subjects were required to move a robotic manipulandum to one of two physical locations to categorize a concurrently timed suprasecond. Critically, subjects were divided into two groups: one in which movement during the interval was unrestricted and one in which they were restricted from moving until the stimulus interval had elapsed. Our results revealed a higher degree of precision for subjects in the free-moving group. A further experiment (n = 14) verified that these findings were not due to proximity to the target, counting strategies, bias, or movement length. A final experiment (n = 10) replicated these findings using a within-subjects design, performing a time reproduction task, in which movement during encoding of the interval led to more precise performance. Our findings suggest that time estimation may be instantiated within the motor system as an ongoing readout of timing judgment and confidence.
- Published
- 2019
34. A model of the uncertainty effects in choice reaction time that includes a major contribution from effector selection.
- Author
-
Wright, Charles E, Marino, Valerie F, Chubb, Charles, and Mann, Daniel
- Subjects
Humans ,Uncertainty ,Choice Behavior ,Psychomotor Performance ,Reaction Time ,Models ,Psychological ,Adult ,Female ,Male ,Hick's law ,choice RT ,stimulus-response mapping ,effector selection ,cognitive architecture ,Psychology ,Cognitive Sciences ,Experimental Psychology - Abstract
Hick's law describes the relation between choice reaction time (RT) and the number of stimulus-response alternatives (NA). For over half a century, this uncertainty effect has been ascribed primarily to the time taken to map a stimulus to its associated response. Here, data from 2 experiments suggests that selection of the appropriate effector-the particular body part to make a response-also contributes substantially to the uncertainty effect. This insight is important both for our understanding of basic cognitive architecture and because many classic experiments studying stimulus-response mapping have confounded NA with the number of effectors. Our data also suggest that, when stimuli are spatial and linked to the responses in an intuitively simple layout, the time required for stimulus-response mapping depends minimally on the NA, independent of effector. Experiment 1 demonstrated that in order to account for the complex patterns of uncertainty effects observed when stimulus type (spatial vs. symbolic), response mode (typing, with multiple effectors vs. touching with a single, known effector), and participant population (skilled vs. novice typists) are all manipulated a model is required that includes effector selection, along with stimulus-response mapping, and a proper treatment of stimulus-response repetitions. Using spatial indicator stimuli that minimized the contributions of stimulus-response mapping, Experiment 2 compared 4 effector conditions-the factorial combination of 1 or 3 fingers on one or both hands. The results showed that the increase in the uncertainty effect associated with the number of effectors is negatively accelerated and possibly additive across the variation of hands and fingers. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2019 APA, all rights reserved).
- Published
- 2019
35. Altered Brain Activation During Memory Retrieval Precedes and Predicts Conversion to Psychosis in Individuals at Clinical High Risk.
- Author
-
Cao, Hengyi, McEwen, Sarah C, Chung, Yoonho, Chén, Oliver Y, Bearden, Carrie E, Addington, Jean, Goodyear, Bradley, Cadenhead, Kristin S, Mirzakhanian, Heline, Cornblatt, Barbara A, Carrión, Ricardo E, Mathalon, Daniel H, McGlashan, Thomas H, Perkins, Diana O, Belger, Aysenil, Seidman, Larry J, Thermenos, Heidi, Tsuang, Ming T, van Erp, Theo GM, Walker, Elaine F, Hamann, Stephan, Anticevic, Alan, Woods, Scott W, and Cannon, Tyrone D
- Subjects
Clinical Research ,Serious Mental Illness ,Neurosciences ,Mental Health ,Schizophrenia ,Brain Disorders ,2.1 Biological and endogenous factors ,2.3 Psychological ,social and economic factors ,Aetiology ,Mental health ,Adolescent ,Adult ,Association Learning ,Cerebral Cortex ,Disease Progression ,Female ,Functional Neuroimaging ,Humans ,Longitudinal Studies ,Magnetic Resonance Imaging ,Male ,Mental Recall ,Prodromal Symptoms ,Psychotic Disorders ,Reaction Time ,Risk ,Young Adult ,fMRI ,associative memory ,memory retrieval ,psychosis ,clinical high risk ,Medical and Health Sciences ,Psychology and Cognitive Sciences ,Psychiatry - Abstract
Memory deficits are a hallmark of psychotic disorders such as schizophrenia. However, whether the neural dysfunction underlying these deficits is present before the onset of illness and potentially predicts conversion to psychosis is unclear. In this study, we investigated brain functional alterations during memory processing in a sample of 155 individuals at clinical high risk (including 18 subjects who later converted to full psychosis) and 108 healthy controls drawn from the second phase of the North American Prodrome Longitudinal Study (NAPLS-2). All participants underwent functional magnetic resonance imaging with a paired-associate memory paradigm at the point of recruitment and were clinically followed up for approximately 2 years. We found that at baseline, subjects at high risk showed significantly higher activation during memory retrieval in the prefrontal, parietal, and bilateral temporal cortices (PFWE < .035). This effect was more pronounced in converters than nonconverters and was particularly manifested in unmedicated subjects (P < .001). The hyperactivation was significantly correlated with retrieval reaction time during scan in converters (P = .009) but not in nonconverters and controls, suggesting an exaggerated retrieval effort. These findings suggest that hyperactivation during memory retrieval may mark processes associated with conversion to psychosis, and such measures have potential as biomarkers for psychosis prediction.
