30 results on '"Karaminis, Themis"'
Search Results
2. The Presence of Background Noise Extends the Competitor Space in Native and Non-Native Spoken-Word Recognition: Insights from Computational Modeling
- Author
-
Karaminis, Themis, Hintz, Florian, and Scharenborg, Odette
- Abstract
Oral communication often takes place in noisy environments, which challenge spoken-word recognition. Previous research has suggested that the presence of background noise extends the number of candidate words competing with the target word for recognition and that this extension affects the time course and accuracy of spoken-word recognition. In this study, we further investigated the temporal dynamics of competition processes in the presence of background noise, and how these vary in listeners with different language proficiency (i.e., native and non-native) using computational modeling. We developed ListenIN (Listen-In-Noise), a neural-network model based on an autoencoder architecture, which learns to map phonological forms onto meanings in two languages and simulates native and non-native spoken-word comprehension. We also examined the model's activation states during online spoken-word recognition. These analyses demonstrated that the presence of background noise increases the number of competitor words, which are engaged in phonological competition and that this happens in similar ways intra and interlinguistically and in native and non-native listening. Taken together, our results support accounts positing a "many-additional-competitors scenario" for the effects of noise on spoken-word recognition.
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
3. The Effects of Background Noise on Native and Non-native Spoken-wordRecognition: A Computational Modelling Approach
- Author
-
Karaminis, Themis and Scharenborg, Odette
- Subjects
spoken-word recognition ,non-native listening ,noise ,computational modelling ,deep neural networks - Abstract
How does the presence of background noise affect thecognitive processes underlying spoken-word recognition? Andhow do these effects differ in native and non-native languagelisteners? We addressed these questions using artificial neural-network modelling. We trained a deep auto-encoderarchitecture on binary phonological and semanticrepresentations of 121 English and Dutch translationequivalents. We also varied exposure to the two languages togenerate ‘native English’ and ‘non-native English’ trainednetworks. These networks captured key effects in theperformance (accuracy rates and the number of erroneousresponses per word stimulus) of English and Dutch listeners inan offline English spoken-word identification experiment(Scharenborg et al., 2017), which considered clean and noisylistening conditions and three intensities of speech-shapednoise, applied word-initially or word-finally. Our simulationssuggested that the effects of noise on native and non-nativelistening are comparable and can be accounted for within thesame cognitive architecture for spoken-word recognition.
- Published
- 2018
4. A Cross-linguistic Model of the Acquisition of Inflectional Morphology in English and Modern Greek
- Author
-
Karaminis, Themis and Thomas, Michael
- Published
- 2010
5. Autistic individuals show less grouping-induced bias in numerosity judgments
- Author
-
Pomè, Antonella, primary, Karaminis, Themis, additional, and Burr, David C., additional
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
6. Supporting the wellbeing of caregivers of children on the autism spectrum: A qualitative report on experiences of attending group dance movement psychotherapy
- Author
-
Aithal, Supritha, primary, Karkou, Vicky, additional, Makris, Stergios, additional, Karaminis, Themis, additional, and Powell, Joanne, additional
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
7. Editorial: Advances in the cross-cultural assessment and diagnosis of developmental conditions
- Author
-
Karaminis, Themis, primary, Volpato, Francesca, additional, and Stavrakaki, Stavroula, additional
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
8. A technology-enhanced learning intervention for statistics in higher education using bite-sized video-based learning and precision teaching
- Author
-
Tan, Angel J. Y., primary, Davies, Jean, additional, Nicolson, Roderick I., additional, and Karaminis, Themis, additional
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
9. The Presence of Background Noise Extends the Competitor Space in Native and Non-Native Spoken-Word Recognition: Insights from Computational Modeling
- Author
-
Karaminis, Themis (author), Hintz, Florian (author), Scharenborg, O.E. (author), Karaminis, Themis (author), Hintz, Florian (author), and Scharenborg, O.E. (author)
- Abstract
Oral communication often takes place in noisy environments, which challenge spoken-word recognition. Previous research has suggested that the presence of background noise extends the number of candidate words competing with the target word for recognition and that this extension affects the time course and accuracy of spoken-word recognition. In this study, we further investigated the temporal dynamics of competition processes in the presence of background noise, and how these vary in listeners with different language proficiency (i.e., native and non-native) using computational modeling. We developed ListenIN (Listen-In-Noise), a neural-network model based on an autoencoder architecture, which learns to map phonological forms onto meanings in two languages and simulates native and non-native spoken-word comprehension. We also examined the model's activation states during online spoken-word recognition. These analyses demonstrated that the presence of background noise increases the number of competitor words, which are engaged in phonological competition and that this happens in similar ways intra and interlinguistically and in native and non-native listening. Taken together, our results support accounts positing a “many-additional-competitors scenario” for the effects of noise on spoken-word recognition., Multimedia Computing
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
10. A technology-enhanced learning intervention for statistics in higher education using bite-sized video-based learning and precision teaching.
