159 results on '"Paracchini S"'
Search Results
2. Hypothesis-driven genome-wide association studies provide novel insights into genetics of reading disabilities
- Author
-
Price, K.M., Wigg, K.G., Eising, E., Feng, Y, Blokland, K., Wilkinson, M., Kerr, E.N., Guger, S.L., Abbondanza, F., Allegrini, A.G., Andlauer, T.F.M., Bates, T.C., Bernard, M., Bonte, M., Boomsma, D.I., Bourgeron, T., Brandeis, D., Carreiras, M., Ceroni, F., Csépe, V., Dale, P.S., DeFries, J.C., Jong, P.F. de, Démonet, J.F., Zeeuw, E.L. de, Franken, M.-C.J., Francks, C., Gerritse, M.L., Gialluisi, A., Gordon, S.D., Gruen, J.R., Hayiou-Thomas, M.E., Hernández-Cabrera, J., Hottenga, J.-J., Hulme, C., Jansen, P.R., Kere, J., Koomar, T., Landerl, K., Leonard, G.T., Liao, Z., Luciano, M., Lyytinen, H., Martin, N.G., Martinelli, A., Maurer, U., Michaelson, J.J., Mirza-Schreiber, N., Moll, K., Monaco, A.P., Morgan, A.T., Müller-Myhsok, B., Newbury, D.F., Nöthen, M.M., Olson, R.K., Paracchini, S., Paus, T., Pausova, Z., Pennell, C.E., Pennington, B.F., Plomin, R.J., Ramus, F., Reilly, S., Richer, L., Rimfeld, K., Schulte-Körne, G., Shapland, C.Y., Simpson, N.H., Smith, S.D., Snowling, M.J., St Pourcain, B., Stein, J.F., Talcott, J.B., Tiemeier, H., Tomblin, J.B., Truong, D.T., Bergen, E. van, Schroeff, M.P. van der, Donkelaar, M.M.J. van, Verhoef, E., Wang, C.A., Watkins, K.E., Whitehouse, A.J.O., Willcutt, E.G., Wright, M.J., Zhu, G., Fisher, S.E., Lovett, M.W., Strug, L.J., Barr, C.L., University of St Andrews. School of Medicine, University of St Andrews. Centre for Biophotonics, University of St Andrews. Biomedical Sciences Research Complex, University of St Andrews. Institute of Behavioural and Neural Sciences, University of St Andrews. St Andrews Bioinformatics Unit, University of St Andrews. Cellular Medicine Division, STEMM - Stem Cells and Metabolism Research Program, Juha Kere / Principal Investigator, Research Programs Unit, University of Helsinki, Consortium, Quantitative Trait Working Group of the GenLang, European Commission, Otorhinolaryngology and Head and Neck Surgery, Child and Adolescent Psychiatry / Psychology, RS: FPN CN 7, Language, Biological Psychology, Amsterdam Reproduction & Development, APH - Mental Health, APH - Methodology, APH - Health Behaviors & Chronic Diseases, APH - Personalized Medicine, Complex Trait Genetics, Amsterdam Neuroscience - Complex Trait Genetics, LEARN! - Educational neuroscience, learning and development, and Human genetics
- Subjects
Neuroinformatics ,single nucleotide ,Candidate gene ,Autism Spectrum Disorder ,Developmental dyslexia ,autism spectrum disorder ,QH426 Genetics ,Polymorphism, Single Nucleotide ,Neuronal migration ,3124 Neurology and psychiatry ,polymorphism ,Dyslexia ,Cellular and Molecular Neuroscience ,All institutes and research themes of the Radboud University Medical Center ,problem solving ,SDG 3 - Good Health and Well-being ,dyslexia ,Humans ,Kiaa0319 ,Family ,humans ,Children ,QH426 ,Problem Solving ,Biological Psychiatry ,MCC ,Neurodevelopmental disorders Donders Center for Medical Neuroscience [Radboudumc 7] ,genome-wide association study ,Dyx1c1 ,Plasma-membrane ,3rd-DAS ,Psychiatry and Mental health ,Susceptibility ,RC0321 ,SDG 4 - Quality Education ,RC0321 Neuroscience. Biological psychiatry. Neuropsychiatry ,Genome-Wide Association Study ,Knockout mice - Abstract
Reading Disability (RD) is often characterized by difficulties in the phonology of the language. While the molecular mechanisms underlying it are largely undetermined, loci are being revealed by genome-wide association studies (GWAS). In a previous GWAS for word reading (Price, 2020), we observed that top single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) were located near to or in genes involved in neuronal migration/axon guidance (NM/AG) or loci implicated in autism spectrum disorder (ASD). A prominent theory of RD etiology posits that it involves disturbed neuronal migration, while potential links between RD-ASD have not been extensively investigated. To improve power to identify associated loci, we up-weighted variants involved in NM/AG or ASD, separately, and performed a new Hypothesis-Driven (HD)–GWAS. The approach was applied to a Toronto RD sample and a meta-analysis of the GenLang Consortium. For the Toronto sample (n = 624), no SNPs reached significance; however, by gene-set analysis, the joint contribution of ASD-related genes passed the threshold (p~1.45 × 10–2, threshold = 2.5 × 10–2). For the GenLang Cohort (n = 26,558), SNPs in DOCK7 and CDH4 showed significant association for the NM/AG hypothesis (sFDR q = 1.02 × 10–2). To make the GenLang dataset more similar to Toronto, we repeated the analysis restricting to samples selected for reading/language deficits (n = 4152). In this GenLang selected subset, we found significant association for a locus intergenic between BTG3-C21orf91 for both hypotheses (sFDR q < 9.00 × 10–4). This study contributes candidate loci to the genetics of word reading. Data also suggest that, although different variants may be involved, alleles implicated in ASD risk may be found in the same genes as those implicated in word reading. This finding is limited to the Toronto sample suggesting that ascertainment influences genetic associations., Translational Psychiatry, 12 (1), ISSN:2158-3188
- Published
- 2022
3. Genome-wide analyses of individual differences in quantitatively assessed reading- and language-related skills in up to 34,000 people
- Author
-
Eising, E., Mirza-Schreiber, N., Zeeuw, E.L. de, Wang, C.A., Truong, D.T., Allegrini, A.G., Shapland, C.Y., Zhu, G., Wigg, K.G., Gerritse, M.L., Molz, B., Alagöz, G., Gialluisi, A., Abbondanza, F., Rimfeld, K., Donkelaar, M.M.J. van, Liao, Z., Jansen, P.R., Andlauer, T.F.M., Bates, T.C., Bernard, M., Blokland, K., Bonte, M., Børglum, A.D., Bourgeron, T., Brandeis, D., Ceroni, F., Csépe, V., Dale, P.S., Jong, P.F. de, DeFries, J.C., Démonet, J.F., Demontis, D., Feng, Yu, Gordon, S.D.S., Guger, S.L., Hayiou-Thomas, M.E., Hernández-Cabrera, J.A., Hottenga, J.J., Hulme, C., Kere, J., Kerr, E.N., Koomar, T., Landerl, K., Leonard, G.T., Lovett, M.W., Lyytinen, H., Martin, N.G., Martinelli, A., Maurer, U., Michaelson, J.J., Moll, K., Monaco, A.P., Morgan, A.T., Nöthen, M.M., Pausova, Z., Pennell, C.E., Pennington, B.F., Price, K.M., Rajagopal, V.M., Ramus, F., Richer, L., Simpson, N.H., Smith, S.D., Snowling, M.J., Stein, J., Strug, L.J., Talcott, J.B., Tiemeier, H., Schroeff, M.P. van der, Verhoef, E., Watkins, K.E., Wilkinson, M., Wright, M.J., Barr, C.L., Boomsma, D.I., Carreiras, M., Franken, M.J., Gruen, J.R., Luciano, M., Müller-Myhsok, B., Newbury, D.F., Olson, R.K., Paracchini, S., Paus, T., Plomin, R., Reilly, S., Schulte-Körne, G., Tomblin, J.B., Bergen, E. van, Whitehouse, A.J.O., Willcutt, E.G., Pourcain, B. St, Francks, C., Fisher, S.E., St Pourcain, B., Génétique humaine et fonctions cognitives - Human Genetics and Cognitive Functions (GHFC (UMR_3571 / U-Pasteur_1)), Institut Pasteur [Paris] (IP)-Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS)-Université Paris Cité (UPCité), Laboratoire de sciences cognitives et psycholinguistique (LSCP), Département d'Etudes Cognitives - ENS Paris (DEC), École normale supérieure - Paris (ENS-PSL), Université Paris sciences et lettres (PSL)-Université Paris sciences et lettres (PSL)-École normale supérieure - Paris (ENS-PSL), Université Paris sciences et lettres (PSL)-Université Paris sciences et lettres (PSL)-École des hautes études en sciences sociales (EHESS)-Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CNRS), Human genetics, APH - Aging & Later Life, APH - Mental Health, Biological Psychology, Complex Trait Genetics, Amsterdam Neuroscience - Complex Trait Genetics, APH - Health Behaviors & Chronic Diseases, APH - Personalized Medicine, Amsterdam Reproduction & Development, APH - Methodology, LEARN! - Educational neuroscience, learning and development, Child and Adolescent Psychiatry / Psychology, Otorhinolaryngology and Head and Neck Surgery, STEMM - Stem Cells and Metabolism Research Program, Juha Kere / Principal Investigator, Research Programs Unit, University of Helsinki, Language, RS: FPN CN 7, The Royal Society, University of St Andrews. Cellular Medicine Division, University of St Andrews. School of Medicine, University of St Andrews. Centre for Biophotonics, University of St Andrews. Biomedical Sciences Research Complex, University of St Andrews. Institute of Behavioural and Neural Sciences, and University of St Andrews. St Andrews Bioinformatics Unit
- Subjects
Neuroinformatics ,Adult ,kieli ja kielet ,Adolescent ,Individuality ,QH426 Genetics ,Polymorphism, Single Nucleotide ,lukeminen ,Language in Interaction ,Young Adult ,SDG 3 - Good Health and Well-being ,RA0421 ,reading ,RA0421 Public health. Hygiene. Preventive Medicine ,Humans ,Speech ,study ,Polymorphism ,Reading j ,Preschool ,Child ,QH426 ,perinnöllisyys ,Genome-wide Association Study ,Language ,Meta-analysis ,Reading ,MCC ,Neurodevelopmental disorders Donders Center for Medical Neuroscience [Radboudumc 7] ,genome-wide association study ,language ,Multidisciplinary ,meta-analyysi ,1184 Genetics, developmental biology, physiology ,kielitaito ,[SDV.NEU.SC]Life Sciences [q-bio]/Neurons and Cognition [q-bio.NC]/Cognitive Sciences ,DAS ,Single Nucleotide ,meta-analysis ,Genetic Loci ,Child, Preschool ,Genome-Wide Association Study ,perimä ,lukutaito ,genome-wide association ,SDG 4 - Quality Education - Abstract