- Published
- 2019
36. Beta-band activity in medial prefrontal cortex predicts source memory encoding and retrieval accuracy.
- Author
-
Subramaniam, Karuna, Hinkley, Leighton BN, Mizuiri, Danielle, Kothare, Hardik, Cai, Chang, Garrett, Coleman, Findlay, Anne, Houde, John F, and Nagarajan, Srikantan S
- Subjects
Prefrontal Cortex ,Humans ,Magnetic Resonance Imaging ,Beta Rhythm ,Brain Mapping ,Cognition ,Memory ,Mental Recall ,Decision Making ,Reaction Time ,Adult ,Middle Aged ,Female ,Male ,Young Adult - Abstract
Reality monitoring is defined as the ability to distinguish internally self-generated information from externally-derived information. The medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) is a key brain region subserving reality monitoring and has been shown to be activated specifically during the retrieval of self-generated information. However, it is unclear if mPFC is activated during the encoding of self-generated information into memory. If so, it is important to understand whether successful retrieval of self-generated information critically depends on enhanced neural activity within mPFC during initial encoding of this self-generated information. We used magnetoencephalographic imaging (MEGI) to determine the timing and location of cortical activity during a reality-monitoring task involving self generated contextual source memory encoding and retrieval. We found both during encoding and retrieval of self-generated information, when compared to externally-derived information, mPFC showed significant task induced oscillatory power modulation in the beta-band. During initial encoding of self-generated information, greater mPFC beta-band power reductions occurred within a time window of -700 ms to -500 ms prior to vocalization. This increased activity in mPFC was not observed during encoding of externally-derived information. Additionally, increased mPFC activity during encoding of self-generated information predicted subsequent retrieval accuracy of this self-generated information. Beta-band activity in mPFC was also observed during the initial retrieval of self-generated information within a time window of 300 to 500 ms following stimulus onset and correlated with accurate retrieval performance of self-generated information. Together, these results further highlight the importance of mPFC in mediating the initial generation and awareness of participants' internal thoughts.
- Published
- 2019
37. Biases in processing of mood-congruent facial expressions in depression
- Author
-
Van Vleet, Thomas, Stark-Inbar, Alit, Merzenich, Michael M, Jordan, Joshua T, Wallace, Deanna L, Lee, Morgan B, Dawes, Heather E, Chang, Edward F, and Nahum, Mor
- Subjects
Biological Psychology ,Psychology ,Clinical Research ,Behavioral and Social Science ,Brain Disorders ,Depression ,Mind and Body ,Mental Health ,Mental health ,Adult ,Affect ,Bias ,Cognition ,Depressive Disorder ,Major ,Emotions ,Facial Expression ,Female ,Happiness ,Humans ,Male ,Middle Aged ,Reaction Time ,Young Adult ,Major depressive disorder ,MDD ,Mood disorders ,Affect perception ,Processing bias ,Medical and Health Sciences ,Psychology and Cognitive Sciences ,Psychiatry ,Clinical sciences ,Clinical and health psychology - Abstract
Cognitive models of depression suggest that depressed individuals exhibit a tendency to attribute negative meaning to neutral stimuli, and enhanced processing of mood-congruent stimuli. However, evidence thus far has been inconsistent. In this study, we sought to identify both differential interpretation of neutral information as well as emotion processing biases associated with depression. Fifty adult participants completed standardized mood-related questionnaires, a novel immediate mood scale questionnaire (IMS-12), and a novel task, Emotion Matcher, in which they were required to indicate whether pairs of emotional faces show the same expression or not. We found that overall success rate and reaction time on the Emotion Matcher task did not differ as a function of severity of depression. However, more depressed participants had significantly worse performance when presented with sad-neutral face pairs, as well as increased reaction times to happy-happy pairs. In addition, accuracy of the sad-neutral pairs was found to be significantly associated with depression severity in a regression model. Our study provides partial support for the mood-congruent hypothesis, revealing only a potential bias in interpretation of sad and neutral expressions, but not a general deficit in processing of facial expressions. The potential of such bias in serving as a predictor for depression should be further examined in future studies.