- Author
-
Tan, Angel J. Y., Davies, Jean, Nicolson, Roderick I., and Karaminis, Themis
- Subjects
EDUCATION statistics ,HIGHER education ,ACHIEVEMENT tests ,LEARNING ,ONLINE education - Abstract
Adjustments to life and learning following the COVID-19 pandemic have transformed user acceptance of online learning methods. It is, therefore, imperative to analyse factors relating to user performance and preferences for such interactions. In this study, we combined video-based learning with precision teaching to reinforce previously learnt statistics skills in university students without a mathematical background. We developed a learning design consisting of eight 'bite-sized' online learning episodes. Each episode started with a brief learning video followed by a practice phase and an end-of-episode assessment. The practice phase differed in two groups of participants, matched on statistics attainment pre-intervention. A precision-teaching intervention group (N = 19) completed practice guided by a frequency-based approach aiming at building fluency in statistics. A control group (N = 19) completed self-directed practice for the same amount of time as the intervention group. All participants completed a statistics attainment test and a questionnaire on their attitudes towards statistics pre- and post-intervention, and a review of the learning materials post-intervention. The intervention group achieved, consistently, higher scores in all end-of-episode assessments compared to the control group. Both groups showed significant and comparable improvements in statistics attainment post-intervention. Both groups also reported more positive feelings towards statistics post-intervention, while the review of the learning materials suggested that the video-based learning design was well-received by students. Our results suggest that video-based learning has great potential to support, as a supplementary teaching aid, university students in learning statistics. We discuss future research directions and implications of the study. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
11. A Systematic Review of the Contribution of Dance Movement Psychotherapy Towards the Well-Being of Children With Autism Spectrum Disorders
- Author
-
Aithal, Supritha, primary, Moula, Zoe, additional, Karkou, Vicky, additional, Karaminis, Themis, additional, Powell, Joanne, additional, and Makris, Stergios, additional
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
12. A Dance Movement Psychotherapy Intervention for the Wellbeing of Children With an Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Pilot Intervention Study
- Author
-
Aithal, Supritha, primary, Karkou, Vicky, additional, Makris, Stergios, additional, Karaminis, Themis, additional, and Powell, Joanne, additional
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
13. A Dance Movement Psychotherapy Intervention for the Wellbeing of Children With an Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Pilot Intervention Study
- Author
-
Aithal, Supritha, Karkou, Vicky, Makris, Stergios, Karaminis, Themis, Powell, Joanne, Aithal, Supritha, Karkou, Vicky, Makris, Stergios, Karaminis, Themis, and Powell, Joanne
- Abstract
Background: Sustaining the wellbeing for children with an autism spectrum disorder (ASD) can be highly demanding. Dance Movement Psychotherapy (DMP), a form of psychotherapy with a non-verbal character, may present as a relevant intervention option for this group of children. Methods: A protocol-based group DMP intervention was developed and implemented in two special educational needs schools in the North West of England. We aimed to investigate the effects of DMP on children with ASD using the Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ) and Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ). Twenty-six children aged between 8 and 13 years (mean age = 10.65 years) with ASD were randomly allocated to DMP and a control group with standard care, following a crossover research design. Results: Results showed no significant carryover or period effects for either the SCQ or SDQ (p > 0.05). A significant intervention effect was found only for SCQ (p = 0.005) but not for SDQ (p > 0.05). ANCOVAs were performed on the data before the crossover to test for differences in SCQ and SDQ scores between the DMP intervention and control groups while controlling for pre-intervention scores. Those in the DMP intervention group presented significantly lower SCQ scores following the intervention period than those in the control group (p = 0.001). No significant differences in post-intervention SDQ scores were found between DMP intervention and control groups (p = 0.2). However, minimal clinically important differences (MCID) were reached for both SCQ and SDQ measures before crossover for those in the DMP intervention group. Moreover, repeated measures ANOVAs performed on SCQ and SDQ measures following crossover were significant, with the change in both SCQ (p = 0.001) and SDQ (p = 0.009) pre-and post-intervention being significantly greater for those in the DMP intervention than the control group. Conclusion: The pilot DMP intervention has shown promising resul
