55 results on '"Joy A. Mench"'
Search Results
2. The Days and Nights of Zoo Elephants: Using Epidemiology to Better Understand Stereotypic Behavior of African Elephants (Loxodonta africana) and Asian Elephants (Elephas maximus) in North American Zoos.
- Author
-
Brian J Greco, Cheryl L Meehan, Jen N Hogan, Katherine A Leighty, Jill Mellen, Georgia J Mason, and Joy A Mench
- Subjects
Medicine ,Science - Abstract
Stereotypic behavior is an important indicator of compromised welfare. Zoo elephants are documented to perform stereotypic behavior, but the factors that contribute to performance have not been systematically assessed. We collected behavioral data on 89 elephants (47 African [Loxodonta africana], 42 Asian [Elephas maximus]) at 39 North American zoos during the summer and winter. Elephants were videoed for a median of 12 daytime hours per season. A subset of 32 elephants (19 African, 13 Asian) was also observed live for a median of 10.5 nighttime hours. Percentages of visible behavior scans were calculated from five minute instantaneous samples. Stereotypic behavior was the second most commonly performed behavior (after feeding), making up 15.5% of observations during the daytime and 24.8% at nighttime. Negative binomial regression models fitted with generalized estimating equations were used to determine which social, housing, management, life history, and demographic variables were associated with daytime and nighttime stereotypic behavior rates. Species was a significant risk factor in both models, with Asian elephants at greater risk (daytime: p
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
3. Housing and Social Environments of African (Loxodonta africana) and Asian (Elephas maximus) Elephants in North American Zoos.
- Author
-
Cheryl L Meehan, Jennifer N Hogan, Mary K Bonaparte-Saller, and Joy A Mench
- Subjects
Medicine ,Science - Abstract
We evaluated 255 African (Loxodonta africana) and Asian (Elephas maximus) elephants living in 68 North American zoos over one year to quantify housing and social variables. All parameters were quantified for the both the day and the night and comparisons were made across these time periods as well as by species and sex. To assess housing, we evaluated not only total exhibit size, but also individual animals' experiences based on the time they spent in the unique environments into which the exhibits were subdivided. Variables developed to assess housing included measurements of area as a function of time (Total Space Experience), environment type (Indoor, Outdoor, In/Out Choice) and time spent on hard and soft flooring. Over the year, Total Space Experience values ranged from 1,273 square feet to 169,692 square feet, with Day values significantly greater than Night values (p
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
4. Elephant Management in North American Zoos: Environmental Enrichment, Feeding, Exercise, and Training.
- Author
-
Brian J Greco, Cheryl L Meehan, Lance J Miller, David J Shepherdson, Kari A Morfeld, Jeff Andrews, Anne M Baker, Kathy Carlstead, and Joy A Mench
- Subjects
Medicine ,Science - Abstract
The management of African (Loxodonta africana) and Asian (Elephas maximus) elephants in zoos involves a range of practices including feeding, exercise, training, and environmental enrichment. These practices are necessary to meet the elephants' nutritional, healthcare, and husbandry needs. However, these practices are not standardized, resulting in likely variation among zoos as well as differences in the way they are applied to individual elephants within a zoo. To characterize elephant management in North America, we collected survey data from zoos accredited by the Association of Zoos and Aquariums, developed 26 variables, generated population level descriptive statistics, and analyzed them to identify differences attributable to sex and species. Sixty-seven zoos submitted surveys describing the management of 224 elephants and the training experiences of 227 elephants. Asian elephants spent more time managed (defined as interacting directly with staff) than Africans (mean time managed: Asians = 56.9%; Africans = 48.6%; p
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
5. Determining Connections between the Daily Lives of Zoo Elephants and Their Welfare: An Epidemiological Approach.
- Author
-
Cheryl L Meehan, Joy A Mench, Kathy Carlstead, and Jennifer N Hogan
- Subjects
Medicine ,Science - Abstract
Concerns about animal welfare increasingly shape people's views about the acceptability of keeping animals for food production, biomedical research, and in zoos. The field of animal welfare science has developed over the past 50 years as a method of investigating these concerns via research that assesses how living in human-controlled environments influences the behavior, health and affective states of animals. Initially, animal welfare research focused on animals in agricultural settings, but the field has expanded to zoos because good animal welfare is essential to zoos' mission of promoting connections between animals and visitors and raising awareness of conservation issues. A particular challenge for zoos is ensuring good animal welfare for long-lived, highly social species like elephants. Our main goal in conducting an epidemiological study of African (Loxodonta africana) and Asian (Elephas maximus) elephant welfare in 68 accredited North American zoos was to understand the prevalence of welfare indicators in the population and determine the aspects of an elephant's zoo environment, social life and management that are most important to prevent and reduce a variety of welfare problems. In this overview, we provide a summary of the findings of the nine papers in the collection titled: Epidemiological Investigations of North American Zoo Elephant Welfare with a focus on the life history, social, housing, and management factors found to be associated with particular aspects of elephant welfare, including the performance of abnormal behavior, foot and joint problems, recumbence, walking rates, and reproductive health issues. Social and management factors were found to be important for multiple indicators of welfare, while exhibit space was found to be less influential than expected. This body of work results from the largest prospective zoo-based animal welfare study conducted to date and sets in motion the process of using science-based welfare benchmarks to optimize care of zoo elephants.
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
6. The Relationship between Personality Dimensions and Resiliency to Environmental Stress in Orange-Winged Amazon Parrots (Amazona amazonica), as Indicated by the Development of Abnormal Behaviors.
- Author
-
Victoria A Cussen and Joy A Mench
- Subjects
Medicine ,Science - Abstract
Parrots are popular companion animals, but are frequently relinquished because of behavioral problems, including abnormal repetitive behaviors like feather damaging behavior and stereotypy. In addition to contributing to pet relinquishment, these behaviors are important as potential indicators of diminished psychological well-being. While abnormal behaviors are common in captive animals, their presence and/or severity varies between animals of the same species that are experiencing the same environmental conditions. Personality differences could contribute to this observed individual variation, as they are known risk factors for stress sensitivity and affective disorders in humans. The goal of this study was to assess the relationship between personality and the development and severity of abnormal behaviors in captive-bred orange-winged Amazon parrots (Amazona amazonica). We monitored between-individual behavioral differences in enrichment-reared parrots of known personality types before, during, and after enrichment deprivation. We predicted that parrots with higher scores for neurotic-like personality traits would be more susceptible to enrichment deprivation and develop more abnormal behaviors. Our results partially supported this hypothesis, but also showed that distinct personality dimensions were related to different forms of abnormal behavior. While neuroticism-like traits were linked to feather damaging behavior, extraversion-like traits were negatively related to stereotypic behavior. More extraverted birds showed resiliency to environmental stress, developing fewer stereotypies during enrichment deprivation and showing lower levels of these behaviors following re-enrichment. Our data, together with the results of the few studies conducted on other species, suggest that, as in humans, certain personality types render individual animals more susceptible or resilient to environmental stress. Further, this susceptibility/resiliency can have a long-term effect on behavior, as evidenced by behavioral changes that persisted despite re-enrichment. Ours is the first study evaluating the relationship between personality dimensions, environment, and abnormal behaviors in an avian species.
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
7. Northern fowl mite infestation affects the nocturnal behavior of laying hens
- Author
-
Leonie Jacobs, G. Vezzoli, Joy A. Mench, Bonne Beerda, Jacobs, L., Vezzoli, G., Beerda, B., and Mench, J. A.
- Subjects
Veterinary medicine ,Ectoparasite ,Nighttime behavior ,Nocturnal ,medicine.disease_cause ,Behavioral Ecology ,Food Animals ,Irritation ,Infestation ,parasitic diseases ,medicine ,Mite ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,050102 behavioral science & comparative psychology ,Laying hen ,Northern fowl mite ,biology ,integumentary system ,Inoculation ,05 social sciences ,0402 animal and dairy science ,04 agricultural and veterinary sciences ,Ornithonyssus sylviarum ,biology.organism_classification ,040201 dairy & animal science ,respiratory tract diseases ,Gedragsecologie ,WIAS ,Animal Science and Zoology ,PEST analysis ,Macronyssidae ,Sleep - Abstract
The northern fowl mite (Ornithonyssus sylviarum) is a key pest for caged laying hens. High infestation levels can lead to anemia, reduced production, and mortality. Although the mites cause skin irritation and thus likely affect resting behavior, the nocturnal behavior of infested chickens has not yet been studied. We investigated the impact of mite infestation on nighttime behavior of 16 beak-trimmed White Leghorn hens. The hens were housed individually in wire cages. They were experimentally inoculated with approximately 35 mites at 25 weeks of age and observed for the behaviors dozing, sleeping, preening and being active. Continuous observations were made from video recordings taken from 22:00 h until 06:00 h for two consecutive nights at pre-infestation week 0 and post-infestation weeks 3, 5 and 7. Mite infestation levels were measured weekly on an 8-point scale (0 (no mites) to 7 (≥10,000 mites per hen)). For statistical analyses with linear mixed models, mite infestation levels were categorized as score 0 (no mites), 3–4 (51–500), 5 (501–1000) and 6–7 (>1000). Higher levels of mite infestation were associated with hens spending less time dozing (P < 0.001) and more time preening (P < 0.001) and being active (P = 0.012). Dozing decreased from 90% of the observed time for mite score 0 to 76% for score 6–7. Preening increased from 2% (score 0) to 9% (6–7) and time spent being active increased from 1% (0) to 7% (6–7). Mite infestation increased the number of uninterrupted bouts of all behaviors (P ≤ 0.020), especially dozing and preening (P < 0.001), suggesting restlessness in the mite-infested hens due to irritation and itching. The mite-infested hens’ increased nocturnal activity, including preening, as well as the fragmentation of behavioral activities together with decreased dozing, indicate disturbed resting behavior and suggest a reduction in the welfare of mite-infested hens.
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
8. Assessing the dyadic social relationships of female african (Loxodonta africana) and asian (Elephas maximus) zoo elephants using proximity, tactile contact, and keeper surveys
- Author
-
Mary K. Bonaparte-Saller and Joy A. Mench
- Subjects
0106 biological sciences ,biology ,05 social sciences ,Social bond ,biology.organism_classification ,010603 evolutionary biology ,01 natural sciences ,Elephas ,Food Animals ,Social relationship ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Animal Science and Zoology ,050102 behavioral science & comparative psychology ,Psychology ,Social psychology ,Dyad - Abstract
Understanding the affiliative social relationships, or bonds, between zoo elephants has implications for both their welfare and management, yet there is limited work assessing and describing these bonds. Consequently, there is a need for the development of a reliable assessment tool. We used multiple metrics of proximity and tactile contact, as well as keeper surveys, to assess the social bond strength of 41 elephant dyads from 22 different zoos. Survey descriptions of social bond strength were based on previous research and included proximity and separation-reunion behaviors between individuals in a dyad. Approximately half of the elephant dyads in our study were rated as having a “strong” or “strongest” bond by keepers, who showed excellent agreement in their ratings of elephant bond strength (ICC(1,k) = 0.82). Elephant dyads that spent more time in proximity (within two elephant body lengths), and those that were more consistent in this behavior across time had an increased predicted probability of being rated as having a “strong or strongest bond” by keepers (p
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
9. A Method for Guarding Animal Welfare and Public Health: Tracking the Rise of Backyard Poultry Ordinances
- Author
-
Joy A. Mench, Catherine Brinkley, and Jacqueline Scarlett Kingsley
- Subjects
medicine.medical_specialty ,Colorado ,Health (social science) ,040301 veterinary sciences ,media_common.quotation_subject ,0211 other engineering and technologies ,02 engineering and technology ,Animal Welfare ,Animal slaughter ,Disease cluster ,Poultry ,0403 veterinary science ,Urban planning ,Animal welfare ,medicine ,Animals ,Humans ,Environmental planning ,media_common ,Public health ,Public Health, Environmental and Occupational Health ,021107 urban & regional planning ,04 agricultural and veterinary sciences ,One Health ,Public Health ,Tracking (education) ,Business ,Welfare - Abstract
In response to a growing number of permissive urban animal agriculture ordinances across the nation, this research reviews the spatial and temporal attributes of urban poultry ordinances across 100 municipalities in Colorado. More poultry ordinances have been passed or modified in Colorado in the last 5 years than in the previous hundred. Comparing regulations to commercial operations and animal shelter surrenders, we find that permissive ordinances cluster near major urban areas even where they are in close proximity to large commercial operations. Most ordinances inadequately address both human and animal health and welfare concerns. Provisions governing animal slaughter and routine veterinary care are rare, presenting a concern for monitoring and intervening in public health crises. In addition, shelters anticipate higher poultry intakes, particularly as unwanted birds are turned loose to become strays.