- Published
- 2019
38. Cross-Task Contributions of Frontobasal Ganglia Circuitry in Response Inhibition and Conflict-Induced Slowing.
- Author
-
Jahfari, Sara, Ridderinkhof, K Richard, Collins, Anne GE, Knapen, Tomas, Waldorp, Lourens J, and Frank, Michael J
- Subjects
Biological Psychology ,Psychology ,Basic Behavioral and Social Science ,Clinical Research ,Behavioral and Social Science ,Mental health ,Adult ,Basal Ganglia ,Brain Mapping ,Conflict ,Psychological ,Decision Making ,Female ,Frontal Lobe ,Humans ,Inhibition ,Psychological ,Magnetic Resonance Imaging ,Male ,Neural Pathways ,Psychomotor Performance ,Reaction Time ,Reinforcement ,Psychology ,Subthalamic Nucleus ,Uncertainty ,Young Adult ,basal ganglia systems ,Bayesian hierarchical modeling ,fMRI effective and functional connectivity ,reinforcement learning ,response inhibition ,Neurosciences ,Cognitive Sciences ,Experimental Psychology ,Biological psychology ,Cognitive and computational psychology - Abstract
Why are we so slow in choosing the lesser of 2 evils? We considered whether such slowing relates to uncertainty about the value of these options, which arises from the tendency to avoid them during learning, and whether such slowing relates to frontosubthalamic inhibitory control mechanisms. In total, 49 participants performed a reinforcement-learning task and a stop-signal task while fMRI was recorded. A reinforcement-learning model was used to quantify learning strategies. Individual differences in lose-lose slowing related to information uncertainty due to sampling, and independently, to less efficient response inhibition in the stop-signal task. Neuroimaging analysis revealed an analogous dissociation: subthalamic nucleus (STN) BOLD activity related to variability in stopping latencies, whereas weaker frontosubthalamic connectivity related to slowing and information sampling. Across tasks, fast inhibitors increased STN activity for successfully canceled responses in the stop task, but decreased activity for lose-lose choices. These data support the notion that fronto-STN communication implements a rapid but transient brake on response execution, and that slowing due to decision uncertainty could result from an inefficient release of this "hold your horses" mechanism.
- Published
- 2019
39. Causal Evidence for the Role of Neuronal Oscillations in Top–Down and Bottom–Up Attention
- Author
-
Riddle, Justin, Hwang, Kai, Cellier, Dillan, Dhanani, Sofia, and D'Esposito, Mark
- Subjects
Biological Psychology ,Psychology ,Eye Disease and Disorders of Vision ,Adolescent ,Adult ,Attention ,Beta Rhythm ,Brain Mapping ,Female ,Frontal Lobe ,Gamma Rhythm ,Humans ,Magnetic Resonance Imaging ,Male ,Parietal Lobe ,Reaction Time ,Saccades ,Spatial Processing ,Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation ,Visual Fields ,Visual Perception ,Young Adult ,Neurosciences ,Cognitive Sciences ,Experimental Psychology ,Biological psychology ,Cognitive and computational psychology - Abstract
Beta and gamma frequency neuronal oscillations have been implicated in top-down and bottom-up attention. In this study, we used rhythmic TMS to modulate ongoing beta and gamma frequency neuronal oscillations in frontal and parietal cortex while human participants performed a visual search task that manipulates bottom-up and top-down attention (single feature and conjunction search). Both task conditions will engage bottom-up attention processes, although the conjunction search condition will require more top-down attention. Gamma frequency TMS to superior precentral sulcus (sPCS) slowed saccadic RTs during both task conditions and induced a response bias to the contralateral visual field. In contrary, beta frequency TMS to sPCS and intraparietal sulcus decreased search accuracy only during the conjunction search condition that engaged more top-down attention. Furthermore, beta frequency TMS increased trial errors specifically when the target was in the ipsilateral visual field for the conjunction search condition. These results indicate that beta frequency TMS to sPCS and intraparietal sulcus disrupted top-down attention, whereas gamma frequency TMS to sPCS disrupted bottom-up, stimulus-driven attention processes. These findings provide causal evidence suggesting that beta and gamma oscillations have distinct functional roles for cognition.
- Published
- 2019
40. Variation in target and distractor heterogeneity impacts performance in the centroid task
- Author
-
Lu, Vivian T, Wright, Charles E, Chubb, Charles, and Sperling, George
- Subjects
Adult ,Attention ,Female ,Humans ,Male ,Pattern Recognition ,Visual ,Perceptual Masking ,Psychophysics ,Reaction Time ,Young Adult ,feature-based attention ,centroid task ,target/distractor heterogeneity ,Medical and Health Sciences ,Psychology and Cognitive Sciences ,Experimental Psychology - Abstract
In a selective centroid task, the participant views a brief cloud of items of different types-some of which are targets, the others distractors-and strives to mouse-click the centroid of the target items, ignoring the distractors. Advantages of the centroid task are that multiple target types can appear in the same display and that influence functions, which estimate the weight of each stimulus type in the cloud on the perceived centroid for each participant, can be obtained easily and efficiently. Here we document the strong, negative impact on performance that results when the participant is instructed to attend to target dots that consist of two or more levels of a single feature dimension, even when those levels differ categorically from those of the distractor dots. The results also show a smaller, but still observable decrement in performance that results when there is heterogeneity in the distractor dots.