- Published
- 2021
14. A Systematic Review of the Contribution of Dance Movement Psychotherapy Towards the Well-Being of Children With Autism Spectrum Disorders
- Author
-
AITHAL, SUPRITHA, MOULA, ZOI, Karkou, Vicky, MAKRIS, STERGIOS, Karaminis, Themis, Powell, Joanne, AITHAL, SUPRITHA, MOULA, ZOI, Karkou, Vicky, MAKRIS, STERGIOS, Karaminis, Themis, and Powell, Joanne
- Abstract
Background: The present review provides an original examination of published literature on the use of Dance Movement Psychotherapy (DMP) as an intervention for children with an Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD). Method: The review was systematically conducted using the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis (PRISMA) guidelines. A protocol consisting of four phases: identification; screening and selection; data extraction and synthesis; quality assurance was developed and registered with the PROSPERO. A search strategy was developed using population and intervention as the key concepts and ten databases were searched between 6.1.2018 to 4.4.2018 and 10.07.2021 to 20.07.2021. The intervention characteristics were extracted based on the TIDieR template for intervention description and replication checklist. Quality assessment and level of evidence of all the included studies were evaluated using the Mixed Methods Appraisal Tool (MMAT) and the Centre for Evidence-Based Medicine (CEBM) for treatment criteria. Results: Nine research studies with a total of 133 participants were identified through a systematic search process. There was only one mixed-methods study with the component of randomisation found during the literature search. Collected information was synthesised in relation to (a) ways in which dance movement psychotherapists work with children; (b) data collection methods and findings. Results from the reviewed literature suggest that DMP can potentially promote various aspects of well-being in children with ASD. Eight out of nine studies mentioned the effects of DMP on improving different social and communication skills. However, results from quality assessments and synthesised outcomes indicate that research in DMP is still in its infancy. Conclusions: We conclude that further large-scale, high-quality studies are required to generate further evidence that explains the processes involved in DMP, the effectiven
- Published
- 2021
15. Impact of Dance Movement Psychotherapy on the Wellbeing of Caregivers of Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder
- Author
-
AITHAL, SUPRITHA, Karkou, Vicky, MAKRIS, STERGIOS, Karaminis, Themis, Powell, Joanne, AITHAL, SUPRITHA, Karkou, Vicky, MAKRIS, STERGIOS, Karaminis, Themis, and Powell, Joanne
- Abstract
Objectives Sustaining the wellbeing of caregivers of children with Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) can be highly demanding. This study explored the impact of DMP intervention on the wellbeing of caregivers in comparison with their standard care routine. Study design This pilot evaluation study used a quasi-experimental design. Methods Thirty-seven caregivers of children with ASD were recruited from two special educational needs settings and were allocated to the DMP intervention or the control group depending on their availability to attend the sessions. The participants in the intervention group received five DMP sessions lasting 90 minutes each. Adult Wellbeing Scale (AWS) and Parenting Stress Index- Short Form (PSI-SF) were the two outcome measures that were administered before and after DMP to measure the impact of DMP intervention on caregiver’s wellbeing and parental stress. Results The retention rates were poor, with only 50% of participants in the DMP intervention arm attending at least 70% of the sessions until its end. The Minimal Clinically Important Difference (MCID) was achieved for a small effect size in both outcome measures in the DMP intervention group but not in the control group. Results from the ANCOVAs showed a significant difference in post-intervention scores between the DMP intervention and the control group for AWS (F1,33 = 106.474, P<0.001) but not for PSI-SF. In addition, a significant association was found between pre-intervention scores and the number of sessions attended with the post-intervention scores of both AWS and PSI-SF. Conclusions The results of this pilot DMP study are promising. However, before running a larger randomised controlled trial, strategies to support caregivers to attend the intervention need to be considered carefully.