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
10. Why pace? The influence of social, housing, management, life history, and demographic characteristics on locomotor stereotypy in zoo elephants
- Author
-
Jennifer L. Heinsius, Brian J. Greco, Cheryl L. Meehan, and Joy A. Mench
- Subjects
Binomial regression ,05 social sciences ,0402 animal and dairy science ,Social environment ,04 agricultural and veterinary sciences ,040201 dairy & animal science ,Odds ,Developmental psychology ,Social group ,Stereotypy (non-human) ,Food Animals ,Animal welfare ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Animal Science and Zoology ,050102 behavioral science & comparative psychology ,Psychology ,Generalized estimating equation ,Social behavior - Abstract
Stereotypic behaviors (SB) are common in zoo-housed elephants, and these behaviors can be performed at high rates. Elephants perform different SB forms ( e.g., weaving, pacing), but no published studies have evaluated the factors contributing to the development or performance of these different forms. Instead, as with most SB studies across species, elephant studies have relied on analyses that aggregate all SB forms, which limits the development and testing of form-specific hypotheses or abatement practices. Our objectives were to characterize the SB forms of North American zoo elephants and use multivariable epidemiological models to test form-specific hypotheses. We videotaped 77 elephants (African: N = 5 males, 31 females; Asian N = 8 males, 33 females) at 39 zoos who performed SBs and used a novel classification scheme and 5-min instantaneous samples to characterize their SB forms. Locomotor and whole-body SBs were the most common, but most elephants who performed locomotor SBs also performed whole-body SBs. Thus, we characterized each elephant according to whether it included locomotion in its SB repertoire [Locomotor Presence (LP)] or only whole-body movements. We used binomial regression models fitted with generalized estimating equations to test hypotheses about which of 26 social, housing, management, life history, and demographic variables were most associated with LP. The odds of LP increased by 26% for every 10% increase in time housed separately (odds ratio = 1.026, p = 0.04), 96.2% for every additional social group with which an elephant had contact (odds ratio = 1.962, p = 0.01), and 46% for every 10% increase in time housed indoors (odds ratio = 1.046, p = 0.01). Age was non-significantly confounded with all three variables. We hypothesize that the social variables in our models increase LP risk because they are associated with uncontrollable social group changes, anticipation of potentially rewarding social experiences, or the frustration of social behaviors. The housing variable included in our model likely increases LP risk because indoor spaces are less complex, resulting in the channeling of walking or social avoidance behaviors into more simplistic movements. Overall, our results suggest that elephant managers may best be able to prevent locomotor SB by enhancing their elephants’ social environment and the spatial complexity of their enclosures. Future research should focus on determining whether addressing the risk factors for LP results in less frequent performance and identifying other temporally proximate eliciting factors.
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
11. Exposing avian embryos to light affects post-hatch anti-predator fear responses
- Author
-
Gregory S. Archer and Joy A. Mench
- Subjects
0301 basic medicine ,Behavioral phenotypes ,Wing ,0402 animal and dairy science ,Broiler ,LIGHT STIMULATION ,Embryo ,04 agricultural and veterinary sciences ,Anatomy ,040201 dairy & animal science ,Tonic (physiology) ,03 medical and health sciences ,030104 developmental biology ,Animal science ,Food Animals ,Animal Science and Zoology ,Psychology ,Incubation ,Predator - Abstract
Environmental stimuli present during incubation can impact the behavior of birds post-hatch. To determine the effect of exposing broiler chicken embryos to light on fear-related behavior post-hatch, we conducted two experiments in which we incubated eggs under various light schedules, and then measured fear responses when the chickens (N = 720) were 3–6 wk of age. In Expt. 1, the incubation photoperiods were 0L:24D, 12L:12D, and 24L:0D, and tonic immobility (TI) and inversion (INV) tests were administered. In Expt. 2, the incubation photoperiods were 0L:24D, 1L:23D, 6L:18D, and 12L:12D; and an approach test (APPR) and an emergence (EMRG) test were administered in addition to TI and INV. In Expt. 1, both 12L:12D and 24L:0D had shorter latencies to right during TI (213.5 ± 23.7 and 231.8 ± 24.2 s, respectively) than 0L:24D (305.5 ± 26.1 s) and also wing flapped less intensely during INV (12L:12D 5.0 ± 0.1 wing flaps; 24L:0D 5.4 ± 0.2) than 0L:24D (5.7 ± 0.1). In Expt. 2, the 12L:12D birds once again had shorter latencies to right during TI (120.0 ± 16.5 s) and wing flapped less intensely during INV (4.7 ± 0.1 wing flaps) than 0L:24D (201.4 ± 24.9 and 5.5 ± 0.1, respectively). They also had shorter latencies to exit the dark box in EMRG (28.9 ± 3.3 s), and were less active (28 ± 2%), vocalized less (178.8 ± 9.3 times/3 min) and spent more time closer to the observer during APPR (63 ± 3%) than 0L:12D (42.9 ± 5.0 s, 35 ± 3%, 211.2 ± 10.4 times/3 min, 51 ± 3). The 1L:23D and 6L:18D showed some reductions in fearfulness compared to 0L:24D, but these were not consistent across tests. The 6L:18D and 12L:12D birds demonstrated lateralization in the direction to leave the box in EMRG, whereas 1L:23D and 0L:12D exited left or right at chance levels. The results of these experiments indicate that providing at 12 h of light stimulation daily during embryogenesis results in long-term reductions in fearfulness as measured by multiple tests, and that this may be related to cerebral lateralization. In conjunction with other research, these findings show that light exposure during embryogenesis has important implications for behavioral phenotypes and welfare in chickens.
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
12. Effect of outdoor structural enrichments on the performance, use of range area, and behavior of organic meat chickens
- Author
-
Yi Liang, Casey M. Owens, A. C. Fanatico, Annie M. Donoghue, Gregory S. Archer, V. B. Brewer Gunsaulis, and Joy A. Mench
- Subjects
Male ,Organic Agriculture ,Veterinary medicine ,Range (biology) ,Foraging ,0402 animal and dairy science ,04 agricultural and veterinary sciences ,General Medicine ,Motor Activity ,Biology ,Outdoor area ,Housing, Animal ,040201 dairy & animal science ,Random Allocation ,040103 agronomy & agriculture ,Animals ,0401 agriculture, forestry, and fisheries ,Female ,Animal Science and Zoology ,Animal Husbandry ,Chickens - Abstract
Chickens provided with outdoor access often do not fully use the range area. Natural cover, such as trees, can provide shelter and increase range use, but may not be practical for use in all free-range operations. A study was conducted to determine whether constructed enrichments that simulate natural structures increase range use and impact behavior of meat chickens. Slow-growing Delaware chickens were raised in floor pens (17 birds/pen) in a naturally ventilated house, with a pophole in each pen that allowed daily access to a vegetation-covered outdoor area. Feed and water were provided inside and outside. Birds were randomly assigned to one of 2 treatments: no range enrichment (control; NON) or range enrichment (ENR), which had roosts made of plastic pipe or screened shelters placed 7.6 m and 22.9 m from the house and overhead shade panels at 15.2 m. There were 4 replications. The numbers of birds inside and in the 4 quadrants of the range were counted in 7-min intervals 3 times daily (0900 h, 1300 h, and 1600 h) for 3 consecutive d when the birds were 7 and 10 wk of age; the numbers of birds performing various behaviors also were counted. On average, only 12.9% of birds used the range at any given time. The behavior of the birds was strongly affected by location (inside vs. outside), with birds mainly foraging and walking while outside and feeding, standing, and sitting while inside. Time of d impacted the number of birds that ranged (P ≤ 0.0001), with fewest birds outside during mid-day. Overall, birds using the range were most often (71%) observed in the quadrant nearest the house (zero to 7.6 m). However, in the ENR treatment, more birds (13%; P ≤ 0.0001) were observed in the farthest 2 quadrants (15.2 to 30.5 m from the house) than in the NON treatment (6%). This indicates that adding constructed enrichments to the range encouraged meat chickens to use it more fully.
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
13. Relationships between beak condition, preening behavior and ectoparasite infestation levels in laying hens
- Author
-
G. Vezzoli, Bradley A. Mullens, Joy A. Mench, Vezzoli, G., Mullens, B. A., and Mench, J. A.
- Subjects
Mite Infestations ,mites ,animal structures ,preening ,Biology ,Louse ,medicine.disease_cause ,hen ,Random Allocation ,Animal science ,biology.animal ,parasitic diseases ,Infestation ,medicine ,Mite ,Animals ,Animal Husbandry ,Poultry Diseases ,beak-trimming ,integumentary system ,Beak ,General Medicine ,Lice Infestations ,lice ,biology.organism_classification ,Grooming ,Female ,Animal Science and Zoology ,Chickens - Abstract
The effects of beak condition on ectoparasite populations and preening in laying hens were investigated. Beak-trimmed and beak-intact caged Hy-Line W-36 hens were infested with either chicken body lice or northern fowl mites using a 2 × 2 factorial design with 4 replicate cages (each containing 2 hens)/treatment: 1) BTL (beak-trimmed lice-infested); 2) BTM (beak-trimmed mite-infested); 3) BIL (beak-intact lice-infested); and 4) BIM (beak-intact mite-infested). Mite scores and lice numbers were estimated weekly. Hens were video recorded the wk before infestation and at wk 6 and 9 post-infestation. Time spent preening on 6 body areas and in total were analyzed using a repeated measures ANOVA. There was a wk × beak condition interaction for lice loads, with BTL harboring approximately 17 times more lice than BIL from wk 7 to 10 post-infestation (P < 0.0001). Beak condition affected mite loads (P < 0.0001), with BTM having a higher mite score (3.8 ± 0.26) than BIM (1.4 ± 0.26). At peak infestation, BTL spent more total time preening (P = 0.02, s ± SE: 232.1 ± 37.6) than prior to infestation (33.9 ± 37.6) and directed their preening behavior towards the vent. In contrast, BIL (73.9 ± 37.6), BTM (9.4 ± 1.6), and BIM (8.6 ± 1.6) did not increase total time spent preening over pre-infestation levels (103.6 ± 37.6, 5.8 ± 1.6, 6.7 ± 1.6 respectively), although BTM did redirect their preening behavior toward the vent. This study confirmed previous studies showing that an intact beak is important for reducing ectoparasite infestations. Preening behavior increased in response to lice infestation, but only in beak-trimmed hens; preening behavior and louse load were correlated at peak infestation. In contrast, mite infestation did not lead to increased preening, and there was no correlation between preening and mite load. However, both lice- and mite-infested hens directed preening behavior predominantly towards the vent where these parasites are typically found.