- Published
- 2019
41. How the inference of hierarchical rules unfolds over time
- Author
-
Eckstein, Maria K, Starr, Ariel, and Bunge, Silvia A
- Subjects
Patient Safety ,Clinical Research ,Adolescent ,Adult ,Female ,Humans ,Individuality ,Learning ,Male ,Psychomotor Performance ,Pupil ,Reaction Time ,Thinking ,Time Factors ,Young Adult ,Pupil dilation ,Rule inference ,Inductive reasoning ,Individual differences ,Hierarchical representation ,Structure learning ,Information and Computing Sciences ,Psychology and Cognitive Sciences ,Language ,Communication and Culture ,Experimental Psychology - Abstract
Inductive reasoning, which entails reaching conclusions that are based on but go beyond available evidence, has long been of interest in cognitive science. Nevertheless, knowledge is still lacking as to the specific cognitive processes that underlie inductive reasoning. Here, we shed light on these processes in two ways. First, we characterized the timecourse of inductive reasoning in a rule induction task, using pupil dilation as a moment-by-moment measure of cognitive load. Participants' patterns of behavior and pupillary responses indicated that they engaged in rule inference on-line, and were surprised when additional evidence violated their inferred rules. Second, we sought to gain insight into how participants represented rules on this task - specifically, whether they would structure the rules hierarchically when possible. We predicted the cognitive load imposed by hierarchical representations, as well as by non-hierarchical, flat ones. We used task-evoked pupil dilation as a metric of cognitive load to infer, based on these predictions, which participants represented rules with flat or hierarchical structures. Participants categorized as representing the rules hierarchically or flat differed in task performance and self-reports of strategy. Hierarchical rule representation was associated with more efficient performance and more pronounced pupillary responses to rule violations on trials that afford a higher-order regularity, but with less efficient performance on trials that do not. Thus, differences in rule representation can be inferred from a physiological measure of cognitive load, and are associated with differences in performance. These results illustrate how pupillometry can provide a window into reasoning as it unfolds over time.
- Published
- 2019
42. Lateralized Suppression of Alpha-Band EEG Activity As a Mechanism of Target Processing
- Author
-
Bacigalupo, Felix and Luck, Steven J
- Subjects
Medical Physiology ,Biomedical and Clinical Sciences ,Clinical Research ,Neurosciences ,Eye Disease and Disorders of Vision ,Neurological ,Adolescent ,Adult ,Alpha Rhythm ,Attention ,Cues ,Electroencephalography ,Evoked Potentials ,Female ,Functional Laterality ,Humans ,Male ,Reaction Time ,Space Perception ,Visual Fields ,Young Adult ,alpha-band ,attention ,EEG ,lateralization ,N2pc ,Medical and Health Sciences ,Psychology and Cognitive Sciences ,Neurology & Neurosurgery - Abstract
Alpha-band (8-12 Hz) EEG activity has been linked to visual attention since the earliest EEG studies. More recent studies using spatial cuing paradigms have shown that alpha is suppressed over the hemisphere contralateral to a to-be-attended location, suggesting that alpha serves as a mechanism of preparatory attention. Here, we demonstrate that alpha also plays a role in active target processing. EEG activity was recorded from a group of healthy male and female human subjects in two visual search experiments. In addition to alpha activity, we also assessed the N2pc event-related potential component, a lateralized transient EEG response that has been tightly linked with the focusing of attention on visual targets. We found that the visual search targets triggered both an N2pc component and a suppression of alpha-band activity that was greatest over the hemisphere contralateral to the target (which we call "target-elicited lateralized alpha suppression" or TELAS). In Experiment 1, both N2pc and TELAS were observed for targets presented in the lower visual field but were absent for upper-field targets. However, these two lateralized effects had different time courses and they responded differently to manipulations of crowding in Experiment 2. These results indicate that lateralized alpha-band activity is involved in active target processing and is not solely a preparatory mechanism and also that TELAS and N2pc reflect a related but separable neural mechanism of visuospatial attention.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT The very first EEG studies demonstrated that alpha-band (8-12 Hz) EEG oscillations are suppressed when people attend to visual information and more recent research has shown that cuing an individual to expect a target at a specific location produces lateralized suppression in the contralateral hemisphere. Therefore, lateralized alpha may serve as a preparatory mechanism. In the present study, we found that a similar lateralized alpha effect is triggered by the appearance of a visual target even though the location could not be anticipated, demonstrating that alpha also serves as an active mechanism of target processing. Moreover, we found that alpha lateralization can be dissociated from other lateralized measures of target selection, indicating that it reflects a distinct mechanism of attention.