- Published
- 2021
16. Adaptation to the Speed of Biological Motion in Autism
- Author
-
Karaminis, Themis, Arrighi, Roberto, Forth, Georgia, Burr, David, Pellicano, Elizabeth, Karaminis, Themis, Arrighi, Roberto, Forth, Georgia, Burr, David, and Pellicano, Elizabeth
- Abstract
Autistic individuals often present atypicalities in adaptation—the continuous recalibration of perceptual systems driven by recent sensory experiences. Here, we examined such atypicalities in human biological motion. We used a dual-task paradigm, including a running-speed discrimination task (‘comparing the speed of two running silhouettes’) and a change-detection task (‘detecting fixation-point shrinkages’) assessing attention. We tested 19 school-age autistic and 19 age- and ability-matched typical participants, also recording eye-movements. The two groups presented comparable speed-discrimination abilities and, unexpectedly, comparable adaptation. Accuracy in the change-detection task and the scatter of eye-fixations around the fixation point were also similar across groups. Yet, the scatter of fixations reliably predicted the magnitude of adaptation, demonstrating the importance of controlling for attention in adaptation studies.
- Published
- 2020
17. The Effects of Background Noise on Native and Non-native Spoken-word Recognition: A Computational Modelling Approach
- Author
-
Karaminis, Themis and Karaminis, Themis
- Abstract
How does the presence of background noise affect the cognitive processes underlying spoken-word recognition? And how do these effects differ in native and non-native language listeners? We addressed these questions using artificial neural-network modelling. We trained a deep auto-encoder architecture on binary phonological and semantic representations of 121 English and Dutch translation equivalents. We also varied exposure to the two languages to generate ‘native English’ and ‘non-native English’ trained networks. These networks captured key effects in the performance (accuracy rates and the number of erroneous responses per word stimulus) of English and Dutch listeners in an offline English spoken-word identification experiment (Scharenborg et al., 2017), which considered clean and noisy listening conditions and three intensities of speech-shaped noise, applied word-initially or word-finally. Our simulations suggested that the effects of noise on native and non-native listening are comparable and can be accounted for within the same cognitive architecture for spoken-word recognition.