Published August 23, 2022 The use of spoken and written language is a fundamental human capacity. Individual differences in reading- and language-related skills are influenced by genetic variation, with twin-based heritability estimates of 30 to 80% depending on the trait. The genetic architecture is complex, heterogeneous, and multifactorial, but investigations of contributions of single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) were thus far underpowered. We present a multicohort genome-wide association study (GWAS) of five traits assessed individually using psychometric measures (word reading, nonword reading, spelling, phoneme awareness, and nonword repetition) in samples of 13,633 to 33,959 participants aged 5 to 26 y. We identified genome-wide significant association with word reading (rs11208009, P = 1.098 × 1028) at a locus that has not been associated with intelligence or educational attainment. All five reading-/language-related traits showed robust SNP heritability, accounting for 13 to 26% of trait variability. Genomic structural equation modeling revealed a shared genetic factor explaining most of the variation in word/nonword reading, spelling, and phoneme awareness, which only partially overlapped with genetic variation contributing to nonword repetition, intelligence, and educational attainment. A multivariate GWAS of word/nonword reading, spelling, and phoneme awareness maximized power for follow-up investigation. Genetic correlation analysis with neuroimaging traits identified an association with the surface area of the banks of the left superior temporal sulcus, a brain region linked to the processing of spoken and written language. Heritability was enriched for genomic elements regulating gene expression in the fetal brain and in chromosomal regions that are depleted of Neanderthal variants. Together, these results provide avenues for deciphering the biological underpinnings of uniquely human traits. We thank all the children, twins, families, and participants who took part and are taking part in the 22 cohorts whose data contributed to these GWAS meta-analyses; the staff working on the different cohorts, including volunteers, study coordinators, interviewers, teachers, nurses, research scientists, general practitioners, midwives, psychologists, psychometrists, computer and laboratory technicians, and colleagues who assisted in the quality control and preparation of the imputed GWAS data; and the pharmacies and hospitals that were involved. B.M., B.M.-M., B.S.P., C.F., E.E., E.V., G.A., M.v.D., and S.E.F. are supported by the Max Planck Society. A.G. and T.F.M.A. were supported by the Munich Cluster for Systems Neurology (SyNergy), and A.G. was supported by Fondazione Umberto Veronesi. A.T.M. is supported by National Health and Medical Research Council of Australia (NHMRC) Grants 1105008 and 1195955 and Centre of Research Excellence Grant 1116976. A.J.O.W. is supported by NHMRC Grant 1173896. B.S.P. is supported by Simons Foundation Autism Research Initiative Grant 514787. C.Y.S. works in the Medical Research Council Integrative Epidemiology Unit at the University of Bristol (MC_UU_00011/3). D.I.B. acknowledges Royal Netherlands Academy of Science Professor Award PAH/6635. E.E. is supported by NIH Grant R01DC016977. E.G.W. and J.R.G. are supported by National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD) Grant P50 HD 27802. F.R. is supported by Agence Nationale de la Recherche Grants ANR-06-NEURO-019-01, ANR-17-EURE-0017 IEC, ANR-10-IDEX-0001-02 PSL, and ANR-11-BSV4-014-01 and European Commission Grant LSHM-CT-2005-018696. H.T. is supported by the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research (NWO) and Netherlands Organisation for Health Research and Development (ZonMW) Grant VICI 016.VICI.170.200. J.C.D. was supported by NICHD Grant P50 HD 27802. J.J.M., J.B.To., and T.K. were supported by NIH Grant R01 DC014489. K.M.P. was supported by the Hospital for Sick Children Research Training Program (Restracomp). K.R. is supported by a Sir Henry Wellcome Postdoctoral Fellowship (213514/Z/18/Z). M.J.S. is supported by Wellcome Trust Grant WT082032MA. S.P. and F.A. are supported by Royal Society Grants UF150663 and RGF\EA\180141. T.B. is supported by Institut Pasteur, the Bettencourt-Schueller Foundation, and Université de Paris. The Adolescent Brain Cognitive Development Study is supported by the NIH and additional federal partners (NIH Grants U01DA041048, U01DA050989, U01DA051016, U01DA041022, U01DA051018, U01DA051037, U01DA050987, U01DA041174, U01DA041106, U01DA041117, U01DA041028, U01DA041134, U01DA050988, U01DA051039, U01DA041156, U01DA041025, U01DA041120, U01DA051038, U01DA041148, U01DA041093, U01DA041089, U24DA041123, and U24DA041147). The Aston Cohort was supported by funding from European Union (EU) Horizon 2020 Programme 641652 and Waterloo Foundation Grant 797/17290. The St. Andrews Bioinformatics Unit is funded by Wellcome Trust Grants 105621/Z/14/Z and 204821/Z/16/Z. ALSPAC is supported by UK Medical Research Council and Wellcome Grant 217065/Z/19/Z and the University of Bristol. A comprehensive list of grant funding is available on the ALSPAC website (http://www.bristol.ac.uk/alspac/external/documents/grant-acknowledgements.pdf). The Basque Center on Cognition, Brain and Language (BCBL) cohort was supported by the Basque Government through the Basic Excellence Research Centre program and the Agencia Estatal de Investigación through BCBL Severo Ochoa excellence accreditation. The Brisbane Adolescent Twin Sample was supported by Australian Research Council Grants A7960034, A79906588, A79801419, DP0212016, and DP0343921, with genotyping funded by the NHMRC Grant 389891. The Colorado Learning Disabilities Research Center cohort was supported by NICHD Grant P50 HD 27802. The Early Language in Victoria Study was supported by NHMRC Grant 436958. The Familial Influences on Literacy Abilities cohort is supported by the University of Amsterdam, the Max Planck Institue Nijmegen, and NWO Grants Rubicon 446-12-005 and VENI 451-15-017. The GRaD study was funded by the Manton Foundation, NIH Grants P50-HD027802 and K99-HD094902, and the Lambert Family. NeuroDys was funded by an EU Sixth Framework Program grant to the NeuroDys Consortium, Swiss National Science Foundation Grant 32-108130, and Austrian Science Fund Grant 18351-B02. The Netherlands Twin Register is funded by NWO Grants 480-04-004, 481-08-011, 056-32-010, 024.001.003, 480-15-001/674, 184.021.007, 184.033.111, and 56-464-14192; ZonMW Grants 911-09-032 and 912-10-020; the Amsterdam Public Health and Amsterdam Reproduction and Development Research Institutes; European Science Council Grant ERC Advanced 230374; EU Seventh Framework Program (FP7) Grant FP7/2007-2013: 602768; National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) Grants U24 MH068457-06, R01 MH58799-03, and 1RC2 MH089995; and the Avera Institute for Human Genetics. The Pediatric Imaging, Neurocognition, and Genetics cohort is funded by NIH Grant RC2DA029475, the National Institute on Drug Abuse, and the Eunice Kennedy Shriver NICHD. The Philadelphia Neurodevelopmental Cohort is funded by NIH Grants RC2MH089983 and RC2MH089924, an institutional development award to the Center for Applied Genomics from The Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia, and a donation from Adele and Daniel Kubert and thanks the NIH data repository. The Raine study was supported by long-term funding from NHMRC Grants 572613, 403981, and 1059711 and Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR) Grant MOP-82893. Funding was also provided by the University of Western Australia, Curtin University, the Women and Infants Research Foundation, the Telethon Kids Institute, Edith Cowan University, Murdoch University, the University of Notre Dame Australia, and the Raine Medical Research Foundation. The Raine study analyses were supported by the Pawsey Supercomputing Centre with funding from the Australian Government and the Government of Western Australia. The Saguenay Youth Study is supported by the CIHR, the Heart and Stroke Foundation of Quebec, and the Canadian Foundation for Innovation. The SLI Consortium was funded by Wellcome Trust Grant 076566 and UK Medical Research Council Grant G1000569. The Twins Early Development Study is supported by UK Medical Research Council Grants MR/V012878/1 and MR/M021475/1, NIH Grant AG046938, and the EU FP7 grant FP7/2007-2013/: 602768. Toronto was supported by CIHR Grant MOP-133440. UK Dyslexia was supported by Wellcome Trust Grants 076566/Z/05/Z and 075491/Z/04, Waterloo Foundation Grant 797–1720, EU Grant 018696, and Royal Society Grant UF100463. The York cohort was funded by Wellcome Trust Grant 082036/B/07/Z. We acknowledge iPSYCH for sharing their summary statistics. The iPSYCH team was supported by Lundbeck Foundation Grants R102-A9118, R155-2014-1724, and R248-2017-2003; NIMH Grant 1U01MH109514-01; and the Universities and University Hospitals of Aarhus and Copenhagen. The Danish National Biobank resource was supported by the Novo Nordisk Foundation. High-performance computer capacity was provided by the Center for Genomics and Personalized Medicine and the Centre for Integrative Sequencing, Aarhus University, Denmark.