- Published
- 2019
43. Temporal Dynamics and Response Modulation across the Human Visual System in a Spatial Attention Task: An ECoG Study
- Author
-
Martin, Anne B, Yang, Xiaofang, Saalmann, Yuri B, Wang, Liang, Shestyuk, Avgusta, Lin, Jack J, Parvizi, Josef, Knight, Robert T, and Kastner, Sabine
- Subjects
Neurodegenerative ,Bioengineering ,Mental Health ,Eye Disease and Disorders of Vision ,Brain Disorders ,Neurosciences ,Epilepsy ,Clinical Research ,Neurological ,Adolescent ,Adult ,Attention ,Brain Mapping ,Cues ,Electrocorticography ,Electrodes ,Implanted ,Female ,Humans ,Male ,Middle Aged ,Photic Stimulation ,Psychomotor Performance ,Reaction Time ,Space Perception ,Vision ,Ocular ,Visual Cortex ,Visual Pathways ,Young Adult ,attention latencies ,onset latencies ,spatial response fields ,topographic atlas ,Medical and Health Sciences ,Psychology and Cognitive Sciences ,Neurology & Neurosurgery - Abstract
The selection of behaviorally relevant information from cluttered visual scenes (often referred to as "attention") is mediated by a cortical large-scale network consisting of areas in occipital, temporal, parietal, and frontal cortex that is organized into a functional hierarchy of feedforward and feedback pathways. In the human brain, little is known about the temporal dynamics of attentional processing from studies at the mesoscopic level of electrocorticography (ECoG), that combines millisecond temporal resolution with precise anatomical localization of recording sites. We analyzed high-frequency broadband responses (HFB) responses from 626 electrodes implanted in 8 epilepsy patients who performed a spatial attention task. Electrode locations were reconstructed using a probabilistic atlas of the human visual system. HFB responses showed high spatial selectivity and tuning, constituting ECoG response fields (RFs), within and outside the topographic visual system. In accordance with monkey physiology studies, both RF widths and onset latencies increased systematically across the visual processing hierarchy. We used the spatial specificity of HFB responses to quantitatively study spatial attention effects and their temporal dynamics to probe a hierarchical top-down model suggesting that feedback signals back propagate the visual processing hierarchy. Consistent with such a model, the strengths of attentional modulation were found to be greater and modulation latencies to be shorter in posterior parietal cortex, middle temporal cortex and ventral extrastriate cortex compared with early visual cortex. However, inconsistent with such a model, attention effects were weaker and more delayed in anterior parietal and frontal cortex.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT In the human brain, visual attention has been predominantly studied using methods with high spatial, but poor temporal resolution such as fMRI, or high temporal, but poor spatial resolution such as EEG/MEG. Here, we investigate temporal dynamics and attention effects across the human visual system at a mesoscopic level that combines precise spatial and temporal measurements by using electrocorticography in epilepsy patients performing a classical spatial attention task. Electrode locations were reconstructed using a probabilistic atlas of the human visual system, thereby relating them to topography and processing hierarchy. We demonstrate regional differences in temporal dynamics across the attention network. Our findings do not fully support a top-down model that promotes influences on visual cortex by reversing the processing hierarchy.
- Published
- 2019
44. High-capacity preconscious processing in concurrent groupings of colored dots.
- Author
-
Sun, Peng, Sperling, George, Chubb, Charles, and Wright, Charles
- Subjects
centroid judgments ,grouping by similarity ,perceptual grouping ,preconscious processing ,summary statistics ,Adolescent ,Adult ,Brain ,Color ,Color Perception ,Distance Perception ,Female ,Humans ,Judgment ,Male ,Reaction Time ,Young Adult - Abstract
Grouping is a perceptual process in which a subset of stimulus components (a group) is selected for a subsequent-typically implicit-perceptual computation. Grouping is a critical precursor to segmenting objects from the background and ultimately to object recognition. Here, we study grouping by color. We present subjects with 300-ms exposures of 12 dots colored with the same but unknown identical color interspersed among 14 dots of seven different colors. To indicate grouping, subjects point-click the remembered centroid (center of gravity) of the set of homogeneous dots, of heterogeneous dots, or of all dots. Subjects accurately judge all of these centroids. Furthermore, after a single stimulus exposure, subjects can judge both the heterogeneous and homogeneous centroids, that is, subjects simultaneously group by similarity and by dissimilarity. The centroid paradigm reveals the relative weight of each dot among targets and distractors to the underlying grouping process, offering a more detailed, quantitative description of grouping than was previously possible. A change detection experiment reveals that conscious memory contains less than two dots and their locations, whereas an ideal detector would have to perfectly process at least 15 of 26 dots to match the subjects centroid judgments-indicating an extraordinary capacity for preconscious grouping. A different color set yielded identical results. Grouping theories that rely on predefined feature maps would fail to explain these results. Rather, the results indicate that preconscious grouping is automatic, flexible, and rapid, and a far more complex process than previously believed.