- Published
- 2018
18. Children on the autism spectrum update their behaviour in response to a volatile environment
- Author
-
Manning, Catherine, Kilner, James, Neil, Louise, Karaminis, Themis, Pellicano, Elizabeth, Manning, Catherine, Kilner, James, Neil, Louise, Karaminis, Themis, and Pellicano, Elizabeth
- Abstract
Typical adults can track reward probabilities across trials to estimate the volatility of the environment and use this information to modify their learning rate (Behrens et al., 2007). In a stable environment, it is advantageous to take account of outcomes over many trials, whereas in a volatile environment, recent experience should be more strongly weighted than distant experience. Recent predictive coding accounts of autism propose that autistic individuals will demonstrate atypical updating of their behaviour in response to the statistics of the reward environment. To rigorously test this hypothesis, we administered a developmentally appropriate version of Behrens et al.’s (2007) task to 34 cognitively able children on the autism spectrum aged between 6 and 14 years, 32 age- and ability-matched typically developing children and 19 typical adults. Participants were required to choose between a green and a blue pirate chest, each associated with a randomly determined reward value between 0 and 100 points, with a combined total of 100 points. On each trial, the reward was given for one stimulus only. In the stable condition, the ratio of the blue or green response being rewarded was fixed at 75:25. In the volatile condition, the ratio alternated between 80:20 and 20:80 every 20 trials. We estimated the learning rate for each participant by fitting a delta rule model and compared this rate across conditions and groups. All groups increased their learning rate in the volatile condition compared to the stable condition. Unexpectedly, there was no effect of group and no interaction between group and condition. Thus, autistic children used information about the statistics of the reward environment to guide their decisions to a similar extent as typically developing children and adults. These results help constrain predictive coding accounts of autism by demonstrating that autism is not characterized by uniform differences in the weighting of prediction error.
- Published
- 2017
19. The light-from-above prior is intact in autistic children
- Author
-
Croydon, Abigail, Karaminis, Themis, Neil, Louise, Burr, David, Pellicano, Elizabeth, Croydon, Abigail, Karaminis, Themis, Neil, Louise, Burr, David, and Pellicano, Elizabeth
- Abstract
Sensory information is inherently ambiguous. The brain disambiguates this information by anticipating or predicting the sensory environment based on prior knowledge. Pellicano and Burr (2012) proposed that this process may be atypical in autism and that internal assumptions, or ‘‘priors,” may be underweighted or less used than in typical individuals. A robust internal assumption used by adults is the ‘‘light-from-above” prior, a bias to interpret ambiguous shading patterns as if formed by a light source located above (and slightly to the left) of the scene. We investigated whether autistic children (n = 18) use this prior to the same degree as typical children of similar age and intellectual ability (n = 18). Children were asked to judge the shape (concave or convex) of a shaded hexagon stimulus presented in 24 rotations. We estimated the relation between the proportion of convex judgments and stimulus orientation for each child and calculated the light source location most consistent with those judgments. Children behaved similarly to adults in this task, preferring to assume that the light source was from above left, when other interpretations were compatible with the shading evidence. Autistic and typical children used prior assumptions to the same extent to make sense of shading patterns. Future research should examine whether this prior is as adaptable (i.e., modifiable with training) in autistic children as it is in typical adults.
- Published
- 2017
20. Binocular Rivalry in Children on the Autism Spectrum
- Author
-
Karaminis, Themis, Lunghi, Claudia, Neil, Louise, Burr, David, Pellicano, Elizabeth, Karaminis, Themis, Lunghi, Claudia, Neil, Louise, Burr, David, and Pellicano, Elizabeth
- Abstract
hen different images are presented to the eyes, the brain is faced with ambiguity, causing perceptual bistability: visual perception continuously alternates between the monocular images, a phenomenon called binocular rivalry. Many models of rivalry suggest that its temporal dynamics depend on mutual inhibition among neurons representing competing images. These models predict that rivalry should be different in autism, which has been proposed to present an atypical ratio of excitation and inhibition [the E/I imbalance hypothesis; Rubenstein & Merzenich, 2003]. In line with this prediction, some recent studies have provided evidence for atypical binocular rivalry dynamics in autistic adults. In this study, we examined if these findings generalize to autistic children. We developed a childfriendly binocular rivalry paradigm, which included two types of stimuli, low- and high-complexity, and compared rivalry dynamics in groups of autistic and age- and intellectual ability-matched typical children. Unexpectedly, the two groups of children presented the same number of perceptual transitions and the same mean phase durations (times perceiving one of the two stimuli). Yet autistic children reported mixed percepts for a shorter proportion of time (a difference which was in the opposite direction to previous adult studies), while elevated autistic symptomatology was associated with shorter mixed perception periods. Rivalry in the two groups was affected similarly by stimulus type, and consistent with previous findings. Our results suggest that rivalry dynamics are differentially affected in adults and developing autistic children and could be accounted for by hierarchical models of binocular rivalry, including both inhibition and top-down influences.