- Published
- 2022
4. Discovery of 42 genome-wide significant loci associated with dyslexia
- Author
-
Doust, C., Fontanillas, P., Eising, E., Gordon, S.D., Wang, Z, Alagoz, G., Molz, B., St Pourcain, B., Francks, C., Marioni, R.E., Zhao, J, Paracchini, S., Talcott, J.B., Monaco, A.P., Stein, J.F., Gruen, J.R., Olson, R.K., Willcutt, E.G., DeFries, J.C., Pennington, B.F., Smith, S.D., Wright, M.J., Martin, N.G., Auton, A., Bates, T.C., Fisher, S.E., Luciano, M., Doust, C., Fontanillas, P., Eising, E., Gordon, S.D., Wang, Z, Alagoz, G., Molz, B., St Pourcain, B., Francks, C., Marioni, R.E., Zhao, J, Paracchini, S., Talcott, J.B., Monaco, A.P., Stein, J.F., Gruen, J.R., Olson, R.K., Willcutt, E.G., DeFries, J.C., Pennington, B.F., Smith, S.D., Wright, M.J., Martin, N.G., Auton, A., Bates, T.C., Fisher, S.E., and Luciano, M.
- Abstract
Contains fulltext : 284950.pdf (Publisher’s version ) (Open Access)
- Published
- 2022
5. Genome-wide analyses of individual differences in quantitatively assessed reading- and language-related skills in up to 34,000 people
- Author
-
Eising, E, Mirza-Schreiber, N, de Zeeuw, EL, Wang, CA, Truong, DT, Allegrini, AG, Shapland, CY, Zhu, G, Wigg, KG, Gerritse, ML, Molz, B, Alagoz, G, Gialluisi, A, Abbondanza, F, Rimfeld, K, van Donkelaar, M, Liao, Z, Jansen, PR, Andlauer, TFM, Bates, TC, Bernard, M, Blokland, K, Bonte, M, Borglum, AD, Bourgeron, T, Brandeis, D, Ceronihh, F, Csepe, V, Dale, PS, de Jong, PF, DeFries, JC, Demonet, J-F, Demontis, D, Feng, Y, Gordon, SD, Guger, SL, Hayiou-Thomas, ME, Hernandez-Cabrera, JA, Hottenga, J-J, Hulme, C, Kere, J, Kerr, EN, Koomar, T, Landerl, K, Leonard, GT, Lovett, MW, Lyytinen, H, Martin, NG, Martinelli, A, Maurer, U, Michaelson, JJ, Moll, K, Monaco, AP, Morgan, AT, Nothen, MM, Pausova, Z, Pennell, CE, Pennington, BF, Price, KM, Rajagopal, VM, Ramus, F, Richer, L, Simpson, NH, Smith, SD, Snowling, MJ, Stein, J, Struguuu, LJ, Talcott, JB, Tiemeier, H, van der Schroeff, MP, Verhoef, E, Watkins, KE, Wilkinson, M, Wright, MJ, Barr, CL, Boomsma, D, Carreiras, M, Franken, M-CJ, Gruen, JR, Luciano, M, Muller-Myhsok, B, Newbury, DF, Olson, RK, Paracchini, S, Paus, T, Plomin, R, Reilly, S, Schulte-Korn, G, Tomblin, JB, Bergen, E, Whitehouse, AJO, Willcutt, EG, St Pourcain, B, Francks, C, Fisher, SE, Eising, E, Mirza-Schreiber, N, de Zeeuw, EL, Wang, CA, Truong, DT, Allegrini, AG, Shapland, CY, Zhu, G, Wigg, KG, Gerritse, ML, Molz, B, Alagoz, G, Gialluisi, A, Abbondanza, F, Rimfeld, K, van Donkelaar, M, Liao, Z, Jansen, PR, Andlauer, TFM, Bates, TC, Bernard, M, Blokland, K, Bonte, M, Borglum, AD, Bourgeron, T, Brandeis, D, Ceronihh, F, Csepe, V, Dale, PS, de Jong, PF, DeFries, JC, Demonet, J-F, Demontis, D, Feng, Y, Gordon, SD, Guger, SL, Hayiou-Thomas, ME, Hernandez-Cabrera, JA, Hottenga, J-J, Hulme, C, Kere, J, Kerr, EN, Koomar, T, Landerl, K, Leonard, GT, Lovett, MW, Lyytinen, H, Martin, NG, Martinelli, A, Maurer, U, Michaelson, JJ, Moll, K, Monaco, AP, Morgan, AT, Nothen, MM, Pausova, Z, Pennell, CE, Pennington, BF, Price, KM, Rajagopal, VM, Ramus, F, Richer, L, Simpson, NH, Smith, SD, Snowling, MJ, Stein, J, Struguuu, LJ, Talcott, JB, Tiemeier, H, van der Schroeff, MP, Verhoef, E, Watkins, KE, Wilkinson, M, Wright, MJ, Barr, CL, Boomsma, D, Carreiras, M, Franken, M-CJ, Gruen, JR, Luciano, M, Muller-Myhsok, B, Newbury, DF, Olson, RK, Paracchini, S, Paus, T, Plomin, R, Reilly, S, Schulte-Korn, G, Tomblin, JB, Bergen, E, Whitehouse, AJO, Willcutt, EG, St Pourcain, B, Francks, C, and Fisher, SE
- Abstract
The use of spoken and written language is a fundamental human capacity. Individual differences in reading- and language-related skills are influenced by genetic variation, with twin-based heritability estimates of 30 to 80% depending on the trait. The genetic architecture is complex, heterogeneous, and multifactorial, but investigations of contributions of single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) were thus far underpowered. We present a multicohort genome-wide association study (GWAS) of five traits assessed individually using psychometric measures (word reading, nonword reading, spelling, phoneme awareness, and nonword repetition) in samples of 13,633 to 33,959 participants aged 5 to 26 y. We identified genome-wide significant association with word reading (rs11208009, P = 1.098 × 10-8) at a locus that has not been associated with intelligence or educational attainment. All five reading-/language-related traits showed robust SNP heritability, accounting for 13 to 26% of trait variability. Genomic structural equation modeling revealed a shared genetic factor explaining most of the variation in word/nonword reading, spelling, and phoneme awareness, which only partially overlapped with genetic variation contributing to nonword repetition, intelligence, and educational attainment. A multivariate GWAS of word/nonword reading, spelling, and phoneme awareness maximized power for follow-up investigation. Genetic correlation analysis with neuroimaging traits identified an association with the surface area of the banks of the left superior temporal sulcus, a brain region linked to the processing of spoken and written language. Heritability was enriched for genomic elements regulating gene expression in the fetal brain and in chromosomal regions that are depleted of Neanderthal variants. Together, these results provide avenues for deciphering the biological underpinnings of uniquely human traits.