- Published
- 2018
45. Having More Choices Changes How Human Observers Weight Stable Sensory Evidence
- Author
-
Itthipuripat, Sirawaj, Cha, Kexin, Deering, Sean, Salazar, Annalisa M, and Serences, John T
- Subjects
Neurosciences ,Basic Behavioral and Social Science ,Behavioral and Social Science ,1.1 Normal biological development and functioning ,Underpinning research ,Adult ,Attention ,Brain ,Decision Making ,Electroencephalography ,Evoked Potentials ,Visual ,Female ,Humans ,Male ,Models ,Neurological ,Models ,Psychological ,Photic Stimulation ,Reaction Time ,Young Adult ,decision threshold ,divided attention ,event-related potential ,evidence accumulation ,multiple-choice decision-making ,steady-state visually evoked potential ,Medical and Health Sciences ,Psychology and Cognitive Sciences ,Neurology & Neurosurgery - Abstract
Decision-making becomes slower when more choices are available. Existing models attribute this slowing to poor sensory processing, to attenuated rates of sensory evidence accumulation, or to increases in the amount of evidence required before committing to a decision (a higher decision threshold). However, studies have not isolated the effects of having more choices on sensory and decision-related processes from changes in task difficulty and divided attention. Here, we controlled task difficulty while independently manipulating the distribution of attention and the number of choices available to male and female human observers. We used EEG to measure steady-state visually evoked potentials (SSVEPs) and a frontal late positive deflection (LPD), EEG markers of sensory and postsensory decision-related processes, respectively. We found that dividing attention decreased SSVEP and LPD amplitudes, consistent with dampened sensory responses and slower rates of evidence accumulation, respectively. In contrast, having more choices did not alter SSVEP amplitude and led to a larger LPD. These results suggest that having more options largely spares early sensory processing and slows down decision-making via a selective increase in decision thresholds.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENT When more choices are available, decision-making becomes slower. We tested whether this phenomenon is due to poor sensory processing, to reduced rates of evidence accumulation, or to increases in the amount of evidence required before committing to a decision (a higher decision threshold). We measured choice modulations of sensory and decision-related neural responses using EEG. We also minimized potential confounds from changes in the distribution of attention and task difficulty, which often covary with having more choices. Dividing attention reduced the activity levels of both sensory and decision-related responses. However, having more choices did not change sensory processing and led to larger decision-related responses. These results suggest that having more choices spares sensory processing and selectively increases decision thresholds.
- Published
- 2018
46. Nicotine effects on associative learning in human non-smokers
- Author
-
Hahn, Britta, Wells, Ashleigh K, Lenartowicz, Agatha, and Yuille, Marie B
- Subjects
Biological Psychology ,Pharmacology and Pharmaceutical Sciences ,Biomedical and Clinical Sciences ,Psychology ,Drug Abuse (NIDA only) ,Tobacco Smoke and Health ,Brain Disorders ,Behavioral and Social Science ,Prevention ,Substance Misuse ,Tobacco ,Mental Health ,Clinical Research ,Basic Behavioral and Social Science ,6.1 Pharmaceuticals ,Evaluation of treatments and therapeutic interventions ,Mental health ,Good Health and Well Being ,Adult ,Association Learning ,Cross-Over Studies ,Double-Blind Method ,Female ,Humans ,Male ,Middle Aged ,Nicotine ,Nicotinic Agonists ,Non-Smokers ,Photic Stimulation ,Reaction Time ,Tobacco Use Cessation Devices ,Young Adult ,Medical and Health Sciences ,Psychology and Cognitive Sciences ,Psychiatry ,Neurosciences ,Biological psychology - Abstract
Tobacco smoking is the most common preventable cause of death in the US. Nicotine is considered the primary constituent responsible for tobacco addiction. Its paradoxically high abuse potential may reflect behavioral control by drug-associated stimuli, which appears to play a larger role for tobacco dependence than for other abused drugs. We tested a potential explanation, hypothesizing that nicotine enhances associative learning, the mechanism underlying the conditioning of drug-associated stimuli. Thirty-two non-smokers were exposed to transdermal nicotine (7 mg/24 h) and placebo in a double-blind cross-over study and tested with behavioral paradigms designed to isolate incidental stimulus-stimulus or stimulus-response learning. The stop signal task required speeded gender judgments of face stimuli. A tone signaled when to withhold the response. Unbeknownst to participants, some faces were always paired with stop trials. Nicotine enhanced the facilitation of stop-responses to these stimuli, and the slowing of go-responses when previously stop-associated stimuli were paired with go trials, indicating stronger associations between paired stimuli and the stop signal/stop response. Another task required feedback-based learning of associations between pairs of shape stimuli. Five pairs were made from either ten different stimuli, or from different combinations of two identical sets of five stimuli with correct associations depending on contextual information. Nicotine increased incorrect choices of stimuli that were associated in a different context, indicating stronger stimulus-stimulus associations at the expense of flexible context-adaptive behavior. The results indicate that nicotine can enhance incidental associative learning, a mechanism that may promote the formation of smoking-associated stimuli and cue-controlled drug-taking.