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
14. Comparative evaluation of three egg production systems: Housing characteristics and management practices
- Author
-
J. C. Swanson, Yang Zhao, Joy A. Mench, Hongwei Xin, Timothy A. Shepherd, and Darrin M. Karcher
- Subjects
Eggs ,enriched colony ,Animal Welfare ,Comparative evaluation ,Midwestern United States ,aviary ,Animal science ,Stocking ,conventional cage ,Animals ,Animal behavior ,Animal Husbandry ,Management practices ,Mathematics ,hen house ,General Medicine ,egg production ,Housing, Animal ,Manure ,Animal Science and Zoology ,Worker health ,Coalition for Sustainable Egg Supply ,Female ,Cage ,Chickens - Abstract
This paper is an integral part of the special publication series that arose from the multidisciplinary and multi-institutional project of the Coalition for Sustainable Egg Supply (CSES). The CSES project involves 3 housing systems for egg production at the same research farm site in the Midwest, USA, namely, a conventional cage (CC) house, an aviary (AV) house, and an enriched colony (EC) house. The CC house (141.4 m L × 26.6 m W × 6.1 m H) had a nominal capacity of 200,000 hens (6 hens in a cage at a stocking density of 516 cm2/hen), and the cages were arranged in 10 rows, 8 tiers per cage row, with a perforated aisle walkway at 4-tier height. The AV house (154.2 m L × 21.3 m W × 3.0 m H) and the EC house (154.2 m L × 13.7 m W × 4.0 m H) each had a nominal capacity of 50,000 hens. The AV house had 6 rows of aviary colonies, and the EC house had 5 rows of 4-tier enriched colonies containing perches, nestbox, and scratch pads (60 hens per colony at a stocking density of 752 cm2/hen). The overarching goal of the CSES project, as stated in the opening article of this series, was to comprehensively evaluate the 3 egg production systems from the standpoints of animal behavior and well-being, environmental impact, egg safety and quality, food affordability, and worker health. So that all the area-specific papers would not have to repeat a detailed description of the production systems and the management practices, this paper is written to provide such a description and to be used as a common reference for the companion papers.
- Published
- 2015
15. Running around the clock: competition, aggression and temporal partitioning of running wheel use in male mice
- Author
-
Joy A. Mench and Christopher L. Howerton
- Subjects
education.field_of_study ,Aggression ,Ecology ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Resource quality ,Population ,Male mice ,Biology ,Competitive advantage ,Competition (biology) ,Statistics ,medicine ,Animal Science and Zoology ,House mice ,medicine.symptom ,education ,human activities ,Ecology, Evolution, Behavior and Systematics ,media_common - Abstract
The strategies that animals use to obtain valuable resources are affected by resource quality and availability (e.g. spatial distribution), population dynamics (e.g. competitor-to-resource ratio) and individual characteristics (e.g. competitive ability). Theoretical papers and field observations suggest that aggressive resource competition, particularly by individuals with high competitive ability, increases as the competitor-to-resource ratio increases. On the other hand, individuals with low competitive ability should adopt a scramble competitive strategy, for example by expanding their home range or utilizing resources at suboptimal times. We tested these theoretical predictions in male house mice, Mus musculus domesticus, by experimentally manipulating the distribution of a desirable resource, a running wheel. Four male mice were exposed to three treatments (zero, one or four running wheels present) using a repeated measures design. We found that aggression increased as the competitor-to-resource ratio increased. In addition, mice with high competitive ability had species-typical nocturnally biased activity and running wheel use patterns, while mice with low competitive ability had more diurnally biased activity and running wheel use patterns. Our results show that aggression encourages less competitive mice to adopt a scramble competitive strategy through temporal partitioning and provides empirical evidence for the predictions of resource defence theory.
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
16. Personality predicts cognitive bias in captive psittacines, Amazona amazonica
- Author
-
Joy A. Mench and Victoria A. Cussen
- Subjects
media_common.quotation_subject ,Cognition ,Attentional bias ,Neuroticism ,Cognitive bias ,Developmental psychology ,medicine ,Anxiety ,Personality ,Animal Science and Zoology ,medicine.symptom ,Personality Assessment Inventory ,Psychology ,Ecology, Evolution, Behavior and Systematics ,Cognitive psychology ,Vigilance (psychology) ,media_common - Abstract
The prevalence of stable behavioural differences between individuals of a species (i.e. personality) is puzzling because it indicates that there are limits on animals’ behavioural plasticity and, therefore, optimality of behaviour. Personality may result from individual state characteristics (e.g. morphology or physiology). In turn, these characteristics can lead to differential fitness outcomes for individuals. Cognitive processing of environmental information may be such a characteristic. We developed a subjective personality assessment for Amazona amazonica. We then assessed whether personality predicted a cognitive state difference in attention bias, as measured by the proportion of balks and errors when performing a spatial foraging task in the presence of a passive human observer. Attention biases occur either because individuals attend more quickly to certain environmental stimuli, or because they cannot disengage their attention from such stimuli. Two factors, ‘neuroticism’ and ‘extraversion’, accounted for 66% of the total variance in personality. There was individual variation between parrots’ scores on both personality factors and both factors were temporally consistent over 1 year. There was a significant correlation between neuroticism and attention bias. Evolutionarily, attention biases are selected for because the fitness cost of failing to attend to potential threats is much greater than the cost of expending energy attending to benign stimuli. Therefore, cognitive biases such as attention bias are logical candidate cognitive states driving stable personality differences. Our findings show that differences in personality in A. amazonica are correlated with attention bias, a biologically relevant difference in cognition.
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
17. Natural incubation patterns and the effects of exposing eggs to light at various times during incubation on post-hatch fear and stress responses in broiler (meat) chickens
- Author
-
Joy A. Mench and Gregory S. Archer
- Subjects
Cerebral laterality ,Broiler ,Anatomy ,Biology ,Tonic (physiology) ,Broodiness ,chemistry.chemical_compound ,Animal science ,Food Animals ,chemistry ,Corticosterone ,Light sensitive ,Animal Science and Zoology ,Incubation ,Nest box - Abstract
Although environmental conditions during incubation can affect poultry welfare, one factor often overlooked in the commercial incubation of eggs is light. Light stimulation during embryonic development is now known to affect the physiology and behavior of broiler (meat) chickens post-hatch, but little is known about the pattern of exposure needed to produce these effects. We determined how often naturally incubated eggs were exposed to light by giving 10 broody Junglefowl × New Hampshire Red hens a nestbox containing five fertile eggs and a light sensitive data logger which recorded nest attendance by determining whether light was reaching the eggs. On average hens stayed on the nest without leaving for 14.3 of the days of the 21-day incubation cycle, although they did leave periodically, particularly during the last week. Utilizing this information, we then investigated how the timing of light exposure during incubation affected fear and stress responsiveness post-hatch in broiler chickens. Eggs were either not exposed to light (0DL), or exposed to light throughout incubation (21DL) or during only either the last 2 weeks (14DL) or the last week (7DL) of incubation. Lighting pattern had a significant effect on all parameters measured, with the most consistent differences found between 0DL and 21DL. For the fear measures, the 0DL birds had a longer latency to emerge during an emergence test (62.7 versus 37.2 s), a longer latency to right during a tonic immobility test (223.8 versus 107.2 s), wing-flapped more intensely during an inversion test (7.03 versus 6.4 flaps/s), and vocalized more during an isolation test (172.7 versus 127.1/3 min) than the 21DL. For the stress measures, 0DL had a lower IgG titer (52,683 versus 97,375 units) and greater corticosterone response (1.18 versus 0.55 ng/mL) to the crating stressor than 21DL, and showed more composite asymmetry (1.96 versus 1.49 mm). The 14DL and 7DL groups were generally intermediate. Unlike dark-incubated chicks, all light-stimulated groups showed lateralization of escape direction during the emergence test, suggesting that light-induced cerebral laterality could play a role in the observed effects. However, the direction of lateralization differed depending upon timing of exposure. These results confirm the importance of light stimulation during incubation on the later behavior and physiology of broiler chickens, but also indicate that providing light only during the last week of incubation, which would coincide with the maximum light stimulation provided by hens’ excursions from the nest, is insufficient to produce these effects.
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
18. Size does matter: The effect of enclosure size on aggression and affiliation between female New Zealand White rabbits during mixing
- Author
-
Joy A. Mench and Annie J. Valuska
- Subjects
Adult female ,Aggression ,Enclosure ,Repeated measures design ,Affect (psychology) ,Developmental psychology ,Animal science ,Food Animals ,medicine ,Animal Science and Zoology ,New zealand white ,medicine.symptom ,Cage ,Psychology - Abstract
Social enrichment is increasingly employed to improve the welfare of laboratory animals, including rabbits. However, the high levels of injurious aggression that can occur when unfamiliar adult rabbits are introduced to one another are a barrier to implementing social housing. One factor that could affect aggressive interaction is the size of the enclosure in which the animals are introduced, although this has never been tested in rabbits. We evaluated the aggressive and affiliative behavior of 11 pairs of unfamiliar adult female New Zealand White rabbits during 1-h trials. Using a balanced repeated measures design, pairs were tested in a smaller (Small) enclosure the size of a double cage (0.76 m × 1.22 m × 0.61 m) and a larger (Large) enclosure (1.22 m × 1.22 m × 0.61 m), both of which contained PVC barriers that allowed contact but prevented injury. There were no main effects of treatment or treatment order on aggressive or affiliative behavior, but there were significant interaction effects (P = 0.01). During their first trial, pairs tested in the Large enclosure showed higher levels (P = 0.07) of affiliative behavior (mean number = 37.6 ± 5.0) and lower levels (P = 0.02) of aggressive behavior (back-transformed mean number = 1.1) than pairs tested in the Small enclosure (20.7 ± 3.0 and 3.5, respectively). During their second trials, pairs behaved as they did during their first trial, resulting in the opposite pattern: significantly more aggressive behavior in the Large enclosure than the Small enclosure (P = 0.002). In total, there were 47 bites observed among does first introduced in the Small enclosure, as compared to only one in the pairs first introduced in the Large enclosure. Overall, these findings indicate that the Large enclosure was effective at reducing aggression when rabbits were unfamiliar to one another, but that behavior during a subsequent interaction was determined by prior experience rather than enclosure size. More research is needed to determine whether does’ short-term behavior in an experimental apparatus is predictive of long-term pair housing success.
- Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
19. General Principles for the welfare of animals in production systems: The underlying science and its application
- Author
-
H. Rebecca Whay, Temple Grandin, David Mellor, Marek Špinka, David Fraser, Joy A. Mench, Neville G. Gregory, Sandra Edwards, Vincent Guyonnet, Ian J.H. Duncan, Stella Huertas, J.M. Huzzey, and Paul H Hemsworth
- Subjects
Veterinary Medicine ,General Veterinary ,Animal health ,Public economics ,business.industry ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Control (management) ,Animal husbandry ,Animal Welfare ,Biotechnology ,Animal welfare ,Animals ,Genetic selection ,Production (economics) ,Animal Science and Zoology ,Livestock ,Business ,Animal Husbandry ,Welfare ,media_common - Abstract
In 2012, the World Organisation for Animal Health adopted 10 'General Principles for the Welfare of Animals in Livestock Production Systems' to guide the development of animal welfare standards. The General Principles draw on half a century of scientific research relevant to animal welfare: (1) how genetic selection affects animal health, behaviour and temperament; (2) how the environment influences injuries and the transmission of diseases and parasites; (3) how the environment affects resting, movement and the performance of natural behaviour; (4) the management of groups to minimize conflict and allow positive social contact; (5) the effects of air quality, temperature and humidity on animal health and comfort; (6) ensuring access to feed and water suited to the animals' needs and adaptations; (7) prevention and control of diseases and parasites, with humane euthanasia if treatment is not feasible or recovery is unlikely; (8) prevention and management of pain; (9) creation of positive human-animal relationships; and (10) ensuring adequate skill and knowledge among animal handlers. Research directed at animal welfare, drawing on animal behaviour, stress physiology, veterinary epidemiology and other fields, complements more established fields of animal and veterinary science and helps to create a more comprehensive scientific basis for animal care and management.
- Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
20. An Epidemiological Approach to Welfare Research in Zoos: The Elephant Welfare Project
- Author
-
Janine L. Brown, Kathy Carlstead, Cheryl L. Meehan, and Joy A. Mench
- Subjects
medicine.medical_specialty ,Economic growth ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Elephants ,Animal Welfare ,Environmental protection ,Political science ,Epidemiology ,Social needs ,medicine ,Animals ,Humans ,media_common ,General Veterinary ,Scope (project management) ,Animal Welfare (journal) ,Research ,Welfare state ,Animal husbandry ,Housing, Animal ,Scale (social sciences) ,North America ,Animals, Zoo ,Animal Science and Zoology ,Epidemiologic Methods ,Welfare - Abstract
Multi-institutional studies of welfare have proven to be valuable in zoos but are hampered by limited sample sizes and difficulty in evaluating more than just a few welfare indicators. To more clearly understand how interactions of husbandry factors influence the interrelationships among welfare outcomes, epidemiological approaches are needed as well as multifactorial assessments of welfare. Many questions have been raised about the housing and care of elephants in zoos and whether their environmental and social needs are being met in a manner that promotes good welfare. This article describes the background and rationale for a large-scale study of elephant welfare in North American zoos funded by the (U.S.) Institute of Museum and Library Services. The goals of this project are to document the prevalence of positive and negative welfare states in 291 elephants exhibited in 72 Association of Zoos and Aquariums zoos and then determine the environmental, management, and husbandry factors that impact elephant welfare. This research is the largest scale nonhuman animal welfare project ever undertaken by the zoo community, and the scope of environmental variables and welfare outcomes measured is unprecedented.
- Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
21. Astroturf as a dustbathing substrate for laying hens
- Author
-
Gina M. Alvino, Gregory S. Archer, Joy A. Mench, and Cassandra B. Tucker
- Subjects
Animal science ,Pet therapy ,Food Animals ,Latin square ,medicine.medical_treatment ,medicine ,Animal-assisted therapy ,Animal Science and Zoology ,Biology ,Furnished cages ,Substrate (marine biology) ,Treatment period ,Human animal bond - Abstract
Furnished cages for laying hens often contain an Astroturf (AT) pad which may be sprinkled with feed to promote foraging and dustbathing. We evaluated AT and AT plus feed (ATF) to determine if hens use these substrates for dustbathing. Hens ( N = 30) without prior exposure to substrate were housed singly in cages at 34 weeks of age. Using a 3 × 3 Latin square design, groups of 10 hens were provided with a dustbox containing either sand (control), an AT pad, or an AT pad with 200 g of feed (ATF) delivered daily, with three 20-day treatment Periods. The treatment order was: sand–ATF–AT (1); ATF–AT–sand (2); AT–sand–ATF (3). Behavior was recorded for 8 or 9 days each Period, from 11:00 to 22:00 h. Three measures of dustbathing were evaluated: bout number per day, bout length, and total time spent dustbathing per day. Data were collected and processed to evaluate dustbathing activity in the substrate, on the wire floor and in both locations combined (overall). Data were analyzed using the GLM or Kruskal–Wallis and Dwass-Steel-Critchlow-Fligner tests. Although there were some differences in statistical significance between Periods, in general hens provided with sand exhibited fewer bouts both overall and on wire, and longer bouts both overall and one wire, than AT or ATF hens, and longer bouts both overall and in substrate than AT. Hens with sand also spent significantly (all P ≤ 0.04) more time dustbathing on substrate (median min = 18.71 ± 5.16) and less time on wire (0.59 ± 1.56) than AT (0.33 ± 2.62 and 13.39 ± 7.15, respectively) and ATF (2.05 ± 4.43 and 11.26 ± 4.37, respectively) treatments during Period 1, and also more time dustbathing on substrate (7.66 ± 8.13) than hens in the AT treatment (0.00 ± 2.52) during Period 2. Exposure to AT and ATF during the first two treatment periods reduced hens’ use of sand during the third treatment period, with only 11% of bouts occurring in sand during Period 3 as compared to 64% and 41% during Periods 1 and 2, respectively. These findings demonstrated that AT did not provide an adequate dustbathing substrate, even with the addition of feed, as hens were less likely to dustbathe on both AT and ATF than sand, and were also less likely to dustbathe on sand if they were initially exposed to AT and ATF as dustbathing substrates.
- Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
22. Incidence, Severity, and Welfare Implications of Lesions Observed Postmortem in Laying Hens from Commercial Noncage Farms in California and Iowa
- Author
-
Anya S. Kajlich, Darrell W. Trampel, Ashley E. Hill, Joy A. Mench, Rebecca L. Parsons, Suzanne T. Millman, and H. L. Shivaprasad
- Subjects
Veterinary medicine ,animal structures ,040301 veterinary sciences ,Biology ,medicine.disease_cause ,Animal Welfare ,California ,0403 veterinary science ,Lesion ,Food Animals ,Animal welfare ,Infestation ,medicine ,Prevalence ,Animals ,Poultry Diseases ,Feather pecking ,General Immunology and Microbiology ,Incidence (epidemiology) ,Incidence ,0402 animal and dairy science ,Cannibalism ,04 agricultural and veterinary sciences ,040201 dairy & animal science ,Housing, Animal ,Iowa ,Beak ,Feather ,visual_art ,visual_art.visual_art_medium ,Animal Science and Zoology ,Female ,medicine.symptom ,Chickens - Abstract
The egg industry is moving away from the use of conventional cages to enriched cage and noncage laying hen housing systems because of animal welfare concerns. In this study, the prevalence and severity of lesions in noncage laying hens from commercial farms in two of the largest egg-producing states, California and Iowa, were evaluated by postmortem examination. Hens that died or were culled were collected during early, mid, and late stages of the laying cycle from 16 houses on three farms. Of the 25 gross lesions identified for study, 22 were observed, with an average of four lesions per hen. Vent cannibalism, reduced feather cover, keel bone deformation, and beak abnormalities were the most frequent lesions, observed in ≥40% of hens. Other common lesions were cloacal prolapse (30.5%), footpad dermatitis (24.3%), and septicemia (23.1%). Beak abnormality and enteric disease had the highest proportion of severe lesions. Pearson chi-square analysis revealed a number of stage-of-lay effects (P ≤ 0.05), some of which differed by state. For both states combined, the lesions observed more frequently during early lay were beak abnormalities, northern fowl mite infestation, and cage layer fatigue, whereas during mid lay, they were poor feather cover, vent cannibalism, footpad dermatitis, keel bone deformation, respiratory disease and roundworms. Feather pecking and cloacal prolapse were most common during late lay. Although differences in hen genetics, farm management practices, and environmental factors could all have affected the results of this study, the information provides a better understanding of hen health in noncage housing systems and could help to identify potential interventions to reduce hen welfare problems.
- Published
- 2016
23. Contrast in light intensity, rather than day length, influences the behavior and health of broiler chickens
- Author
-
Joy A. Mench, R. A. Blatchford, and Gregory S. Archer
- Subjects
Male ,photoperiodism ,Time Factors ,Behavior, Animal ,Eye Diseases ,Light ,Photoperiod ,Broiler ,General Medicine ,Biology ,Intensity (physics) ,Light intensity ,Low contrast ,Animal science ,Animals ,Day length ,Female ,Animal Science and Zoology ,Circadian rhythm ,Animal Husbandry ,Chickens ,Lighting ,Poultry Diseases - Abstract
Day length and intensity are commonly manipulated aspects of the light environment in commercial broiler production. Both influence circadian rhythms, but it is unclear if they do this independently or synergistically. The effect of light:dark (20L:4D, 16L:8D) and intensity contrasts (1 lx:0.5 lx, 200 lx:0.5 lx) on broiler behavior and health (n=1,004, 4 replicates/treatment) was evaluated. Activity was measured using passive infrared detection, and feeding activity was measured by the amount of feed consumed/h over one 24-h period each week. Broilers were gait scored and weighed at 6 wk of age. Following euthanasia, eyes were dissected from 30 birds/treatment. Behavior and performance were analyzed using the GLM, gait score using the Kruskal-Wallis test, and eye measures using a MANOVA. The 200 lx birds were more active (P=0.03) and fed more (P=0.001) during the photophase but were less active (P=0.02) and fed less (P
- Published
- 2012
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
24. A system utilizing radio frequency identification (RFID) technology to monitor individual rodent behavior in complex social settings
- Author
-
Christopher L. Howerton, Joseph P. Garner, and Joy A. Mench
- Subjects
Data collection ,Behavior, Animal ,business.industry ,General Neuroscience ,Real-time computing ,Biology ,Radio Frequency Identification Device ,Mice ,Software ,Animals ,Radio-frequency identification ,Social Behavior ,business ,Behavioral Sciences ,Simulation - Abstract
Pre-clinical investigation of human CNS disorders relies heavily on mouse models. However these show low predictive validity for translational success to humans, partly due to the extensive use of rapid, high-throughput behavioral assays. Improved assays to monitor rodent behavior over longer time scales in a variety of contexts while still maintaining the efficiency of data collection associated with high-throughput assays are needed. We developed an apparatus that uses radio frequency identification device (RFID) technology to facilitate long-term automated monitoring of the behavior of mice in socially or structurally complex cage environments. Mice that were individually marked and implanted with transponders were placed in pairs in the apparatus, and their locations continuously tracked for 24 h. Video observation was used to validate the RFID readings. The apparatus and its associated software accurately tracked the locations of all mice, yielding information about each mouse's location over time, its diel activity patterns, and the amount of time it was in the same location as the other mouse in the pair. The information that can be efficiently collected in this apparatus has a variety of applications for pre-clinical research on human CNS disorders, for example major depressive disorder and autism spectrum disorder, in that it can be used to quantify validated endophenotypes or biomarkers of these disorders using rodent models. While the specific configuration of the apparatus described here was designed to answer particular experimental questions, it can be modified in various ways to accommodate different experimental designs.
- Published
- 2012
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
25. Reverse-translational biomarker validation of Abnormal Repetitive Behaviors in mice: An illustration of the 4P's modeling approach
- Author
-
Hanno Würbel, James D. Murray, Joy A. Mench, Collette M. Thogerson, Joseph P. Garner, and Brett D. Dufour
- Subjects
Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder ,Impulse control disorder ,Models, Neurological ,Article ,Trichotillomania ,Mice ,Behavioral Neuroscience ,Discrimination, Psychological ,Reward ,medicine ,Animals ,Autistic Disorder ,Neuropsychology ,Feeding Behavior ,medicine.disease ,Mice, Inbred C57BL ,Developmental disorder ,Affect ,Disease Models, Animal ,Stereotypy (non-human) ,Compulsive behavior ,Gambling ,Compulsive Behavior ,Set, Psychology ,Autism ,Biomarker (medicine) ,Orbitofrontal cortex ,Cues ,Stereotyped Behavior ,medicine.symptom ,Psychology ,Neuroscience ,Biomarkers ,Psychomotor Performance - Abstract
The NIMH's new strategic plan, with its emphasis on the "4P's" (Prediction, Pre-emption, Personalization, and Populations) and biomarker-based medicine requires a radical shift in animal modeling methodology. In particular 4P's models will be non-determinant (i.e. disease severity will depend on secondary environmental and genetic factors); and validated by reverse-translation of animal homologues to human biomarkers. A powerful consequence of the biomarker approach is that different closely related disorders have a unique fingerprint of biomarkers. Animals can be validated as a highly specific model of a single disorder by matching this 'fingerprint'; or as a model of a symptom seen in multiple disorders by matching common biomarkers. Here we illustrate this approach with two Abnormal Repetitive Behaviors (ARBs) in mice: stereotypies and barbering (hair pulling). We developed animal versions of the neuropsychological biomarkers that distinguish human ARBs, and tested the fingerprint of the different mouse ARBs. As predicted, the two mouse ARBs were associated with different biomarkers. Both barbering and stereotypy could be discounted as models of OCD (even though they are widely used as such), due to the absence of limbic biomarkers which are characteristic of OCD and hence are necessary for a valid model. Conversely barbering matched the fingerprint of trichotillomania (i.e. selective deficits in set-shifting), suggesting it may be a highly specific model of this disorder. In contrast stereotypies were correlated only with a biomarker (deficits in response shifting) correlated with stereotypies in multiple disorders, suggesting that animal stereotypies model stereotypies in multiple disorders.