- Published
- 2017
21. Ensemble perception of emotions in autistic and typical children and adolescents.
- Author
-
Karaminis, Themis, Neil, Louise, Manning, Catherine, Turi, Marco, Florentini, Chiara, Burr, David, Pellicano, Elizabeth, Karaminis, Themis, Neil, Louise, Manning, Catherine, Turi, Marco, Florentini, Chiara, Burr, David, and Pellicano, Elizabeth
- Abstract
Ensemble perception, the ability to assess automatically the summary of large amounts of informationpresented in visual scenes, is available early in typical development. This ability might be compromisedin autistic children, who are thought to present limitations in maintaining summary statistics representa-tions for the recent history of sensory input. Here we examined ensemble perception of facial emotionalexpressions in 35 autistic children, 30 age- and ability-matched typical children and 25 typical adults.Participants received three tasks: a) an ‘ensemble’ emotion discrimination task; b) a baseline (single-face)emotion discrimination task; and c) a facial expression identification task. Children performed worse thanadults on all three tasks. Unexpectedly, autistic and typical children were, on average, indistinguishablein their precision and accuracy on all three tasks. Computational modelling suggested that, on average,autistic and typical children used ensemble-encoding strategies to a similar extent; but ensemble percep-tion was related to non-verbal reasoning abilities in autistic but not in typical children. Eye-movementdata also showed no group differences in the way children attended to the stimuli. Our combined find-ings suggest that the abilities of autistic and typical children for ensemble perception of emotions arecomparable on average.
- Published
- 2017
22. No rapid audiovisual recalibration in adults on the autism spectrum
- Author
-
Turi, Marco, Karaminis, Themis, Pellicano, Elizabeth, Burr, David, Turi, Marco, Karaminis, Themis, Pellicano, Elizabeth, and Burr, David
- Abstract
Autism spectrum disorders (ASD) are characterized by difficulties in social cognition, but are also associated with atypicalities in sensory and perceptual processing. Several groups have reported that autistic individuals show reduced integration of socially relevant audiovisual signals, which may contribute to the higher-order social and cognitive difficulties observed in autism. Here we use a newly devised technique to study instantaneous adaptation to audiovisual asynchrony in autism. Autistic and typical participants were presented with sequences of brief visual and auditory stimuli, varying in asynchrony over a wide range, from 512 ms auditory-lead to 512 ms auditory-lag, and judged whether they seemed to be synchronous. Typical adults showed strong adaptation effects, with trials proceeded by an auditory-lead needing more auditory-lead to seem simultaneous, and vice versa. However, autistic observers showed little or no adaptation, although their simultaneity curves were as narrow as the typical adults. This result supports recent Bayesian models that predict reduced adaptation effects in autism. As rapid audiovisual recalibration may be fundamental for the optimisation of speech comprehension, recalibration problems could render language processing more difficult in autistic individuals, hindering social communication.
- Published
- 2016
23. Recognizing the same face in different contexts: Testing within-person face recognition in typical development and in autism
- Author
-
Neil, Louise, Cappagli, Giulia, Karaminis, Themis, Jenkins, Rob, Pellicano, Elizabeth, Neil, Louise, Cappagli, Giulia, Karaminis, Themis, Jenkins, Rob, and Pellicano, Elizabeth
- Abstract
Unfamiliar face recognition follows a particularly protracted developmental trajectory and is more likely to be atypical in children with autism than those without autism. There is a paucity of research, however, examining the ability to recognize the same face across multiple naturally varying images. Here, we investigated within-person face recognition in children with and without autism. In Experiment 1, typically developing 6- and 7-year-olds, 8- and 9-year-olds, 10- and 11-year-olds, 12- to 14-year-olds, and adults were given 40 grayscale photographs of two distinct male identities (20 of each face taken at different ages, from different angles, and in different lighting conditions) and were asked to sort them by identity. Children mistook images of the same person as images of different people, subdividing each individual into many perceived identities. Younger children divided images into more perceived identities than adults and also made more misidentification errors (placing two different identities together in the same group) than older children and adults. In Experiment 2, we used the same procedure with 32 cognitively able children with autism. Autistic children reported a similar number of identities and made similar numbers of misidentification errors to a group of typical children of similar age and ability. Fine-grained analysis using matrices revealed marginal group differences in overall performance. We suggest that the immature performance in typical and autistic children could arise from problems extracting the perceptual commonalities from different images of the same person and building stable representations of facial identity.