- Published
- 2022
6. Hypothesis-driven genome-wide association studies provide novel insights into genetics of reading disabilities
- Author
-
Price, KM, Wigg, KG, Eising, E, Feng, Y, Blokland, K, Wilkinson, M, Kerr, EN, Guger, SL, Abbondanza, F, Allegrini, AG, Andlauer, TFM, Bates, TC, Bernard, M, Bonte, M, Boomsma, DI, Bourgeron, T, Brandeis, D, Carreiras, M, Ceroni, F, Csepe, V, Dale, PS, DeFries, JC, de Jong, PF, Demonet, JF, de Zeeuw, EL, Franken, M-CJ, Francks, C, Gerritse, M, Gialluisi, A, Gordon, SD, Gruen, JR, Hayiou-Thomas, ME, Hernandez-Cabrera, J, Hottenga, J-J, Hulme, C, Jansen, PR, Kere, J, Koomar, T, Landerl, K, Leonard, GT, Liao, Z, Luciano, M, Lyytinen, H, Martin, NG, Martinelli, A, Maurer, U, Michaelson, JJ, Mirza-Schreiber, N, Moll, K, Monaco, AP, Morgan, AT, Mueller-Myhsok, B, Newbury, DF, Noethen, MM, Olson, RK, Paracchini, S, Paus, T, Pausova, Z, Pennell, CE, Pennington, BF, Plomin, RJ, Ramus, F, Reilly, S, Richer, L, Rimfeld, K, Schulte-Korne, G, Shapland, CY, Simpson, NH, Smith, SD, Snowling, MJ, St Pourcain, B, Stein, JF, Talcott, JB, Tiemeier, H, Tomblin, JB, Truong, DT, van Bergen, E, van der Schroeff, MP, Van Donkelaar, M, Verhoef, E, Wang, CA, Watkins, KE, Whitehouse, AJO, Willcutt, EG, Wright, MJ, Zhu, G, Fisher, SE, Lovett, MW, Strug, LJ, Barr, CL, Price, KM, Wigg, KG, Eising, E, Feng, Y, Blokland, K, Wilkinson, M, Kerr, EN, Guger, SL, Abbondanza, F, Allegrini, AG, Andlauer, TFM, Bates, TC, Bernard, M, Bonte, M, Boomsma, DI, Bourgeron, T, Brandeis, D, Carreiras, M, Ceroni, F, Csepe, V, Dale, PS, DeFries, JC, de Jong, PF, Demonet, JF, de Zeeuw, EL, Franken, M-CJ, Francks, C, Gerritse, M, Gialluisi, A, Gordon, SD, Gruen, JR, Hayiou-Thomas, ME, Hernandez-Cabrera, J, Hottenga, J-J, Hulme, C, Jansen, PR, Kere, J, Koomar, T, Landerl, K, Leonard, GT, Liao, Z, Luciano, M, Lyytinen, H, Martin, NG, Martinelli, A, Maurer, U, Michaelson, JJ, Mirza-Schreiber, N, Moll, K, Monaco, AP, Morgan, AT, Mueller-Myhsok, B, Newbury, DF, Noethen, MM, Olson, RK, Paracchini, S, Paus, T, Pausova, Z, Pennell, CE, Pennington, BF, Plomin, RJ, Ramus, F, Reilly, S, Richer, L, Rimfeld, K, Schulte-Korne, G, Shapland, CY, Simpson, NH, Smith, SD, Snowling, MJ, St Pourcain, B, Stein, JF, Talcott, JB, Tiemeier, H, Tomblin, JB, Truong, DT, van Bergen, E, van der Schroeff, MP, Van Donkelaar, M, Verhoef, E, Wang, CA, Watkins, KE, Whitehouse, AJO, Willcutt, EG, Wright, MJ, Zhu, G, Fisher, SE, Lovett, MW, Strug, LJ, and Barr, CL
- Abstract
Reading Disability (RD) is often characterized by difficulties in the phonology of the language. While the molecular mechanisms underlying it are largely undetermined, loci are being revealed by genome-wide association studies (GWAS). In a previous GWAS for word reading (Price, 2020), we observed that top single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) were located near to or in genes involved in neuronal migration/axon guidance (NM/AG) or loci implicated in autism spectrum disorder (ASD). A prominent theory of RD etiology posits that it involves disturbed neuronal migration, while potential links between RD-ASD have not been extensively investigated. To improve power to identify associated loci, we up-weighted variants involved in NM/AG or ASD, separately, and performed a new Hypothesis-Driven (HD)-GWAS. The approach was applied to a Toronto RD sample and a meta-analysis of the GenLang Consortium. For the Toronto sample (n = 624), no SNPs reached significance; however, by gene-set analysis, the joint contribution of ASD-related genes passed the threshold (p~1.45 × 10-2, threshold = 2.5 × 10-2). For the GenLang Cohort (n = 26,558), SNPs in DOCK7 and CDH4 showed significant association for the NM/AG hypothesis (sFDR q = 1.02 × 10-2). To make the GenLang dataset more similar to Toronto, we repeated the analysis restricting to samples selected for reading/language deficits (n = 4152). In this GenLang selected subset, we found significant association for a locus intergenic between BTG3-C21orf91 for both hypotheses (sFDR q < 9.00 × 10-4). This study contributes candidate loci to the genetics of word reading. Data also suggest that, although different variants may be involved, alleles implicated in ASD risk may be found in the same genes as those implicated in word reading. This finding is limited to the Toronto sample suggesting that ascertainment influences genetic associations.
- Published
- 2022
7. Low-Pressure Laparoscopy Using the AirSeal System versus Standard Insufflation in Early-Stage Endometrial Cancer: A Multicenter, Retrospective Study (ARIEL Study)
- Author
-
Buda, A., Di Martino, G., Borghese, M., Restaino, S., Surace, A., Puppo, A., Paracchini, S., Ferrari, D., Perotto, S., Novelli, A., De Ponti, E., Borghi, C., Fanfani, Francesco, Fruscio, R., Fanfani F. (ORCID:0000-0003-1991-7284), Buda, A., Di Martino, G., Borghese, M., Restaino, S., Surace, A., Puppo, A., Paracchini, S., Ferrari, D., Perotto, S., Novelli, A., De Ponti, E., Borghi, C., Fanfani, Francesco, Fruscio, R., and Fanfani F. (ORCID:0000-0003-1991-7284)
- Abstract
The aim of our study was to evaluate the benefits of a low-pressure insufflation system (AirSeal) vs. a standard insufflation system in terms of anesthesiologists’ parameters and postoperative pain in patients undergoing laparoscopic surgery for early-stage endometrial cancer. This retrospective study involved five tertiary centers and included 152 patients with apparent early-stage disease who underwent laparoscopic surgical staging with either the low-pressure AirSeal system (8–10 mmHg, n = 84) or standard laparoscopic insufflation (10–12 mmHg, n = 68). All the intraoperative anesthesia variables evaluated (systolic blood pressure, end-tidal CO2, peak airway pressure) were significantly lower in the AirSeal group. We recorded a statistically significant difference between the two groups in the median NRS scores for global pain recorded at 4, 8, and 24 h, and for overall shoulder pain after surgery. Significantly more women in the AirSeal group were also discharged on day one compared to the standard group. All such results were confirmed when analyzing the subgroup of women with a BMI >30 kg/m2. In conclusion, according to our preliminary study, low-pressure laparoscopy represents a valid alternative to standard laparoscopy and could facilitate the development of outpatient surgery.
- Published
- 2022
8. Low-Pressure Laparoscopy Using the AirSeal System versus Standard Insufflation in Early-Stage Endometrial Cancer: A Multicenter, Retrospective Study (ARIEL Study)
- Author
-
Buda, A, Di Martino, G, Borghese, M, Restaino, S, Surace, A, Puppo, A, Paracchini, S, Ferrari, D, Perotto, S, Novelli, A, De Ponti, E, Borghi, C, Fanfani, F, Fruscio, R, Buda, A, Di Martino, G, Borghese, M, Restaino, S, Surace, A, Puppo, A, Paracchini, S, Ferrari, D, Perotto, S, Novelli, A, De Ponti, E, Borghi, C, Fanfani, F, and Fruscio, R
- Abstract
The aim of our study was to evaluate the benefits of a low-pressure insufflation system (AirSeal) vs. a standard insufflation system in terms of anesthesiologists’ parameters and postoperative pain in patients undergoing laparoscopic surgery for early-stage endometrial cancer. This retrospective study involved five tertiary centers and included 152 patients with apparent early-stage disease who underwent laparoscopic surgical staging with either the low-pressure AirSeal system (8–10 mmHg, n = 84) or standard laparoscopic insufflation (10–12 mmHg, n = 68). All the intraoperative anesthesia variables evaluated (systolic blood pressure, end-tidal CO2, peak airway pressure) were significantly lower in the AirSeal group. We recorded a statistically significant difference between the two groups in the median NRS scores for global pain recorded at 4, 8, and 24 h, and for overall shoulder pain after surgery. Significantly more women in the AirSeal group were also discharged on day one compared to the standard group. All such results were confirmed when analyzing the subgroup of women with a BMI >30 kg/m2. In conclusion, according to our preliminary study, low-pressure laparoscopy represents a valid alternative to standard laparoscopy and could facilitate the development of outpatient surgery.
- Published
- 2022
9. Evaluation of the laparoscopic component of GESEA Programme in two different groups: Obstetrics and Gynaecology Residents versus Participants in the Annual GESEA Diploma Course in Clermont Ferrand, France
- Author
-
Bustos B, Avilés R, Paracchini S, Pereira B, Revaz Botchorishvili, and Rabischong B
- Subjects
Surgical education ,GESEA Certification ,Laparoscopic training courses ,Original Article ,Practical skills ,Minimally Invasive Surgery - Abstract
Background Structured laparoscopic training courses are important in surgical education. Different programmes have been proposed, but there is currently no evidence available comparing the performance of specialists versus residents in Obstetrics and Gynaecology at these courses. Objective To evaluate the impact of the laparoscopic component of Gynaecological Endoscopic Surgical Education and Assessment (GESEA) Training and Certification courses in two different populations. Materials and methods Prospective cohort study. Two groups were analysed - participants of the Residents’ Courses and participants of the Annual Francophone GESEA Diploma Course. Both groups were evaluated using the GESEA Level 1 laparoscopic standardised exercises and carried out in the International Center of Endoscopic Surgery (CICE), Clermont Ferrand, France in 2019. Results 57 French residents and 69 participants of the Annual GESEA Diploma were evaluated. The average age of participants in the Residents’ Course was lower than those in the Annual Diploma Course (28.4±1.6 versus 35.2±8.0 years, p
- Published
- 2020
10. Lack of replication for the myosin-18B association with mathematical ability in independent cohorts
- Author
-
Pettigrew, K. A., Fajutrao Valles, S. F., Moll, K., Northstone, K., Ring, S., Pennell, C., Wang, C., Leavett, R., Hayiou-Thomas, M. E., Thompson, P., Simpson, N. H., Fisher, S. E., Whitehouse, A. J. O., Snowling, M. J., Newbury, D. F., and Paracchini, S.
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
11. Hand preference and Mathematical Learning Difficulties: New data from Greece, the United Kingdom, and Germany and two meta-analyses of the literature
- Author
-
Papadatou-Pastou, M. Panagiotidou, D.-A. Abbondanza, F. Fischer, U. Paracchini, S. Karagiannakis, G.