- Published
- 2011
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
26. Factors affecting nest choice by Pekin ducks
- Author
-
M. M. Makagon, Joy A. Mench, and Cassandra B. Tucker
- Subjects
Veterinary medicine ,medicine.medical_treatment ,Animal-assisted therapy ,Biology ,Human animal bond ,Pet therapy ,Animal science ,Food Animals ,Nest ,medicine ,Preference testing ,Animal Science and Zoology ,Nest site ,Flock ,Nest box - Abstract
To encourage nest use in breeder poultry flocks, it is important that nest boxes are attractive. Few studies have evaluated factors affecting nest attractiveness to ducks. We assessed the effects of nest box experience (Experiments 1 and 2) and features of nests, including degree of enclosure (Experiments 1 and 2) and the presence of an egg (Experiment 3), on nest site selection by sexually mature Pekin ducks. The hens were tested individually in pens containing different nest box choices. Nest preferences were determined based on the locations of 14 successively laid eggs. Ducks in Experiment 1 were provided with nest box access prior to the onset of lay (20 weeks of age, EXP group, N = 12) or at the time of preference testing (35 weeks of age, DELAYED-EXP group, N = 12). Delaying nest box access by 15 weeks did not significantly affect the proportion of eggs laid in nest boxes rather than on the floor ( P = 0.40). Regardless of experience ducks laid more than 66% of their eggs in nest boxes offering a higher degree of enclosure. Ducks in Experiment 2 were reared with either open-top (OP, N = 12) or closed-top (CL, N = 12) boxes. Regardless of rearing condition, ducks preferred boxes with the highest level of enclosure offered. Ducks laid twice the expected proportion (25%) of eggs in CL boxes with curtains ( P = 0.0004). CL boxes without curtains were used as predicted by chance, 25% ( P = 0.746), while OP boxes with ( P = 0.009) and without ( P = 0.015) curtains each contained only half the expected number of eggs. Ducks in Experiment 3 were provided with two nest boxes, one of which contained the egg laid the previous day. Each day, the newly laid egg was marked and either placed back in the nest box where it was found (HANDLED, N = 12) or moved into the adjacent nest box (MOVED, N = 12). Ducks in HANDLED and MOVED laid 97.6% and 79.8%, respectively, of their eggs in boxes containing the previous day's egg. While ducks in HANDLED were consistent in their choice throughout the test, those in MOVED developed the preference over time ( P = 0.039), suggesting that the preference for laying in a nest containing an egg may be influenced by experience. These results indicate that nest box enclosure and the presence of an egg are important in determining the nesting choices of Pekin ducks. Incorporating these features into nest boxes may be useful for increasing consistency of nest use in breeder duck flocks.
- Published
- 2011
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
27. Light intensity during rearing affects the behavioural synchrony and resting patterns of broiler chickens
- Author
-
Gregory S. Archer, Gina M. Alvino, Joy A. Mench, and R. A. Blatchford
- Subjects
Male ,photoperiodism ,Periodicity ,Behavior, Animal ,Light ,Photoperiod ,Rest ,Broiler ,General Medicine ,Anatomy ,Biology ,Housing, Animal ,Light intensity ,Animal science ,Light Cycle ,Animals ,Female ,Animal Science and Zoology ,Chickens ,Food Science - Abstract
1. The effect of light intensity on behavioural synchrony and rest in broilers was investigated by randomly assigning 504 Cobb chicks to environmental chambers at low (5 lux), moderate (50 lux), or high (200 lux) daytime illumination under a 16 h L : 8 h D light cycle. We hypothesised that behavioural synchrony and resting behaviour would be affected by light : dark amplitude, and predicted that broilers reared at higher intensities would show increased synchrony and thus have fewer, but longer, resting bouts during the scotophase, since these bouts would be less likely to be interrupted by active birds. 2. Digital recordings were made for 48 h from weeks 3 to 5 of age, and scan samples taken of 11 behaviours at 15-min intervals. Z-scores for the amount of synchronisation were calculated using the kappa coefficient of agreement. Frequency and length of resting bouts and the number of resting bout interruptions by flockmates were also calculated from continuous observations of 4 focal broilers per pen. 3. Resting and sitting behaviour were significantly synchronised or showed a strong trend towards synchronisation in all treatments during the 3 weeks of observation. There were significant treatment differences in the degree of synchrony for preen, eat, rest and forage, with broilers reared in 200 lux showing the greatest degree of synchrony. 4. During the scotophase, broilers reared with high illumination had fewer, longer, and less interrupted bouts of resting than those reared with moderate or low illumination. During the photophase, broilers in the low-illumination treatment had more frequent, longer, and more interrupted resting bouts than those in the moderate or high illumination treatment. 5. In conclusion, rearing broilers under a 16 h L : 8 h D photoperiod with high daytime light intensity resulted in greater behavioural synchrony in the flock, with the potential to improve welfare by increasing uninterrupted resting behaviour during the dark phase.
- Published
- 2009
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
28. Behavioural time budgets of broiler chickens reared in varying light intensities
- Author
-
Joy A. Mench, Gregory S. Archer, and Gina M. Alvino
- Subjects
photoperiodism ,Veterinary medicine ,medicine.medical_treatment ,Broiler ,Animal-assisted therapy ,Biology ,Human animal bond ,Light level ,Light intensity ,Pet therapy ,Animal science ,Food Animals ,Light management ,medicine ,Animal Science and Zoology - Abstract
Light management is a critical component of production for intensively housed animals. To increase productivity, broiler chickens are often raised commercially in houses that are dimly illuminated on a near-continuous basis. There have been relatively few studies examining the effects of light intensity on the behaviour of broilers. We evaluated the time budgets of broilers (N = 464) housed under one of three photophase light levels (5, 50 or 200 lx) chosen to produce low, moderate or high illumination contrasts with the scotophase light level (1 lx). There were four replicate pens of broilers per treatment. We recorded behaviour for 48 continuous hours during weeks 3, 4, and 5 of age and analysed the time broilers spent exhibiting eight behaviours and two behavioural categories (inactive and active) during the overall photoperiod, photophase and scotophase. There were only two main effects of light intensity on behaviour during the overall photoperiod: 5 lx broilers preened and foraged less than the 50 (P = 0.03, P = 0.03, respectively) and 200 lx birds (P
- Published
- 2009
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
29. Effect of providing light during incubation on the health, productivity, and behavior of broiler chickens
- Author
-
Gregory S. Archer, H. L. Shivaprasad, and Joy A. Mench
- Subjects
Male ,Veterinary medicine ,animal structures ,Behavior, Animal ,Light ,Broiler ,Chick Embryo ,General Medicine ,Motor Activity ,Biology ,Continuous light ,Feed conversion ratio ,Incubators ,Light level ,Feeding behavior ,Animal science ,Productivity (ecology) ,Darkness ,Animals ,Female ,Animal Science and Zoology ,Chickens ,Incubation ,Ovum - Abstract
Environmental stimuli present during incubation can affect the behavior and health of birds posthatch. Commercial broiler chickens are often incubated in complete darkness, receiving light only intermittently when the incubator is opened. To determine the effect of providing light during the prehatch period, we incubated Cobb broiler eggs (n = 664) under continuous light (24L:0D), no light (0L:24D), or 12 h of light (12L:12D); the light level was 550 lx. Various parameters were measured posthatch in these broilers (n = 506), which were raised in floor pens on a 12L:12D regimen. There were no incubation treatment differences in hatchability, mortality, growth, feed consumption, feed conversion ratio, gait score, overall feeding behavior activity, or general behavioral activity. However, broilers hatched from eggs incubated under 24L:0D or 12L:12D had greater feeding activity (P < 0.05) than the 0L:24D broilers during the 2 h after the lights came on. Eye dimensions did not differ between treatments, but the eyes of the 12L:12D broilers weighed less (P < 0.05) than those of the other treatments. Broilers incubated under 0L:24D had a greater level of composite physical asymmetry (1.87 +/- 0.11 mm), considered to be an indicator of developmental stress, than did 12L:12D (1.57 +/- 0.09 mm; P = 0.05) broilers; they also tended to have more composite asymmetry than 24L:0D (1.62 +/- 0.11 mm; P = 0.10) broilers. The results of this study indicate that providing light during incubation has no detrimental effect on production or health of broilers, but does have potential benefits in terms of reducing the effects of stressors associated with production and growth.
- Published
- 2009
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
30. Effects of a running wheel-igloo enrichment on aggression, hierarchy linearity, and stereotypy in group-housed male CD-1 (ICR) mice
- Author
-
Christopher L. Howerton, Joseph P. Garner, and Joy A. Mench
- Subjects
Environmental enrichment ,Aggression ,medicine.medical_treatment ,Animal-assisted therapy ,Social behaviour ,Developmental psychology ,Dominance hierarchy ,Dominance (ethology) ,Animal science ,Food Animals ,Stereotypy ,medicine ,Animal Science and Zoology ,medicine.symptom ,Psychology ,Cage - Abstract
Environmental enrichment is an important strategy to improve the welfare of laboratory animals. Running wheels might serve as enrichment devices for laboratory mice, since mice use them extensively when they are provided. However, competition among group-housed mice for such highly preferred enrichment devices could also lead to injurious aggression. We therefore assessed the effects of providing a running wheel-igloo enrichment on the social behaviour of group-housed male mice. CD-1 (ICR) mice (n = 60, 20 per treatment) were housed in standard polycarbonate cages (five mice per cage) for 2 weeks (BASE), and then placed in either: (1) a standard cage with bedding (CONTROL), or (2) same as CONTROL, but with the addition of a Bio-Serv® mouse running wheel-igloo (WHEEL) or (3) same as WHEEL, but with the running wheel glued in place so that it could not rotate (FIXED). Social behaviour and dominance ranks were assessed at the end of BASE and 2 weeks after imposition of treatments. There was increased escalated aggression in both enriched cages (FIXED, Tukey: T8 = −8.31; P = 0.0003; WHEEL, Tukey: T8 = −4.577; P = 0.0153), and decreased stereotypy in WHEEL (Tukey: T8 = 4.226; P = 0.0237). There was also a decrease in dominance hierarchy linearity in enriched treatments (FIXED, Tukey: T9 = 3.653; P = 0.0433; WHEEL, Tukey: T9 = 6.273; P = 0.0014) and a negative correlation between hierarchy linearity and escalated aggression overall (GLM: F1,7 = 61.41; P < 0.001; partial r = 0.90). Thus, providing the running wheel-igloo enrichment had a negative effect in that it increased potentially injurious aggression. Additionally, the decrease in hierarchy linearity suggests that the social structure in enriched groups was disrupted. The cause of the decrease in cage stereotypies in WHEEL is unknown, but could be because these stereotypies were re-directed towards stereotypic wheel-running, or because wheel-running reduced frustration due to limited opportunities for exploratory locomotion or caused a shift in the behavioural time budget. Despite this decrease in stereotypy, this form of enrichment is not beneficial for group-housed male CD-1 mice due to the associated increased aggression. However, female mice or males of less aggressive mouse strains might respond differently, and singly housed mice should generally benefit from such enrichment.
- Published
- 2008
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
31. Farm animal welfare in the U.S.A.: Farming practices, research, education, regulation, and assurance programs
- Author
-
Joy A. Mench
- Subjects
Public economics ,business.industry ,medicine.medical_treatment ,Environmental resource management ,Animal-assisted therapy ,Legislation ,Audit ,Ethology ,Food Animals ,Agriculture ,Animal welfare ,medicine ,Animal Science and Zoology ,business ,Research education ,Public awareness - Abstract
What impact has the Brambell Report, and the growth of the field of applied ethology, had on the United States of America? There has been increasing public awareness of farm animal welfare issues and a concomitant increase in animal welfare research and teaching activities. This has occurred more slowly in the U.S.A. than in Europe, but is now gathering momentum as the agricultural industries and food retailers write guidelines and implement animal welfare auditing programs in an attempt to reassure consumers that farm animals are raised and slaughtered humanely. Applied ethologists have played an important role in these advances, not just in terms of providing relevant scientific research results, but also as consultants and members of advisory committees. This is likely to continue, although the long-term effectiveness of depending upon voluntary standards and auditing programs to foster sustained industry-wide improvements in farm animal welfare and to reassure consumers remains to be seen.