- Published
- 2018
47. Deficits in physiological and self-conscious emotional response to errors in hoarding disorder
- Author
-
Zakrzewski, Jessica J, Datta, Samir, Scherling, Carole, Nizar, Krystal, Vigil, Ofilio, Rosen, Howard, and Mathews, Carol A
- Subjects
Biological Psychology ,Social and Personality Psychology ,Psychology ,Applied and Developmental Psychology ,Behavioral and Social Science ,Clinical Research ,Brain Disorders ,Mind and Body ,Adult ,Aged ,Conditioning ,Classical ,Emotions ,Facial Expression ,Female ,Hoarding Disorder ,Humans ,Male ,Middle Aged ,Photic Stimulation ,Psychomotor Performance ,Reaction Time ,Self Concept ,Hoarding disorder ,Error response ,Facial emotion ,Stop-Change Task ,Medical and Health Sciences ,Psychology and Cognitive Sciences ,Psychiatry ,Clinical sciences ,Clinical and health psychology - Abstract
Hoarding disorder (HD) has been hypothesized to arise from deficits in error monitoring and abnormalities in emotional processing, but the relationship between error monitoring and emotional processing has not been examined. We examined measures of self-report, as well as behavioral, physiological, and facial responses to errors during a Stop-Change Task. 25 participants with HD and 32 healthy controls (HC) were recruited. Participants reported on number of errors committed and pre/post emotional response to errors. Skin conductance response (SCR) during correct and error commission trials was examined. Facial expression during task performance was coded for self-conscious and negative emotions. HD and HC participants had significantly different error rates but comparable error correction and post-error slowing. SCR was significantly lower for HD during error commission than for HC. During error trials, HD participants showed a significant deficit in displays of self-conscious emotions compared to HC. Self-reported emotions were increased in HD, with more negative and self-conscious emotion reported than was reported for HC participants. These findings suggest that hypoactive emotional responding at a physiological level may play a role in how errors are processed in individuals with HD.
- Published
- 2018
48. Controlling pre-movement sensorimotor rhythm can improve finger extension after stroke
- Author
-
Norman, SL, McFarland, DJ, Miner, A, Cramer, SC, Wolbrecht, ET, Wolpaw, JR, and Reinkensmeyer, DJ
- Subjects
Engineering ,Biomedical and Clinical Sciences ,Neurosciences ,Biomedical Engineering ,Stroke ,Rehabilitation ,Brain Disorders ,Clinical Research ,Bioengineering ,Assistive Technology ,Adult ,Aged ,Aged ,80 and over ,Algorithms ,Brain-Computer Interfaces ,Cues ,Electroencephalography ,Exoskeleton Device ,Female ,Fingers ,Humans ,Male ,Middle Aged ,Movement ,Reaction Time ,Recovery of Function ,Robotics ,Stroke Rehabilitation ,BCI ,robot ,stroke ,rehabilitation ,sensorimotor rhythm ,motor control ,Clinical Sciences ,Biomedical engineering - Abstract
ObjectiveBrain-computer interface (BCI) technology is attracting increasing interest as a tool for enhancing recovery of motor function after stroke, yet the optimal way to apply this technology is unknown. Here, we studied the immediate and therapeutic effects of BCI-based training to control pre-movement sensorimotor rhythm (SMR) amplitude on robot-assisted finger extension in people with stroke.ApproachEight people with moderate to severe hand impairment due to chronic stroke completed a four-week three-phase protocol during which they practiced finger extension with assistance from the FINGER robotic exoskeleton. In Phase 1, we identified spatiospectral SMR features for each person that correlated with the intent to extend the index and/or middle finger(s). In Phase 2, the participants learned to increase or decrease SMR features given visual feedback, without movement. In Phase 3, the participants were cued to increase or decrease their SMR features, and when successful, were then cued to immediately attempt to extend the finger(s) with robot assistance.Main resultsOf the four participants that achieved SMR control in Phase 2, three initiated finger extensions with a reduced reaction time after decreasing (versus increasing) pre-movement SMR amplitude during Phase 3. Two also extended at least one of their fingers more forcefully after decreasing pre-movement SMR amplitude. Hand function, measured by the box and block test (BBT), improved by 7.3 ± 7.5 blocks versus 3.5 ± 3.1 blocks in those with and without SMR control, respectively. Higher BBT scores at baseline correlated with a larger change in BBT score.SignificanceThese results suggest that learning to control person-specific pre-movement SMR features associated with finger extension can improve finger extension ability after stroke for some individuals. These results merit further investigation in a rehabilitation context.