- Published
- 2015
24. The effects of grouping on speed discrimination thresholds in adults, typically developing children, and children with autism.
- Author
-
Manning, Catherine, Neil, Louise, Karaminis, Themis, Pellicano, Elizabeth, Manning, Catherine, Neil, Louise, Karaminis, Themis, and Pellicano, Elizabeth
- Abstract
Adult observers show elevated speed discrimination thresholds when comparing the speeds of objects moving across a boundary compared to those moving parallel to a boundary (Verghese & McKee, 2006)—an effect that has been attributed to grouping processes in conjunction with a prior for smooth motion. Here, we extended Verghese and McKee's (2006) paradigm to typically developing children (n = 35) and children with autism (n = 26) and compared their performance with that of typical adults (n = 19). Speed discrimination thresholds were measured in three conditions: (a) with dots moving parallel to a boundary, (b) with dots moving perpendicular to a boundary, and (c) with dots in each stimulus half moving in orthogonal, oblique directions. As expected, participants had higher speed discrimination thresholds when dots appeared to cross a boundary compared to when dots moved parallel to the boundary. However, participants had even higher thresholds when dots moved in oblique, orthogonal directions, where grouping should be minimal. All groups of participants showed a similar pattern of performance across conditions although children had higher thresholds than adult participants overall. We consider various explanations for the pattern of performance obtained, including enhanced sensitivity for shearing motions and reduced sensitivity for discriminating different directions. Our results demonstrate that the speed discrimination judgments of typically developing children and children with autism are similarly affected by spatial configuration as those of typical adults and provide further evidence that speed discrimination is unimpaired in children with autism.
- Published
- 2015
25. Atypicalities in Perceptual Adaptation in Autism Do Not Extend to Perceptual Causality
- Author
-
Karaminis, Themis, Turi, Marco, Neil, Louise, Badcock, Nicholas A., Burr, David, Pellicano, Elizabeth, Karaminis, Themis, Turi, Marco, Neil, Louise, Badcock, Nicholas A., Burr, David, and Pellicano, Elizabeth
- Abstract
A recent study showed that adaptation to causal events (collisions) in adults caused subsequent events to be less likely perceived as causal. In this study, we examined if a similar negative adaptation effect for perceptual causality occurs in children, both typically developing and with autism. Previous studies have reported diminished adaptation for face identity, facial configuration and gaze direction in children with autism. To test whether diminished adaptive coding extends beyond high-level social stimuli (such as faces) and could be a general property of autistic perception, we developed a child-friendly paradigm for adaptation of perceptual causality. We compared the performance of 22 children with autism with 22 typically developing children, individually matched on age and ability (IQ scores). We found significant and equally robust adaptation aftereffects for perceptual causality in both groups. There were also no differences between the two groups in their attention, as revealed by reaction times and accuracy in a change-detection task. These findings suggest that adaptation to perceptual causality in autism is largely similar to typical development and, further, that diminished adaptive coding might not be a general characteristic of autism at low levels of the perceptual hierarchy, constraining existing theories of adaptation in autism.