- Published
- 2008
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
32. Effects of bill-trimming Muscovy ducks on behavior, body weight gain, and bill morphopathology
- Author
-
Heng-Wei Cheng, Joy A. Mench, Edmond A. Pajor, Leslie A. Gustafson, and Joseph P. Garner
- Subjects
Veterinary medicine ,Feather pecking ,animal structures ,Food Animals ,Treatment difference ,viruses ,Animal Science and Zoology ,Biology ,Body weight ,Scan sampling ,Upper bill ,Morning ,Skin damage - Abstract
In commercial production facilities, ducks are often bill-trimmed to prevent feather pecking and cannibalism. Beak-trimming of chickens and turkeys has been criticized because of its potential to cause acute and chronic pain, but little is known about the effects of bill-trimming on the welfare of ducks. We conducted a study to determine the effects of a commercial bill-trimming method, cutting without cautery, on the behavior and bill morphopathology of ducks. Muscovy ducks were housed in six pens each containing 16 ducks (eight males and eight females). Three pens of ducks were trimmed (TRIM) using scissors at 20 days post-hatch, while ducks in the remaining three pens were sham-trimmed (NOTRIM). The average length removed from the upper bill was 0.51cm, or 21.1% of the bill length from nares to tip. The behavior of all ducks was recorded during the first week post-trim using scan sampling. In addition, two ducks/sex/pen were randomly selected as focal birds, and observed using 15-min focal samples during the morning (0900–1100) and afternoon (1200–1500) for 7 weeks post-trim. Ducks were weighed weekly. Behavioral and body weight data were analyzed using the General Linear Model. At 12 weeks of age, the male focal ducks were killed and their bills were then collected and subjected to gross and morphopathological analysis. In the days immediately post-trim, TRIM ducks spent significantly less ( p p =0.0064) than NOTRIM, but there was no treatment difference in weights by 2 weeks post-trim. By 6 weeks post-trim the upper bill of TRIM was only 11.9% shorter than the lower bill. There was evidence of feather pecking in the TRIM pens, but feather pecking and skin damage were more extensive in the NOTRIM pens. The TRIM bill stumps were covered with epithelium, lacked blood vessels and showed evidence of scarring, but there were no neuromas. These results are consistent with this bill-trimming method causing acute, but not chronic, pain in Muscovy ducks.
- Published
- 2007
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
33. The challenge of challenge: Can problem solving opportunities enhance animal welfare?
- Author
-
Cheryl L. Meehan and Joy A. Mench
- Subjects
Coping (psychology) ,Empirical research ,Food Animals ,Social skills ,Foraging ,Animal Science and Zoology ,Cognition ,Complex problem solving ,Cognitive skill ,Psychology ,Social psychology ,Eustress ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Cognitive mechanisms are an important part of the organization of the behavior systems of animals. In the wild, animals regularly face problems that they must overcome in order to survive and thrive. Solving such problems often requires animals to process, store, retrieve, and act upon information from the environment—in other words, to use their cognitive skills. For example, animals may have to use navigational, tool-making or cooperative social skills in order to procure their food. However, many enrichment programs for captive animals do not include the integration of these types of cognitive challenges. Thus, foraging enrichments typically are designed to facilitate the physical expression of feeding behaviors such as food-searching and food consumption, but not to facilitate complex problem solving behaviors related to food acquisition. Challenging animals by presenting them with problems is almost certainly a source of frustration and stress. However, we suggest here that this is an important, and even necessary, feature of an enrichment program, as long as animals also possess the skills and resources to effectively solve the problems with which they are presented. We discuss this with reference to theories about the emotional consequences of coping with challenge, the association between lack of challenge and the development of abnormal behavior, and the benefits of stress (arousal) in facilitating learning and memory of relevant skills. Much remains to be done to provide empirical support for these theories. However, they do point the way to a practical approach to improving animal welfare—to design enrichments to facilitate the cognitive mechanisms which underlie the performance of complex behaviors that cannot be performed due to the restrictions inherent to the captive environment.
- Published
- 2007
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
34. Is fearfulness a trait that can be measured with behavioural tests? A validation of four fear tests for Japanese quail
- Author
-
Katherine A. Miller, Joy A. Mench, and Joseph P. Garner
- Subjects
biology ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Discriminant validity ,biology.organism_classification ,Developmental psychology ,Test (assessment) ,Surprise ,Convergent validity ,Coturnix coturnix ,Trait ,Animal Science and Zoology ,Internal validity ,Psychology ,Ecology, Evolution, Behavior and Systematics ,Factor analysis ,media_common ,Clinical psychology - Abstract
If fearfulness is stable, consistent and trait-like, then valid measures of fearfulness should be stable, consistent and independent of influences unrelated to fear. We assessed the validity of six fear measures using Japanese quail, Coturnix coturnix japonica, a common species in fear research. Measures were made during emergence, novel object, novel food and predator surprise tests. These were considered to have internal validity if they were stable over 18 days, when we controlled for nonexperimental variables including season of testing and cage location. We determined convergent and discriminant validity by factor analysis of fear measures plus measures of sociality, activity level and repetitive behaviour. Fear measures with good convergent validity showed agreement in their factor loadings. Those with good discriminant validity loaded on to different factors from nonfear measures. Most of the fear measures examined were moderately stable over time, but only half had good discriminant validity. Convergence was good among measures from the same test but poor across tests. Measures from each fear test loaded separately. Overall, flight distance and freezing duration in the predator surprise test and amount eaten in the novel food test showed the best internal, convergent and discriminant validity. When we considered only these three measures, convergence remained higher among measures from the same test than from different tests. Fearfulness thus appeared somewhat unstable over time and inconsistent across situations, which, if true across species, greatly limits the utility of fear tests.
- Published
- 2006
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
35. The differential effects of four types of environmental enrichment on the activity budgets, fearfulness, and social proximity preference of Japanese quail
- Author
-
Katherine A. Miller and Joy A. Mench
- Subjects
Environmental enrichment ,biology ,Social proximity ,Foraging ,Differential effects ,Preference ,Quail ,Developmental psychology ,Animal science ,Food Animals ,biology.animal ,General activity ,Animal Science and Zoology ,Psychology ,Flight distance - Abstract
Environmental enrichment falls into four general categories: foraging opportunities, structural complexity, sensory stimulation/novelty, and social companionship. Each may have distinct behavioural effects. We examined the use of these four enrichment types, and their effects on activity budgets (foraging, general activity, pacing), social proximity preference (time spent observing companions through a window), and fear (flight distance in a predator surprise test, consumption of novel food) in adult Japanese quail. Foraging and structural enrichments were used extensively (29 and 26% of scans, respectively), as were the dustbaths available in all pens (16%). Use of enrichments and the dustbath was consistent over 24 h and over 17 d (partial r > 0.42, p F 3,89 = 5.66, p F 3,89 = 4.75, p F 3,89 = 6.33, p = 0.001). Novelty enrichment had no significant effects. Social housing reduced use of enrichments (enrichment treatment × housing type F 3,89 = 3.30, p F 1,89 = 4.59, p F 1,45 = 5.43, p F 1,45 = 7.88, p F 1,89 = 13.35, p F 1,89 = 38.18, p F 1,89 = 34.83, p
- Published
- 2005
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
36. The test–retest reliability of four behavioural tests of fearfulness for quail: a critical evaluation
- Author
-
Joy A. Mench, Joseph P. Garner, and Katherine A. Miller
- Subjects
medicine.medical_specialty ,biology ,medicine.medical_treatment ,Novel object ,Animal-assisted therapy ,Audiology ,Emergence test ,Quail ,Test (assessment) ,Developmental psychology ,Correlation ,Pet therapy ,Food Animals ,biology.animal ,medicine ,Animal Science and Zoology ,Psychology ,Reliability (statistics) - Abstract
Tests of animal fearfulness are being used in a broad range of basic and applied scientific fields. However, rigorous assessment of the test–retest reliability of these tests is relatively uncommon. The importance of such assessment is illustrated by this study of the responses of Japanese quail during four tests of fearfulness. Forty-six adult quail were subjected to an emergence test, novel object test, novel food test, and predator surprise test on two consecutive days to assess test–retest reliability. Multiple behavioural measures were made during each test. One test was performed per day. There were discrepancies in the reliabilities of the various measures made within each test. For example, latency to extend the head into the emergence arena was poorly correlated on two consecutive days (partial r =.19, n =46, P =.29), but the number of times the birds extended their heads into the arena was much more reliable (partial r =.73, n =46, P r =.47, n =46, P r =.81, n =46, P r =.19, Pearson's r =.52). Finally, the statistically significant but small correlation coefficients for many measures in this study and other published fear studies, many less than .4, indicate that some common behavioural measures of fearfulness are not always reliable.
- Published
- 2005
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
37. Social and husbandry factors affecting the prevalence and severity of barbering (‘whisker trimming’) by laboratory mice
- Author
-
Joseph P. Garner, Sandra M. Weisker, Laura E. Gregg, Brett D. Dufour, and Joy A. Mench
- Subjects
Stereotypy (non-human) ,Dominance (ethology) ,integumentary system ,Food Animals ,Animal welfare ,Animal Science and Zoology ,Animal husbandry ,Psychology ,Hair plucking ,Relative dominance ,Developmental psychology ,Repetitive behavior - Abstract
Barbering—the plucking of fur or whiskers from cagemates or oneself—is a common form of abnormal repetitive behavior in laboratory mice. It is often viewed as a ‘normal’ behavior of particular strains, primarily because it is assumed to represent a dominance behavior. Here, we report on a series of experiments investigating the husbandry and social factors involved in barbering. In Experiment 1, we conducted a large-scale epidemiological study. Barbering was found to be related to a number of environmental factors, including cage design, cage location, cagemate relatedness, and the presence of other barbers in the cage. Thus, instead of barbers suppressing the development of barbering in cagemates, as would be expected if barbers were dominant, the presence of a barber in the cage facilitated the behavior in cagemates. In Experiment 2, we quantified the patterns of hair loss seen in barbered mice. These data confirmed that individual barbers pluck idiosyncratic patterns of fur from their cagemates, and that when there were multiple barbers in a cage the patterns plucked by cagemates were significantly more similar than the patterns plucked by non-cagemates. Thus, barbers appear to be facilitating or learning idiosyncratic patterns from their barbering cagemates. Aspects of these data question the assumption that barbering is a dominance behavior. Therefore in Experiment 3, we directly investigated the relationship between dominance and barbering in C57BL/6J mice. The relative dominance score of a mouse within its cage was unrelated to barbering, and the relative severity of hair loss on a mouse was unrelated to its relative dominance. Barbering, therefore, cannot be considered to be a dominance behavior. Instead, our data indicate that it is most likely a form of abnormal repetitive behavior related to compulsive hair plucking (trichotillomania) in humans. The incorrect assumption that barbering represents an eccentric but benign dominance behavior has allowed the welfare consequences of this behavior to remain unaddressed. Barbering should therefore be considered an important variable both in the assessment of the welfare of individual mice and in experiments addressing the impact of husbandry paradigms upon the behavior and welfare of laboratory mice.