- Published
- 2018
49. Change in Cognitive Performance From Midlife Into Old Age: Findings from the Midlife in the United States (MIDUS) Study.
- Author
-
Hughes, Matthew, Agrigoroaei, Stefan, Bruzzese, Molly, Lachman, Margie, and Jeon, Minjeong
- Subjects
Cognitive aging ,Cognitive function ,Educational status ,Longitudinal studies ,Middle aged ,Sex differences ,Adult ,Aged ,Aged ,80 and over ,Attention ,Cognition ,Cognitive Aging ,Educational Status ,Executive Function ,Female ,Humans ,Longitudinal Studies ,Male ,Memory ,Memory ,Episodic ,Mental Recall ,Middle Aged ,Neuropsychological Tests ,Reaction Time ,Sex Characteristics ,Telephone ,United States - Abstract
OBJECTIVES: A substantial body of research has documented age-related declines in cognitive abilities among adults over 60, yet there is much less known about changes in cognitive abilities during midlife. The goal was to examine longitudinal changes in multiple cognitive domains from early midlife through old age in a large national sample, the Midlife in the United States (MIDUS) study. METHODS: The Brief Test of Adult Cognition by Telephone (BTACT) was administered on two occasions (MIDUS 2, MIDUS 3), an average of 9 years apart. At MIDUS 3, those with the cognitive assessment (N=2518) ranged in age from 42 to 92 years (M=64.30; SD=11.20) and had a mean education of 14.68 years (SD=2.63). The BTACT includes assessment of key aging-sensitive cognitive domains: immediate and delayed free recall, number series, category fluency, backward digit span, processing speed, and reaction time for attention switching and inhibitory control, which comprise two factors: episodic memory and executive functioning. RESULTS: As predicted, all cognitive subtests and factors showed very small but significant declines over 9 years, with differences in the timing and extent of change. Processing speed showed the earliest and steepest decrements. Those with higher educational attainment scored better on all tests except reaction time. Men had better executive functioning and women performed better on episodic memory. CONCLUSIONS: Examining cognitive changes in midlife provides opportunities for early detection of cognitive impairments and possibilities for preventative interventions. (JINS, 2018, 24, 805-820).
- Published
- 2018
50. Neural Mechanisms of Sustained Attention Are Rhythmic
- Author
-
Helfrich, Randolph F, Fiebelkorn, Ian C, Szczepanski, Sara M, Lin, Jack J, Parvizi, Josef, Knight, Robert T, and Kastner, Sabine
- Subjects
Brain Disorders ,Basic Behavioral and Social Science ,Behavioral and Social Science ,Neurosciences ,Mental Health ,Neurological ,Adult ,Attention ,Brain ,Electrodes ,Implanted ,Electroencephalography ,Female ,Humans ,Male ,Middle Aged ,Periodicity ,Photic Stimulation ,Psychomotor Performance ,Random Allocation ,Reaction Time ,Visual Perception ,discrete perception ,electrocorticography ,frontoparietal attention network ,functional network parcellation ,high-frequency activity ,intracranial EEG ,perceptual cycles ,phase-dependent behavior ,rhythmic attention ,theta oscillations ,Psychology ,Cognitive Sciences ,Neurology & Neurosurgery - Abstract
Classic models of attention suggest that sustained neural firing constitutes a neural correlate of sustained attention. However, recent evidence indicates that behavioral performance fluctuates over time, exhibiting temporal dynamics that closely resemble the spectral features of ongoing, oscillatory brain activity. Therefore, it has been proposed that periodic neuronal excitability fluctuations might shape attentional allocation and overt behavior. However, empirical evidence to support this notion is sparse. Here, we address this issue by examining data from large-scale subdural recordings, using two different attention tasks that track perceptual ability at high temporal resolution. Our results reveal that perceptual outcome varies as a function of the theta phase even in states of sustained spatial attention. These effects were robust at the single-subject level, suggesting that rhythmic perceptual sampling is an inherent property of the frontoparietal attention network. Collectively, these findings support the notion that the functional architecture of top-down attention is intrinsically rhythmic.
- Published
- 2018
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.