- Abstract
Increased rates of atypical handedness are observed in neurotypical individuals who are low-performing in mathematical tasks as well as in individuals with special educational needs, such as dyslexia. This is the first investigation of handedness in individuals with Mathematical Learning Difficulties (MLD). We report three new studies (N = 134; N = 1,893; N = 153) and two sets of meta-analyses (22 studies; N = 3,667). No difference in atypical hand preference between MLD and Typically Achieving (TA) individuals was found when handedness was assessed with self-report questionnaires, but weak evidence of a difference was found when writing hand was the handedness criterion in Study 1 (p =.049). Similarly, when combining data meta-analytically, no hand preference differences were detected. We suggest that: (i) potential handedness effects require larger samples, (ii) direction of hand preference is not a sensitive enough measure of handedness in this context, or that (iii) increased rates of atypical hand preference are not associated with MLD. The latter scenario would suggest that handedness is specifically linked to language-related conditions rather than conditions related to cognitive abilities at large. Future studies need to consider hand skill and degree of hand preference in MLD. © 2021 Informa UK Limited, trading as Taylor & Francis Group.
- Published
- 2021
12. Investigation of Dyslexia and SLI Risk Variants in Reading- and Language-Impaired Subjects
- Author
-
Newbury, D. F., Paracchini, S., Scerri, T. S., Winchester, L., Addis, L., Richardson, Alex J., Walter, J., Stein, J. F., Talcott, J. B., and Monaco, A. P.
- Published
- 2011
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
13. Genome-wide screening for DNA variants associated with reading and language traits
- Author
-
Gialluisi, A., Newbury, D. F., Wilcutt, E. G., Olson, R. K., DeFries, J. C., Brandler, W. M., Pennington, B. F., Smith, S. D., Scerri, T. S., Simpson, N. H., Luciano, M., Evans, D. M., Bates, T. C., Stein, J. F., Talcott, J. B., Monaco, A. P., Paracchini, S., Francks, C., and Fisher, S. E.
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
14. Genome-wide association analyses of child genotype effects and parent-of-origin effects in specific language impairment
- Author
-
Nudel, R., Simpson, N. H., Baird, G., OʼHare, A., Conti-Ramsden, G., Bolton, P. F., Hennessy, E. R., Ring, S. M., Davey Smith, G., Francks, C., Paracchini, S., Monaco, A. P., Fisher, S. E., and Newbury, D. F.
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
15. Human handedness: A meta-analysis
- Author
-
Papadatou-Pastou, M. Ntolka, E. Schmitz, J. Martin, M. Munafò, M.R. Ocklenburg, S. Paracchini, S.
- Abstract
Across time and place, right hand preference has been the norm, but what is the precise prevalence of leftand right-handedness? Frequency of left-handedness has shaped and underpinned different fields of research, from cognitive neuroscience to human evolution, but reliable distributional estimates are still lacking. While hundreds of empirical studies have assessed handedness, a large-scale, comprehensive review of the prevalence of handedness and the factors that moderate it, is currently missing. Here, we report 5 meta-analyses on hand preference for different manual tasks and show that left-handedness prevalence lies between 9.3% (using the most stringent criterion of left-handedness) to 18.1% (using the most lenient criterion of nonright-handedness), with the best overall estimate being 10.6% (10.4% when excluding studies assessing elite athletes' handedness). Handedness variability depends on (a) study characteristics, namely year of publication and ways to measure and classify handedness, and (b) participant characteristics, namely sex and ancestry. Our analysis identifies the role of moderators that require taking into account in future studies on handedness and hemispheric asymmetries. We argue that the same evolutionary mechanisms should apply across geographical regions to maintain the roughly 1:10 ratio, while cultural factors, such as pressure against left-hand use, moderate the magnitude of the prevalence of left-handedness. Although handedness appears as a straightforward trait, there is no universal agreement on how to assess it. Therefore, we urge researchers to fully report study and participant characteristics as well as the detailed procedure by which handedness was assessed and make raw data publicly available. © 2020 American Psychological Association.
- Published
- 2020
16. Four meta-analyses across 164 studies on atypical footedness prevalence and its relation to handedness
- Author
-
Packheiser, J. Schmitz, J. Berretz, G. Carey, D.P. Paracchini, S. Papadatou-Pastou, M. Ocklenburg, S.
- Abstract
Human lateral preferences, such as handedness and footedness, have interested researchers for decades due to their pronounced asymmetries at the population level. While there are good estimates on the prevalence of handedness in the population, there is no large-scale estimation on the prevalence of footedness. Furthermore, the relationship between footedness and handedness still remains elusive. Here, we conducted meta-analyses with four different classification systems for footedness on 145,135 individuals across 164 studies including new data from the ALSPAC cohort. The study aimed to determine a reliable point estimate of footedness, to study the association between footedness and handedness, and to investigate moderating factors influencing footedness. We showed that the prevalence of atypical footedness ranges between 12.10% using the most conservative criterion of left-footedness to 23.7% including all left- and mixed-footers as a single non-right category. As many as 60.1% of left-handers were left-footed whereas only 3.2% of right-handers were left-footed. Males were 4.1% more often non-right-footed compared to females. Individuals with psychiatric and neurodevelopmental disorders exhibited a higher prevalence of non-right-footedness. Furthermore, the presence of mixed-footedness was higher in children compared to adults and left-footedness was increased in athletes compared to the general population. Finally, we showed that footedness is only marginally influenced by cultural and social factors, which play a crucial role in the determination of handedness. Overall, this study provides new and useful reference data for laterality research. Furthermore, the data suggest that footedness is a valuable phenotype for the study of lateral motor biases, its underlying genetics and neurodevelopment. © 2020, The Author(s).
- Published
- 2020
17. Further evidence that the KIAA0319 gene confers susceptibility to developmental dyslexia
- Author
-
Harold, D, Paracchini, S, Scerri, T, Dennis, M, Cope, N, Hill, G, Moskvina, V, Walter, J, Richardson, A J, Owen, M J, Stein, J F, Green, E D, O'Donovan, M C, Williams, J, and Monaco, A P
- Published
- 2006
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
18. A Large AZFc deletion removes DAZ3/DAZ4 and nearby genes from men in Y haplogroup N
- Author
-
Fernandes, S., Paracchini, S., Meyer, L.H., Floridia, G., Tyler-Smith, C., and Vogt, P.H.
- Subjects
Infertility ,Proteins ,Gene expression ,Genetic disorders ,Human genetics ,Biological sciences - Published
- 2004
19. Analysis of dyslexia candidate genes in the Raine cohort representing the general Australian population
- Author
-
Paracchini, S., Ang, Q. W., Stanley, F. J., Monaco, A. P., Pennell, C. E., and Whitehouse, A. J. O.
- Published
- 2011
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
20. Linkage disequilibrium structure of KIAA0319 and DCDC2, two candidate susceptibility genes for developmental dyslexia
- Author
-
Harold, D, Paracchini, S, Scerri, T, Dennis, M, Cope, N, Hill, G, Moskvina, V, Walter, J, Richardson, A J, Owen, M J, Stein, J F, Green, E D, O'Donovan, M C, Williams, J, and Monaco, A P
- Published
- 2006
21. Putative functional alleles of DYX1C1 are not associated with dyslexia susceptibility in a large sample of sibling pairs from the UK
- Author
-
Scerri, T S, Fisher, S E, Francks, C, MacPhie, I L, Paracchini, S, Richardson, A J, Stein, J F, and Monaco, A P
- Published
- 2004
22. A Y chromosomal influence on prostate cancer risk: the multi-ethnic cohort study
- Author
-
Paracchini, S, Pearce, C L, Kolonel, L N, Altshuler, D, Henderson, B E, and Tyler-Smith, C
- Published
- 2003
23. The neuronal migration hypothesis of dyslexia: A critical evaluation 30 years on
- Author
-
Guidi, LG, Velayos-Baeza, A, Martinez-Garay, I, Monaco, AP, Paracchini, S, Bishop, D, and Molnar, Z
- Subjects
mental disorders ,behavioral disciplines and activities - Abstract
The capacity for language is one of the key features underlying the complexity of human cognition and its evolution. However, little is known about the neurobiological mechanisms that mediate normal or impaired linguistic ability. For developmental dyslexia, early postmortem studies conducted in the 1980s linked the disorder to subtle defects in the migration of neurons in the developing neocortex. These early studies were reinforced by human genetic analyses that identified dyslexia susceptibility genes and subsequent evidence of their involvement in neuronal migration. In this review, we examine recent experimental evidence that does not support the link between dyslexia and neuronal migration. We critically evaluate gene function studies conducted in rodent models and draw attention to the lack of robust evidence from histopathological and imaging studies in humans. Our review suggests that the neuronal migration hypothesis of dyslexia should be reconsidered, and the neurobiological basis of dyslexia should be approached with a fresh start.
- Published
- 2018
24. Identification of genetic interactions involved in dyslexia pathogenesis
- Author
-
Karbalai, N., Czamara, D., Moll, K., Ramus, F., Malik, R., Scerri, T.S., Schumacher, J., Morris, A.P., Bourgeron, T., Monaco, A.P., Paracchini, S., Fisher, S.E., Nothen, M., Schulte-Korne, G., and Muller-Myhsok, B.