- Published
- 2004
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
38. A behavioral comparison of New Zealand White rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus) housed individually or in pairs in conventional laboratory cages
- Author
-
Joseph P. Garner, Joy A. Mench, and Ling-ru Chu
- Subjects
Veterinary medicine ,medicine.medical_treatment ,Repeated measures design ,Animal-assisted therapy ,Biting ,Animal science ,Food Animals ,Animal welfare ,medicine ,HUBzero ,Animal Science and Zoology ,New zealand white ,Psychology ,Disease transmission ,Food competition - Abstract
Despite their gregarious nature, rabbits used for research are often housed individually due to concerns about aggression and disease transmission. However, conventional laboratory cages restrict movement, and rabbits housed singly in these cages often perform abnormal behaviors, an indication of compromised welfare. Pairing rabbits in double-sized cages could potentially improve welfare by providing both increased space and social stimulation. We compared the behavior of female New Zealand White rabbits ( Oryctolagus cuniculus ) housed either individually ( N =4) in cages measuring 61 cm ×76 cm ×41 cm or in non-littermate pairs (four pairs) in double-wide cages measuring 122 cm ×76 cm ×41 cm. The rabbits were kept under a reversed photoperiod (lights on 22:00–12:00 h). Each rabbit was observed five times per week for 5 months, using 15-min focal animal samples taken between 08:00–09:00, 12:00–13:00, and 16:00–17:00 h. Data were analyzed using a repeated measures General Linear Model (GLM). Over the 5 months, individually housed rabbits showed an increase in the proportion of the total behavioral time budget spent engaged in abnormal behaviors (digging, floor chewing, bar biting), from 0.25 to 1.77%, while pairs remained unchanged at 0.95% (treatment×time interaction, F 1,24 =4.60; P ≤0.0422). Paired rabbits engaged in more locomotor behavior ( F 1,6 =16.49; P ≤0.0066) than individual rabbits (average proportions of time budget: 2.71 and 0.70% for paired and individual rabbits, respectively), which may be important because caged rabbits are susceptible to osteoporosis and other bone abnormalities due to the restricted ability to move. Time spent feeding and body weights of dominant and subordinate rabbits in a pair did not differ, indicating that food competition was not a problem, and paired rabbits were often observed in physical contact (26.7% of data records) although the size of the cages allowed physical separation. Aggression between pairmates did not increase significantly during the study. However, one pair did have to be separated at the end of the study due to bite wounds from persistent aggression. Thus, although methods for decreasing injurious aggression require further investigation, the beneficial effects of pair housing in decreasing abnormal behaviors and increasing locomotion suggest that pair housing should be considered as an alternative to individual housing for caged laboratory rabbits.
- Published
- 2004
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
39. Stereotypies in caged parrots, schizophrenia and autism: evidence for a common mechanism
- Author
-
C.L Meehan, Joseph P. Garner, and Joy A. Mench
- Subjects
Male ,Psychosis ,Perseveration ,Video Recording ,Developmental psychology ,Behavioral Neuroscience ,Parrots ,Reaction Time ,medicine ,Animals ,Autistic Disorder ,Behavior, Animal ,medicine.disease ,Developmental disorder ,Disease Models, Animal ,Stereotypy (non-human) ,Social Isolation ,Disinhibition ,Schizophrenia ,Impulsive Behavior ,Autism ,Female ,Stereotyped Behavior ,medicine.symptom ,Abnormality ,Psychology - Abstract
Spontaneously occurring abnormal behaviors in animals have recently received considerable attention, both in veterinary medicine and as a potential model for abnormal behavior in several human mental disorders. Stereotypies are abnormal repetitive, unvarying, and functionless behaviors that are often performed by captive and domesticated animals housed in barren environments. They closely resemble the stereotypies of autistic and mentally retarded patients, stereotypies of unmedicated chronic schizophrenic patients, certain classes of simple tic in Tourette's syndrome, and several drug-induced behaviors. However, evidence for a common mechanism has been lacking. Stereotypies in human mental disorders are indicative of profound brain dysfunction involving the basal ganglia, and are associated with pervasive voluntary-motor impairments and psychological distress. Here we show that stereotypy in captive Orange-Wing Amazon Parrots (Amazona amazonica) is correlated with poor performance on the same psychiatric task (the 'gambling task') as stereotypy in autistic and schizophrenic patients. The task measures recurrent perseveration-the tendency to inappropriately repeat responses. Thus, the more stereotypy a parrot performed, the more likely it was to inappropriately repeat itself from trial-to-trial on the task; and the more rapidly it made repeated, but not switched, responses. These results parallel the executive motor impairments seen in human patients, and therefore suggest that, like in human patients, stereotypy in caged parrots reflects a general disinhibition of the behavioral control mechanisms of the dorsal basal ganglia. If this result holds true in other laboratory species, stereotypic animals are likely to be of questionable utility in behavior, neuroscience, and neuropharmacological experiments. In humans, stereotypies and obsessive-compulsive behaviors are considered to be mutually exclusive categories of behavior, with different neural substrates, and different treatment strategies. These results, therefore, suggest that the pharmacological treatment of stereotypies in veterinary medicine based on the assumption that they are equivalent to human Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder may be inappropriate. As stereotypies in captive animals develop in response to the captive environment, these results also emphasize the role that the environment may play in eliciting or exacerbating stereotypy in human patients. Finally, by parallel to human patients, there is a potential psychological distress in animals showing these behaviors.
- Published
- 2003
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
40. Isosexual pair housing improves the welfare of young Amazon parrots
- Author
-
Joseph P. Garner, C.L Meehan, and Joy A. Mench
- Subjects
Aggression ,media_common.quotation_subject ,medicine.medical_treatment ,Animal-assisted therapy ,Feather-plucking ,Screaming ,Stereotypy (non-human) ,Animal science ,Food Animals ,Animal welfare ,medicine ,HUBzero ,Animal Science and Zoology ,medicine.symptom ,Psychology ,Welfare ,media_common ,Demography - Abstract
It has been suggested that isolation from conspecifics may contribute to the development of abnormal behaviors that are common in captive parrots, including stereotypy, feather plucking, excessive fearfulness and aggression (e.g. [Proceedings of the European Symposium on Bird Diseases, Beerse, Belgium (1987), p. 98; Kleintierpraxis 38 (1993) 511]). Thus, we assessed the influence of isosexual pair housing on the development of these behaviors, as well as the incidence of illness and injury, in young Orange-winged Amazon parrots (Amazona amazonica). Parrots (n=21) were parent raised to 6 months of age and then housed either singly or in isosexual pairs. All cages included inanimate enrichments that were changed regularly, and all parrots were handled regularly. Behavioral activity was recorded 0,3,6,9 and 12 months after the parrots were housed in the experimental cages, and responses (e.g. willingness to approach, tolerance to touch, flight distance) to familiar and strange human handlers and to novel objects introduced into the home cage were recorded periodically. Paired parrots used their enrichments more (GLM: F1,10=13.74; P=0.004), and spent less time screaming (F1,10=4.90; P=0.051), less time preening (F1,10=5.12; P=0.047), and less time inactive (F1,10=9.24; P
- Published
- 2003
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
41. Foraging opportunity and increased physical complexity both prevent and reduce psychogenic feather picking by young Amazon parrots
- Author
-
Joy A. Mench, C.L Meehan, and James R. Millam
- Subjects
Feather pecking ,Ecology ,Foraging ,Repeated measures design ,Feather-plucking ,Biology ,Animal science ,Food Animals ,Feather ,visual_art ,Animal Captivity ,visual_art.visual_art_medium ,Weaning ,Psychogenic disease ,Animal Science and Zoology - Abstract
Although many authors have suggested that the quality of the cage environment contributes to the development and performance of psychogenic feather picking by parrots, there is little scientific evidence for this relationship. In chickens, there is an established relationship between absence of foraging opportunity and the performance of a similar behavior, feather pecking. Thus, we assessed whether providing environmental enrichments designed to facilitate foraging behaviors would prevent or reduce the development of feather picking behavior by parrots, as evidenced by superior feather condition. Two groups of eight parrots were parent-reared to weaning and then housed singly in either enriched or unenriched cages for 48 weeks. In the enriched condition, a unique combination of one foraging and one physical enrichment was presented to each parrot weekly. In both groups, feather condition was quantified using a 10-point scale. The provision of enrichments led to an improvement in feather condition over 48 weeks in the enriched group, while feather scores in the control group decreased significantly (repeated measures GLM: F 1,46 =5.59; P =0.022) during this same period, indicating that feather picking behavior had developed in this group. In the second part of this study, the control group was transferred to the enriched treatment for a period of 16 weeks. During this period re-feathering occurred and feather scores improved significantly, indicating that feather picking behavior had decreased (repeated measures GLM: F 1,53 =35.57, P
- Published
- 2003
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
42. Environmental enrichment affects the fear and exploratory responses to novelty of young Amazon parrots
- Author
-
C.L Meehan and Joy A. Mench
- Subjects
Environmental enrichment ,Increased risk ,Food Animals ,Novel object ,Novelty ,Repeated measures design ,Animal Science and Zoology ,Psychology ,Developmental psychology - Abstract
The development of techniques to reduce fear responses of captive animals is important because fear is generally considered an undesirable emotional state that is related to increased risk of injury and decreased biological functioning. We tested the effects of environmental enrichments designed to increase the physical complexity of the cage and to provide opportunities for foraging behaviors on responses to novelty of young Orange-winged Amazon parrots (Amazona amazonica). Parrots (n ¼ 16) were housed in either barren or enriched conditions for 1 year and responses to novel objects and human handlers were tested periodically. Parrots in the enriched condition had significantly shorter latencies to approach novel objects placed in their home cages than parrots from the control group (repeated measures GLM: F1;13 ¼ 8:00; P ¼ 0:014). In addition, parrots from the enriched condition had shorter bouts of interaction (F1;14 ¼ 27:93; P < 0:0005) and spent significantly less time interacting with novel objects overall (F1;14 ¼ 27:93; P < 0:0005). Taken together, these results suggest that enrichment reduced both the fear response to novel objects and the motivation to explore and interact with those objects. When tested with a familiar handler, the control parrots had significantly higher response scores (i.e. they were less aggressive and more interactive) than the parrots from the enriched group (t-test; P < 0:005). This suggests that parrots housed in barren conditions may be more motivated to interact with familiar humans as a source of environmental stimulation. All parrots showed higher response scores to familiar than unfamiliar handlers, but in the control group there was a significant drop in score from the familiar to the unfamiliar handler (t-test; P < 0:0005) while in the enriched group the scores with familiar and unfamiliar handlers were similar. Thus, environmental enrichment reduced fear responses to both novel objects and unfamiliar human handlers. Factor analysis revealed that the responses to the novel object and novel human tests were independent, which implies that they did not measure the same underlying factor. Similarly, the
- Published
- 2002
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
43. Reliability and validity of a modified gait scoring system and its use in assessing tibial dyschondroplasia in broilers
- Author
-
C. Falcone, Joseph P. Garner, Michael P. Martin, Joy A. Mench, and Patricia S. Wakenell
- Subjects
Male ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Validation study ,Scoring system ,Lameness, Animal ,Osteochondrodysplasias ,Severity of Illness Index ,Physical medicine and rehabilitation ,Animals ,Medicine ,Gait ,Poultry Diseases ,Observer Variation ,Tibia ,Tibial dyschondroplasia ,business.industry ,Reproducibility of Results ,Videotape Recording ,General Medicine ,Gait impairment ,Lameness ,Animal Science and Zoology ,business ,Observer variation ,Chickens ,Food Science - Abstract
1. The gait scoring system for broilers developed by Kestin et al. (Veterinary Record, 131: 190-194, 1992) has been widely used to evaluate leg problems. The many factors and measures associated with this scale have empirically established its external (biological) validity. However, published test-retest (within-observer) reliabilities are poor, and inter-observer reliabilities are unknown. We evaluated several modifications to this scale aimed at improving its objectivity and reliability. 2. Eighteen naïve observers scored a standardised video of birds exhibiting varying degrees of lameness, either using Kestin et al.'s system, or our modified system. 3. Test-retest reliability (0.906) for Kestin et al.'s system was higher than previously reported. Inter-rater reliability was also good (0.892). The modified system offered significantly better test-retest (0.948) and inter-rater reliabilities (0.943), without incurring costs in terms of time taken or difficulty of use. The systems were consistent, assigning individual birds the same score on average. 4. It is concluded that the modified system offers the advantages of reduced error within and between studies. 5. In a second experiment, we used our modified scoring system to examine the relationship between tibial dyschondroplasia (TD) and gait score in 267 selected broilers. 6. Neither the presence nor severity of TD affected gait score, suggesting that, at least in this strain of broilers, other leg problems like slipped tendons or torsional deformities had more influence on gait impairment than did TD.