- Subjects
Neuroinformatics - Abstract
Contains fulltext : 198682.pdf (Publisher’s version ) (Closed access)
- Published
- 2017
25. The DCDC2 deletion is not a risk factor for dyslexia
- Author
-
Scerri, T S, Macpherson, E, Martinelli, A, Wa, W C, Monaco, A P, Stein, J, Zheng, M, Suk-Han Ho, C, McBride, C, Snowling, M, Hulme, C, Hayiou-Thomas, M E, Waye, M M Y, Talcott, Joel B, Paracchini, S, Scerri, T S, Macpherson, E, Martinelli, A, Wa, W C, Monaco, A P, Stein, J, Zheng, M, Suk-Han Ho, C, McBride, C, Snowling, M, Hulme, C, Hayiou-Thomas, M E, Waye, M M Y, Talcott, Joel B, and Paracchini, S
- Abstract
Dyslexia is a specific impairment in learning to read and has strong heritability. An intronic deletion within the DCDC2 gene, with ~8% frequency in European populations, is increasingly used as a marker for dyslexia in neuroimaging and behavioral studies. At a mechanistic level, this deletion has been proposed to influence sensory processing capacity, and in particular sensitivity to visual coherent motion. Our re-assessment of the literature, however, did not reveal strong support for a role of this specific deletion in dyslexia. We also analyzed data from five distinct cohorts, enriched for individuals with dyslexia, and did not identify any signal indicative of associations for the DCDC2 deletion with reading-related measures, including in a combined sample analysis (N=526). We believe we conducted the first replication analysis for a proposed deletion effect on visual motion perception and found no association (N=445 siblings). We also report that the DCDC2 deletion has a frequency of 37.6% in a cohort representative of the general population recruited in Hong Kong (N=220). This figure, together with a lack of association between the deletion and reading abilities in this cohort, indicates the low likelihood of a direct deletion effect on reading skills. Therefore, on the basis of multiple strands of evidence, we conclude that the DCDC2 deletion is not a strong risk factor for dyslexia. Our analyses and literature re-evaluation are important for interpreting current developments within multidisciplinary studies of dyslexia and, more generally, contribute to current discussions about the importance of reproducibility in science.
- Published
- 2017
26. Increased prevalence of sex chromosome aneuploidies in specific language impairment and dyslexia
- Author
-
Simpson, N, Addis, L, Brandler, WM, Slonims, V, Clark, A, Watson, J, Scerri, T, Hennessy, E, Bolton, P, Conti-Ramsden, G, Fairfax, B, Knight, J, Stein, J, Talcott, J, O'Hare, A, Baird, G, Paracchini, S, Fisher, SE, Newbury, D, Nudel, R, Monaco, A, Simonoff, E, Pickles, A, Everitt, A, and Seckl, J
- Abstract
Aim: Sex chromosome aneuploidies increase the risk of spoken or written language disorders but individuals with specific language impairment (SLI) or dyslexia do not routinely undergo cytogenetic analysis. We assess the frequency of sex chromosome aneuploidies in individuals with language impairment or dyslexia. Method: Genome-wide single nucleotide polymorphism genotyping was performed in three sample sets: a clinical cohort of individuals with speech and language deficits (87 probands: 61 males, 26 females; age range 4 to 23 years), a replication cohort of individuals with SLI, from both clinical and epidemiological samples (209 probands: 139 males, 70 females; age range 4 to 17 years), and a set of individuals with dyslexia (314 probands: 224 males, 90 females; age range 7 to 18 years). Results: In the clinical language-impaired cohort, three abnormal karyotypic results were identified in probands (proband yield 3.4%). In the SLI replication cohort, six abnormalities were identified providing a consistent proband yield (2.9%). In the sample of individuals with dyslexia, two sex chromosome aneuploidies were found giving a lower proband yield of 0.6%. In total, two XYY, four XXY (Klinefelter syndrome), three XXX, one XO (Turner syndrome), and one unresolved karyotype were identified. Interpretation: The frequency of sex chromosome aneuploidies within each of the three cohorts was increased over the expected population frequency (approximately 0.25%) suggesting that genetic testing may prove worthwhile for individuals with language and literacy problems and normal non-verbal IQ. Early detection of these aneuploidies can provide information and direct the appropriate management for individuals. © 2013 The Authors. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology published by John Wiley and Sons Ltd on behalf of Mac Keith Press.
- Published
- 2016
27. A Y-chromosomal perspective on the peopling of North Africa
- Author
-
Arredi, B, Poloni, ES, Paracchini, S, Zerjal, T, Fathallah, DM, Makrelouf, M, Novelletto, A, and Tyler-Smith, C
- Published
- 2016
28. Detection of protocadherinX/Y SNPs in patients with schizophrenia and other family members
- Author
-
Giouzeli, M, Williams, NA, Paracchini, S, Tyler-Smith, C, DeLisi, LE, and Crow, TJ
- Published
- 2016
29. Association mapping of the 6p23-21.3 QTL for reading disability
- Author
-
Francks, C, Paracchini, S, Smith, SD, Richardson, AJ, Scerri, TS, Cardon, LR, Marlow, AJ, MacPhie, IL, Walter, J, Pennington, BF, Fisher, SE, Olson, RK, DeFries, JC, Stein, JF, and Monaco, AP
- Published
- 2016
30. Genome-wide association analyses of child genotype effects and parent-of-origin effects in specific language impairment
- Author
-
Nudel, R., Simpson, N., Baird, G., O’Hare, A., Conti-Ramsden, G., Bolton, P., Hennessy, E., The SLli consortium, Ring, S., Smith, G., Francks, C., Paracchini, S., Monaco, A., Fisher, S., Newbury, D., The Royal Society, University of St Andrews. School of Medicine, and University of St Andrews. Biomedical Sciences Research Complex
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Genotype ,Apraxias ,Quantitative Trait Loci ,QH426 Genetics ,Polymorphism, Single Nucleotide ,Genomic Imprinting ,Neurodevelopmental disorder ,RZ ,Chromosomes, Human ,Guanine Nucleotide Exchange Factors ,Humans ,GWAS ,Child ,QH426 ,Adaptor Proteins, Signal Transducing ,Tumor Suppressor Proteins ,RNA-Binding Proteins ,Imprinting ,Original Articles ,ALSPAC ,neurodevelopmental disorder ,specific language impairment ,Specific language impairment ,RC0321 ,Female ,imprinting ,Apoptosis Regulatory Proteins ,Receptors, Prostaglandin E, EP4 Subtype ,RC0321 Neuroscience. Biological psychiatry. Neuropsychiatry ,Genome-Wide Association Study - Abstract
Dianne Newbury is an MRC Career Development Fellow and a Junior Research Fellow at St John’s College, University of Oxford. The work of the Newbury lab is funded by the Medical Research Council [G1000569/1 and MR/J003719/1]. Ron Nudel is funded by a University of Oxford Nuffield Department of Medicine Prize Studentship. The genotyping of samples was funded by the Max Planck Society. Silvia Paracchini is a Royal Society University Research Fellow. The analyses of the ALSPAC cohort were supported by a grant from the Medical Research Council [G0800523/86473]. The collection of the SLIC samples was supported by the Wellcome Trust (060774 and 076566). Patrick Bolton is supported by a National Institute of Health Research (UK) Senior Investigator award and the Biomedical Research Centre in Mental Health at the South London & Maudsley NHS Trust Hospital, London. The work of the Wellcome Trust Centre in Oxford is supported by the Wellcome Trust [090532/Z/09/Z]. Specific language impairment (SLI) is a neurodevelopmental disorder that affects linguistic abilities when development is otherwise normal. We report the results of a genome-wide association study of SLI which included parent-of-origin effects and child genotype effects and used 278 families of language-impaired children. The child genotype effects analysis did not identify significant associations. We found genome-wide significant paternal parent-of-origin effects on chromosome 14q12 (P = 3.74 × 10-8 ) and suggestive maternal parent-of-origin-effects on chromosome 5p13 (P = 1.16 × 10-7 ). A subsequent targeted association of six single-nucleotide-polymorphisms (SNPs) on chromosome 5 in 313 language-impaired individuals from the ALSPAC cohort replicated the maternal effects, albeit in the opposite direction (P = 0.001); as fathers' genotypes were not available in the ALSPAC study, the replication analysis did not include paternal parent-of-origin effects. The paternally-associated SNP on chromosome 14 yields a non-synonymous coding change within the NOP9 gene. This gene encodes an RNA-binding protein that has been reported to be significantly dysregulated in individuals with schizophrenia. The region of maternal association on chromosome 5 falls between the PTGER4 and DAB2 genes, in a region previously implicated in autism and ADHD. The top SNP in this association locus is a potential expression QTL of ARHGEF19 (also called WGEF) on chromosome 1. Members of this protein family have been implicated in intellectual disability. In sum, this study implicates parent-of-origin effects in language impairment, and adds an interesting new dimension to the emerging picture of shared genetic etiology across various neurodevelopmental disorders. Publisher PDF
- Published
- 2016
31. Increased prevalence of sex chromosome aneuploidies in specific language impairment and dyslexia
- Author
-
Simpson, NH, Addis, L, Brandler, WM, Slonims, V, Clark, A, Watson, J, Scerri, TS, Hennessy, ER, Bolton, PF, Conti-Ramsden, G, Fairfax, BP, Knight, JC, Stein, J, Talcott, JB, O'Hare, A, Baird, G, Paracchini, S, Fisher, SE, and Newbury, DF
- Abstract
AIM: Sex chromosome aneuploidies increase the risk of spoken or written language disorders but individuals with specific language impairment (SLI) or dyslexia do not routinely undergo cytogenetic analysis. We assess the frequency of sex chromosome aneuploidies in individuals with language impairment or dyslexia. METHOD: Genome-wide single nucleotide polymorphism genotyping was performed in three sample sets: a clinical cohort of individuals with speech and language deficits (87 probands: 61 males, 26 females; age range 4 to 23 years), a replication cohort of individuals with SLI, from both clinical and epidemiological samples (209 probands: 139 males, 70 females; age range 4 to 17 years), and a set of individuals with dyslexia (314 probands: 224 males, 90 females; age range 7 to 18 years). RESULTS: In the clinical language-impaired cohort, three abnormal karyotypic results were identified in probands (proband yield 3.4%). In the SLI replication cohort, six abnormalities were identified providing a consistent proband yield (2.9%). In the sample of individuals with dyslexia, two sex chromosome aneuploidies were found giving a lower proband yield of 0.6%. In total, two XYY, four XXY (Klinefelter syndrome), three XXX, one XO (Turner syndrome), and one unresolved karyotype were identified. INTERPRETATION: The frequency of sex chromosome aneuploidies within each of the three cohorts was increased over the expected population frequency (approximately 0.25%) suggesting that genetic testing may prove worthwhile for individuals with language and literacy problems and normal non-verbal IQ. Early detection of these aneuploidies can provide information and direct the appropriate management for individuals.