- Published
- 2002
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
44. Broiler breeders: feed restriction and welfare
- Author
-
Joy A. Mench
- Subjects
Alternative methods ,animal diseases ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Broiler ,food and beverages ,Broiler breeder ,Body weight ,Agricultural science ,Genetic selection ,Animal Science and Zoology ,Business ,Welfare ,Physiological stress ,Management practices ,media_common - Abstract
Broiler breeders are truly caught in a welfare dilemma, because the management practices that are necessary to ensure health and reproductive competence may also result in a reduction in other aspects of welfare. In this paper I review the welfare concerns associated with feed restriction of broiler breeders. Broiler breeders show evidence of physiological stress as well as an increased incidence of abnormal behaviours, and are also chronically hungry. Alternative methods of decreasing feed intake, dietary dilution or the use of anorexic agents, are also discussed. Thus far, all of these methods have proven unsatisfactory in terms of body weight control, and in addition they do not necessarily improve welfare. Emphasis needs to continue to be placed on developing alternatives to, and modifications of, current feed restriction programmes in order to improve broiler breeder welfare. In addition, the use of genetic selection to decrease the need for restriction should be further explored.
- Published
- 2002
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
45. Determination of space use by laying hens using kinematic analysis
- Author
-
Joy A. Mench and R. A. Blatchford
- Subjects
Wing ,Reproduction ,Space use ,Videotape Recording ,General Medicine ,Kinematics ,TOPS ,Motor Activity ,Animal Welfare ,Laying ,Housing, Animal ,Biomechanical Phenomena ,Animal science ,Space requirements ,Flapping ,Animals ,Animal Science and Zoology ,Female ,Animal Husbandry ,Cage ,Chickens ,Mathematics - Abstract
Two states in the United States now have legislation requiring that laying hens be provided with sufficient space to perform particular behaviors. To provide a framework for translating these performance standards into a space requirement, kinematic analysis was used to measure the amount of space needed for White Leghorn hens to stand, turn around 180°, lie down, and wing flap. Hyline W-36 hens (n = 9) were marked on the tops of their heads and the tips of both wings and 3 toes with black livestock marker. Each hen was then placed in a floor pen (91.4 × 91.4 cm) and filmed using 2 high-speed cameras. The resulting images were processed using a software program that generated 3-dimensional space use for each behavior. Because none of the hens lay down in the test pen, the 2-dimensional space required for lying was determined by superimposing a grid over videos of the hens lying down in their home cages. On average, hens required a mean area of 563 (± 8) cm2 to stand, 1,316 (± 23) cm2 to turn around, 318 (± 6) cm2 to lie down, and 1,693 (± 136) cm2 to wing flap. The mean heights used were 34.8 (± 1.3) cm for standing, 38.6 (± 2.3) cm for turning, and 49.5 (± 1.8) cm for wing flapping. However, space requirements for hens housed in multiple-hen groups in cage or noncage systems cannot be based simply on information about the space required for local movement by a single hen. It must also incorporate consideration of the tendency of hens in a flock to synchronize their behaviors. In addition, it must include not just local movement space but also the space that hens may need to use for longer-distance movements to access resources such as food, water, perches, and nest boxes.
- Published
- 2014
46. The Coalition for Sustainable Egg Supply project: An introduction
- Author
-
Darrin M. Karcher, J. C. Swanson, and Joy A. Mench
- Subjects
business.industry ,Environmental resource management ,General Medicine ,Animal husbandry ,Food safety ,Occupational safety and health ,coalition ,egg supply ,Retail food ,Animal welfare ,Sustainability ,housing system ,Production (economics) ,Coalition for Sustainable Egg Supply ,Animal Science and Zoology ,sustainable ,Marketing ,business ,Social responsibility - Abstract
In the United States, empirical information on the sustainability of commercial-scale egg production is lacking. The passage of state regulations specific to hen housing created urgency to better understand the effects of different housing systems on the sustainability of the egg supply, and stimulated the formation of a coalition, the Coalition for a Sustainable Egg Supply (CSES), to conduct research on this topic. The CSES is a multi-stakeholder group with 27 members, including food manufacturers, research institutions, scientists, restaurants, food service, retail food companies, egg suppliers, and nongovernmental organizations. A commercial-scale study was developed to better understand the effect of 3 housing systems (conventional cage, enriched colony, and cage-free aviary) on 5 areas related to a sustainable egg supply. These 5 sustainability areas represent effects on people, animals, and the environment: animal health and well-being, environment, food safety, worker health and safety, and food affordability. Five teams of scientists, each associated with a sustainability area, conducted an integrated field study at a commercial site in the upper Midwest through 2 flock cycles in 3 housing systems. This paper provides a brief overview of the CSES project to serve as an introduction for the papers that follow in this volume of Poultry Science.
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
47. Effect of Tryptophan Treatment on Self-Biting and Central Nervous System Serotonin Metabolism in Rhesus Monkeys (Macaca mulatta)
- Author
-
Katherine P Weld, Joy A Mench, Ruth A Woodward, Monica S Bolesta, Stephen J Suomi, and J Dee Higley
- Subjects
Pharmacology ,Psychiatry and Mental health - Published
- 1998
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
48. Shackling of broilers: Effects on stress responses and breast meat quality
- Author
-
Joy A. Mench, C J Wabeck, G Kannan, and J. L. Heath
- Subjects
Male ,Restraint, Physical ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Meat ,Radioimmunoassay ,Biology ,Handling, Psychological ,Animal science ,Reference Values ,Internal medicine ,medicine ,Animals ,Muscle, Skeletal ,Stunning ,Fear ,General Medicine ,Crate ,Endocrinology ,Reference values ,Animal Science and Zoology ,Plasma corticosterone ,Negative correlation ,Corticosterone ,Chickens ,Abattoirs ,Stress, Psychological ,Food Science - Abstract
1. Experiments were conducted to study the welfare and meat quality effects of shackling. In experiment 1, broilers with or without leg problems were shackled (S) for 4 min on a moving line and blood sampled; or handled (H), returned to the crate and sampled after 4 min; or sampled immediately after removal from the crate (control, C). 2. Plasma corticosterone (CORT) concentrations, as measured by radioimmunoassay, were highest in S and lowest in C, while the H group was intermediate. Leg problems had no effect on CORT. 3. In experiment 2, tonic immobility (TI) was induced in broilers after 2 min inverted handling to determine fear responses. One week later, the birds were fasted, transported and then shackled on a moving shackle line for 0, 1, 3 or 4 min, then unshackled and blood sampled. Wing flapping during shackling was also quantified. 4. Shackling time did not influence CORT concentrations. There was a negative correlation (r = -0.714) between CORT and wing flapping duration in the 1 min shackling treatment. There was no relationship between TI and wing flapping or CORT. 5. In experiment 3, broilers were exposed to two food withdrawal (FW) times (food withdrawn overnight or during crating only) and held for 4 h prior to processing, shackled (0, 2 or 4 min shackling time, ST), and then killed by exsanguination. Blood samples were collected during the neck-cut. Pectoralis superficialis and Supracoracoideus samples were either collected after 15 min and individually quick frozen (IQF) in liquid nitrogen or collected at 4 h post mortem from carcases chilled on slush ice (COI). 6. CORT increased significantly with increased ST. There was a FW x ST interaction effect on the initial pH of fillets. ST influenced the b*, chroma and Hue values of the COI fillets. FW influenced the L* and Hue values of both IQF and COI fillets as well as the a* value of the COI fillets. 7. In summary, CORT increased with shackling time when birds were held after transport. FW and ST also influenced the colour of fillets, although it is not clear whether these changes are perceptible to the consumer. The duration of wing flapping during shackling did not appear to be related to fearfulness, although it was influenced by properties of the shackle line. We suggest that there be a maximum time lapse between shackling and stunning or killing of 2 min to minimise stress and meat quality changes.
- Published
- 1997
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
49. The effects of light stimulation during incubation on indicators of stress susceptibility in broilers
- Author
-
Gregory S. Archer and Joy A. Mench
- Subjects
Photoperiod ,LIGHT STIMULATION ,Stimulation ,Enzyme-Linked Immunosorbent Assay ,General Medicine ,Chick Embryo ,Biology ,Immunity, Humoral ,chemistry.chemical_compound ,Light level ,Incubators ,Animal science ,Crowding ,chemistry ,Adrenocorticotropic Hormone ,Corticosterone ,Stress, Physiological ,Hemocyanins ,Animals ,Animal Science and Zoology ,Stress measures ,Incubation ,Chickens - Abstract
Providing light during incubation has been shown to decrease bilateral physical asymmetry of broilers posthatch, which may indicate that early light stimulation reduces later stress susceptibility. This experiment evaluated the effects of lighting during embryogenesis on other measures of stress responsiveness in broilers. Cobb 500 eggs (n = 1,404) were incubated under 0L:24D, 1L:23D, 6L:18D, or 12L:12D; the light level was 550 lx. The broilers were then raised in floor pens under a 12L:12D lighting regimen, and various stress parameters were measured during wk 3 to 6 of age. There was an effect of incubation lighting regimen on posthatch stress responses. Following 1 h of crating, the change in corticosterone (CORT) concentration was less in the 12L:12D (-0.06 ng/mL, P0.05) treatment than all other treatments (pooled mean = 0.24 ng/mL); however, there were no treatment differences in CORT response to adrenocorticotropic hormone administration (pooled mean pre- vs. 1 h postadministration = 17.5 ng/mL, P0.05). Anti-keyhole limpet hemocyanin titers were higher in the 12L:12D birds (92,395 units/mL; P0.05) than all other treatments (pooled mean = 68,407 units/mL) on d 1 postcrating. Additionally, composite asymmetry scores were lower in the 12L:12D treatment (0.92 mm) than all other treatments (pooled mean = 1.14 mm, P0.05). These results demonstrate that providing 12 h of light per day during incubation can reduce the stress susceptibility of broilers posthatch.
- Published
- 2013
50. Evaluation of taste repellents with northern bobwhites for deterring ingestion of granular pesticides
- Author
-
F. Nicholas Mastrota and Joy A. Mench
- Subjects
biology ,Methyl anthranilate ,Health, Toxicology and Mutagenesis ,Colinus ,Factorial experiment ,Pesticide ,biology.organism_classification ,Erythrosine ,Toxicology ,chemistry.chemical_compound ,chemistry ,Preference test ,Environmental Chemistry ,Ingestion ,Sucrose octaacetate - Abstract
The risk of accidental poisoning of birds may be reduced by adding an aversive-tasting chemical to granular pesticides. The efficacy of eight taste repellents in deterring consumption was therefore evaluated in a game species, the northern bobwhite (Colinus virginianus). Avoidance was measured using a two-cup preference test in which treated feeds were paired with untreated feeds in one experiment, and against other treated feeds in another experiment. All repellents tested were significantly avoided without evidence of habituation during the 5-d test period. The most effective repellents were d-pulegone and quinine hydrochloride. Methyl anthranilate, 2-heptanone, and lithium chloride also performed reasonably well, whereas sucrose octaacetate and cinnamaldehyde performed relatively poorly. The d-pulegone was further evaluated using a one-cup test with treated clay granules. The effects of adding d-pulegone and/or a red dye (erythrosine) were evaluated with a two-way factorial design. Addition of d-pulegone strongly reduced consumption of granules (p
- Published
- 1995
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.