- Published
- 2016
32. Are sequence family variants useful for identifying deletions in the human Y chromosome? Reply
- Author
-
Fernandes, S, Paracchini, S, Meyer, LH, Floridia, G, Tyler-Smith, C, and Vogt, PH
- Published
- 2016
33. Further evidence for a parent-of-origin effect at the NOP9 locus on language-related phenotypes
- Author
-
Pettigrew, K, Frinton, E, Nudel, R, Chan, M, Thompson, P, Hayiou-Thomas, M, Talcott, J, Stein, J, Newbury, D, and Paracchini, S
- Abstract
Background: Specific Language Impairment (SLI) is a common neurodevelopmental disorder, observed in 5-10% of children. Family and twin studies suggest a strong genetic component, but relatively few candidate genes have been reported to date. A recent genome-wide association study (GWAS) described the first statistically significant association specifically for a SLI cohort between a missense variant (rs4280164) in the NOP9 gene and language-related phenotypes under a parent-of-origin model. Replications of these findings are particularly challenging because the availability of parental DNA is required. Methods: We used two independent family-based cohorts characterised with reading- and language-related traits: a longitudinal cohort (n = 106 informative families) including children with language and reading difficulties and a nuclear family cohort (n = 264 families) selected for dyslexia. Results: We observed association with language-related measures when modelling for parent-of-origin effects at the NOP9 locus in both cohorts: minimum P = 0.001 for phonological awareness with a paternal effect in the first cohort and minimum P = 0.0004 for irregular word reading with a maternal effect in the second cohort. Allelic and parental trends were not consistent when compared to the original study. Conclusions: A parent-of-origin effect at this locus was detected in both cohorts, albeit with different trends. These findings contribute in interpreting the original GWAS report and support further investigations of the NOP9 locus and its role in language-related traits. A systematic evaluation of parent-of-origin effects in genetic association studies has the potential to reveal novel mechanisms underlying complex traits.
- Published
- 2016
34. The DCDC2 deletion is not a risk factor for dyslexia
- Author
-
Scerri, T S, primary, Macpherson, E, additional, Martinelli, A, additional, Wa, W C, additional, Monaco, A P, additional, Stein, J, additional, Zheng, M, additional, Suk-Han Ho, C, additional, McBride, C, additional, Snowling, M, additional, Hulme, C, additional, Hayiou-Thomas, M E, additional, Waye, M M Y, additional, Talcott, J B, additional, and Paracchini, S, additional
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
35. Copy number variation screen identifies a rare de Novo deletion at chromosome 15q13.1-13.3 in a child with language impairment
- Author
-
Pettigrew, K. A., Reeves, E., Leavett, R., Hayious-Thomas, Emma, Sharma, A., Simpson, N. H., Martinelli, A., Thompson, Paul A., Hulme, Charles, Snowling, Margaret J., Newbury, D. F., and Paracchini, S.
- Abstract
A significant proportion of children (up to 7% in the UK) present with pronounced language difficulties that cannot be explained by obvious causes like other neurological and medical conditions. A substantial genetic component is predicted to underlie such language problems. Copy number variants (CNVs) have been implicated in neurodevelopmental and psychiatric conditions, such as autism and schizophrenia, but it is not fully established to what extent they might contribute to language disorders. We conducted a CNV screen in a longitudinal cohort of young children with language-related difficulties (n = 85), focusing on single events at candidate loci. We detected a de novo deletion on chromosome 15q13.1-13.3. The adjacent 15q11-13.1 locus is disrupted in Prader-Willi and Angelman syndromes, while disruptions across the breakpoints (BP1-BP6) have previously been implicated in different neurodevelopmental phenotypes including autism, intellectual disability (ID), seizures and developmental delay (DD). This is the first report of a deletion at BP3-BP5 being linked to a deficit confined to language impairment, in the absence of ID, expanding the range of phenotypes that implicate the chromosome 15q13 locus.
- Published
- 2015
36. Lack of replication for the myosin-18B association with mathematical ability in independent cohorts
- Author
-
Pettigrew, K., Fajutrao Valles, S., Moll, K., Northstone, K., Ring, S., Pennell, C., Wang, C., Leavett, R., Hayiou-Thomas, M., Thompson, P., Simpson, N., Fisher, S., Whitehouse, A., Snowling, M., Newbury, D., Paracchini, S., and The SLI Consortium
- Abstract
Twin studies indicate that dyscalculia (or mathematical disability) is caused partly by a genetic component, which is yet to be understood at the molecular level. Recently, a coding variant (rs133885) in the myosin-18B gene was shown to be associated with mathematical abilities with a specific effect among children with dyslexia. This association represents one of the most significant genetic associations reported to date for mathematical abilities and the only one reaching genome-wide statistical significance. We conducted a replication study in different cohorts to assess the effect of rs133885 maths-related measures. The study was conducted primarily using the Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC), (N = 3819). We tested additional cohorts including the York Cohort, the Specific Language Impairment Consortium (SLIC) cohort and the Raine Cohort, and stratified them for a definition of dyslexia whenever possible. We did not observe any associations between rs133885 in myosin-18B and mathematical abilities among individuals with dyslexia or in the general population. Our results suggest that the myosin-18B variant is unlikely to be a main factor contributing to mathematical abilities.
- Published
- 2015
37. The handedness-associated PCSK6 locus spans an intronic promoter regulating novel transcripts
- Author
-
Shore, R, Covill, L, Pettigrew, K, Brandler, W, Diaz, R, Xu, Y, Tello, J, Talcott, J, Newbury, D, Stein, J, Monaco, A, Paracchini, S, Shore, R, Covill, L, Pettigrew, K, Brandler, W, Diaz, R, Xu, Y, Tello, J, Talcott, J, Newbury, D, Stein, J, Monaco, A, and Paracchini, S
- Abstract
We recently reported the association of the PCSK6 gene with handedness through a quantitative genome-wide association study (GWAS; P < 0.5 × 10−8) for a relative hand skill measure in individuals with dyslexia. PCSK6 activates Nodal, a morphogen involved in regulating left–right body axis determination. Therefore, the GWAS data suggest that the biology underlying the patterning of structural asymmetries may also contribute to behavioural laterality, e.g. handedness. The association is further supported by an independent study reporting a variable number tandem repeat (VNTR) within the same PCSK6 locus to be associated with degree of handedness in a general population cohort. Here, we have conducted a functional analysis of the PCSK6 locus combining further genetic analysis, in silico predictions and molecular assays. We have shown that the previous GWAS signal was not tagging a VNTR effect, suggesting that the two markers have independent effects. We demonstrated experimentally that one of the top GWAS-associated markers, rs11855145, directly alters the binding site for a nuclear factor. Furthermore, we have shown that the predicted regulatory region adjacent to rs11855415 acts as a bidirectional promoter controlling the expression of novel RNA transcripts. These include both an antisense long non-coding RNA (lncRNA) and a short PCSK6 isoform predicted to be coding. This is the first molecular characterization of a handedness-associated locus that supports the role of common variants in non-coding sequences in influencing complex phenotypes through gene expression regulation.
- Published
- 2016
38. Increased prevalence of sex chromosome aneuploidies in specific language impairment and dyslexia
- Author
-
Simpson, NH, Addis, L, Brandler, WM, Slonims, V, Clark, A, Watson, J, Scerri, TS, Hennessy, ER, Bolton, PF, Conti-Ramsden, G, Fairfax, BP, Knight, JC, Stein, J, Talcott, JB, O'Hare, A, Baird, G, Paracchini, S, Fisher, SE, Newbury, DF, Nudel, R, Monaco, AP, Simonoff, E, Pickles, A, Everitt, A, Seckl, J, Cowie, H, Cohen, W, Nasir, J, Bishop, DVM, and Simkin, Z
- Abstract
Aim: Sex chromosome aneuploidies increase the risk of spoken or written language disorders but individuals with specific language impairment (SLI) or dyslexia do not routinely undergo cytogenetic analysis. We assess the frequency of sex chromosome aneuploidies in individuals with language impairment or dyslexia. Method: Genome-wide single nucleotide polymorphism genotyping was performed in three sample sets: a clinical cohort of individuals with speech and language deficits (87 probands: 61 males, 26 females; age range 4 to 23 years), a replication cohort of individuals with SLI, from both clinical and epidemiological samples (209 probands: 139 males, 70 females; age range 4 to 17 years), and a set of individuals with dyslexia (314 probands: 224 males, 90 females; age range 7 to 18 years). Results: In the clinical language-impaired cohort, three abnormal karyotypic results were identified in probands (proband yield 3.4%). In the SLI replication cohort, six abnormalities were identified providing a consistent proband yield (2.9%). In the sample of individuals with dyslexia, two sex chromosome aneuploidies were found giving a lower proband yield of 0.6%. In total, two XYY, four XXY (Klinefelter syndrome), three XXX, one XO (Turner syndrome), and one unresolved karyotype were identified. Interpretation: The frequency of sex chromosome aneuploidies within each of the three cohorts was increased over the expected population frequency (approximately 0.25%) suggesting that genetic testing may prove worthwhile for individuals with language and literacy problems and normal non-verbal IQ. Early detection of these aneuploidies can provide information and direct the appropriate management for individuals. © 2013 The Authors. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology published by John Wiley and Sons Ltd on behalf of Mac Keith Press.
- Published
- 2014
39. Lack of replication for the myosin-18b association with mathematical ability in independent cohorts
- Author
-
Pettigrew, K.A., Valles, S.F.F., Moll, K., Northstone, K., Ring, S., Pennell, C., Wang, C., Leavett, R., Hayiou-Thomas, M.E., Thompson, P., Simpson, N.H., Fisher, S.E., Whitehouse, A.J.O., Snowling, M.J., Newbury, D.F., Paracchini, S., Consortium, S.L.I., Pettigrew, K.A., Valles, S.F.F., Moll, K., Northstone, K., Ring, S., Pennell, C., Wang, C., Leavett, R., Hayiou-Thomas, M.E., Thompson, P., Simpson, N.H., Fisher, S.E., Whitehouse, A.J.O., Snowling, M.J., Newbury, D.F., Paracchini, S., and Consortium, S.L.I.
- Abstract
Contains fulltext : 144502.pdf (Publisher’s version ) (Open Access)
- Published
- 2015
40. Expression of the KIAA0319 gene from a haplotype associated with developmental dyslexia
- Author
-
Dennis, M, Paracchini, S, Wade-Martins, R, Velayos-Baeza, A, Green, E, and Monaco, A
- Published
- 2006
41. Further evidence that KIAA0319 is associated with developmental dyslexia
- Author
-
Harold, D, Paracchini, S, Scerri, T, Dennis, M, Cope, N, Hill, G, Moskvina, V, Walter, J, Richardson, A, Owen, M, Stein, J, Green, E, O'Donovan, M, Williams, J, and Monacco, A
- Published
- 2006
42. A two amino acid changing polymorphism in Protocadherin Y cytodomain identifies a new Y chromosome haplotype unrelated to psychosis
- Author
-
Giouzeli, M, Paracchini, S, Williams, N, Tyler-Smith, C, and Crow, T
- Published
- 2004
43. A clinal pattern of human Y chromosome diversity in North Africa
- Author
-
Arredi, B, Poloni, E, Paracchini, S, Zerjal, T, Fathallah, D, Makrelouf, M, Novelletto, A, and Tyler-Smith, C
- Published
- 2004
44. Common variants in left/right asymmetry genes and pathways are associated with relative hand skill
- Author
-
Brandler, W.M., Morris, A.P., Evans, D.M., Scerri, T.S., Kemp, J.P., Timpson, N.J., Pourcain, B. St, Smith, G.D., Ring, S.M., Stein, J., Monaco, A.P., Talcott, J.B., Fisher, S.E., Webber, C., Paracchini, S., Brandler, W.M., Morris, A.P., Evans, D.M., Scerri, T.S., Kemp, J.P., Timpson, N.J., Pourcain, B. St, Smith, G.D., Ring, S.M., Stein, J., Monaco, A.P., Talcott, J.B., Fisher, S.E., Webber, C., and Paracchini, S.
- Abstract
Contains fulltext : 122877.pdf (publisher's version ) (Open Access)
- Published
- 2013
45. Y-chromosomal DNA haplotypes in infertile European males carrying Y-microdeletions (vol 23, pg 671, 2000)
- Author
-
Paracchini, S, Stuppia, L, Gatta, V, Palka, G, Moro, Enrico, Foresta, Carlo, Mengua, L, Oliva, R, Ballesca, Jl, Kremer, Jam, VAN GOLDE RJT, Tuerlings, Jham, Ross, A, Cooke, H, Huellen, K, Vogt, Ph, and TYLER SMITH, C.
- Published
- 2001
46. Investigation of dyslexia and SLI risk variants in reading- and language-impaired subjects
- Author
-
Newbury, D.F., Paracchini, S., Scerri, T.S., Winchester, L., Addis, L., Richardson, A.J., Walter, J., Stein, J.F., Talcott, J.B., Monaco, A.P., Newbury, D.F., Paracchini, S., Scerri, T.S., Winchester, L., Addis, L., Richardson, A.J., Walter, J., Stein, J.F., Talcott, J.B., and Monaco, A.P.
- Abstract
Dyslexia (or reading disability) and specific language impairment (or SLI) are common childhood disorders that show considerable co-morbidity and diagnostic overlaps and have been suggested to share some genetic aetiology. Recently, genetic risk variants have been identified for SLI and dyslexia enabling the direct evaluation of possible shared genetic influences between these disorders. In this study we investigate the role of variants in these genes (namely MRPL19/C20RF3, ROBO1, DCDC2, KIAA0319, DYX1C1, CNTNAP2, ATP2C2 and CMIP) in the aetiology of SLI and dyslexia. We perform case–control and quantitative association analyses using measures of oral and written language skills in samples of SLI and dyslexic families and cases. We replicate association between KIAA0319 and DCDC2 and dyslexia and provide evidence to support a role for KIAA0319 in oral language ability. In addition, we find association between reading-related measures and variants in CNTNAP2 and CMIP in the SLI families.
- Published
- 2011
47. Analysis of dyslexia candidate genes in the Raine cohort representing the general Australian population
- Author
-
Paracchini, S, Ang, QW, Stanley, FJ, Monaco, AP, Pennell, CE, Whitehouse, AJO, Paracchini, S, Ang, QW, Stanley, FJ, Monaco, AP, Pennell, CE, and Whitehouse, AJO
- Abstract
Several genes have been suggested as dyslexia candidates. Some of these candidate genes have been recently shown to be associated with literacy measures in sample cohorts derived from the general population. Here, we have conducted an association study in a novel sample derived from the Australian population (the Raine cohort) to further investigate the role of dyslexia candidate genes. We analysed markers, previously reported to be associated with dyslexia, located within the MRPL19/C2ORF3, KIAA0319, DCDC2 and DYX1C1 genes in a sample of 520 individuals and tested them for association with reading and spelling measures. Association signals were detected for several single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) within DYX1C1 with both the reading and spelling tests. The high linkage disequilibrium (LD) we observed across the DYX1C1 gene suggests that the association signal might not be refined by further genetic mapping.
- Published
- 2011
48. A predominantly neolithic origin for Y-chromosomal DNA variation in North Africa
- Author
-
Arredi, Barbara, Poloni, E, Paracchini, S, Zerjal, Tatiana, Fathallah, Dm, Makrelouf, M, Pascali, Vincenzo Lorenzo, Novelletto, A, Tyler Smith, C., Poloni, Es, Pascali, Vincenzo Lorenzo (ORCID:0000-0001-6520-5224), Arredi, Barbara, Poloni, E, Paracchini, S, Zerjal, Tatiana, Fathallah, Dm, Makrelouf, M, Pascali, Vincenzo Lorenzo, Novelletto, A, Tyler Smith, C., Poloni, Es, and Pascali, Vincenzo Lorenzo (ORCID:0000-0001-6520-5224)
- Abstract
We have typed 275 men from five populations in Algeria, Tunisia, and Egypt with a set of 119 binary markers and 15 microsatellites from the Y chromosome, and we have analyzed the results together with published data from Moroccan populations. North African Y-chromosomal diversity is geographically structured and fits the pattern expected under an isolation-by-distance model. Autocorrelation analyses reveal an east-west cline of genetic variation that extends into the Middle East and is compatible with a hypothesis of demic expansion. This expansion must have involved relatively small numbers of Y chromosomes to account for the reduction in gene diversity towards the West that accompanied the frequency increase of Y haplogroup E3b2, but gene flow must have been maintained to explain the observed pattern of isolation-by-distance. Since the estimates of the times to the most recent common ancestor (TMRCAs) of the most common haplogroups are quite recent, we suggest that the North African pattern of Y-chromosomal variation is largely of Neolithic origin. Thus, we propose that the Neolithic transition in this part of the world was accompanied by demic diffusion of Afro-Asiatic-speaking pastoralists from the Middle East.
- Published
- 2004
49. Investigation of Dyslexia and SLI Risk Variants in Reading- and Language-Impaired Subjects
- Author
-
Newbury, D. F., primary, Paracchini, S., additional, Scerri, T. S., additional, Winchester, L., additional, Addis, L., additional, Richardson, Alex J., additional, Walter, J., additional, Stein, J. F., additional, Talcott, J. B., additional, and Monaco, A. P., additional
- Published
- 2010
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
50. Analysis of dyslexia candidate genes in the Raine cohort representing the general Australian population
- Author
-
Paracchini, S., primary, Ang, Q. W., additional, Stanley, F. J., additional, Monaco, A. P., additional, Pennell, C. E., additional, and Whitehouse, A. J. O., additional
- Published
- 2010
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.