- Published
- 2015
26. Ensemble perception of emotions in children with autism
- Author
-
Karaminis, Themis, primary, Neil, Louise, additional, Manning, Catherine, additional, Turi, Marco, additional, Fiorentini, Chiara, additional, Burr, David, additional, and Pellicano, Liz, additional
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
27. Language switching in bilingual production: Empirical data and computational modelling
- Author
-
Filippi, Roberto, Karaminis, Themis, Thomas, Michael, Filippi, Roberto, Karaminis, Themis, and Thomas, Michael
- Abstract
One key issue in bilingualism is how bilinguals control production, particularly to produce words in the less dominant language. Language switching is one method to investigate control processes. Language switching has been much studied in comprehension, e.g., in lexical decision task, but less so in production. Here we first present a study of language switching in Italian–English adult bilinguals in a naming task for visually presented words. We demonstrate an asymmetric pattern of time costs to switch language, where participants incurred a greater time cost to switch into naming in their dominant language (Italian). In addition, costs were greater where the stimuli were interlingual cognates or homographs than words existing in only one language, implicating lexical competition as a source of the cost. To clarify the operation of control processes, we then present two connectionist models of bilingual naming, based on the previous models of Seidenberg and McClelland (1989), Cohen, Dunbar and McClelland (1990), Gilbert and Shallice (2002), and Karaminis and Thomas (2010). Crucially, both models acquired their differential language dominance via an experience-dependent learning process. The models embody different assumptions about the language control processes that produce the switch cost. We consider which processing assumptions are sufficient to explain asymmetric language switch costs and word class effects on language switching in individual word reading, as well as generating novel predictions for future testing.
- Published
- 2014
28. Language switching in bilingual production: Empirical data and computational modelling
- Author
-
Filippi, Roberto, Karaminis, Themis, Thomas, Michael, Filippi, Roberto, Karaminis, Themis, and Thomas, Michael
- Abstract
One key issue in bilingualism is how bilinguals control production, particularly to produce words in the less dominant language. Language switching is one method to investigate control processes. Language switching has been much studied in comprehension, e.g., in lexical decision task, but less so in production. Here we first present a study of language switching in Italian–English adult bilinguals in a naming task for visually presented words. We demonstrate an asymmetric pattern of time costs to switch language, where participants incurred a greater time cost to switch into naming in their dominant language (Italian). In addition, costs were greater where the stimuli were interlingual cognates or homographs than words existing in only one language, implicating lexical competition as a source of the cost. To clarify the operation of control processes, we then present two connectionist models of bilingual naming, based on the previous models of Seidenberg and McClelland (1989), Cohen, Dunbar and McClelland (1990), Gilbert and Shallice (2002), and Karaminis and Thomas (2010). Crucially, both models acquired their differential language dominance via an experience-dependent learning process. The models embody different assumptions about the language control processes that produce the switch cost. We consider which processing assumptions are sufficient to explain asymmetric language switch costs and word class effects on language switching in individual word reading, as well as generating novel predictions for future testing.
- Published
- 2013
29. What is typical language development?
- Author
-
Thomas, Michael, Karaminis, Themis, Knowland, Victoria, Thomas, Michael, Karaminis, Themis, and Knowland, Victoria
- Abstract
In light of Musolino, Chunyo and Landau’s findings and conclusions regarding syntax comprehension in Williams syndrome (this issue), we review the criteria used to determine whether the development of language is typical or atypical and our current understanding of the causes of language delay. Given a certain set of theoretical assumptions (e.g., generative / modular), fairly poor performance can nevertheless be viewed as indicating typical development. Given other theoretical assumptions (e.g., a neuroconstructivist view of constrained development), the same data can be viewed as indicative of atypicality.
- Published
- 2010
30. Visual Online Control of Goal-Directed Aiming Movements in Children.
- Author
-
Mackrous, Isabelle, Proteau, Luc, Gill, Simone V., and Karaminis, Themis
- Subjects
GOAL (Psychology) ,MOTOR ability in children ,EMOTION regulation ,ADULTS ,AGE groups - Abstract
The present study investigated whether the initial impulse of goal-directed movements was visually monitored by 5- to 12-years-old children (n = 36) in a manner similar to adults (n = 12). The participants moved a cursor toward a fixed target. In some trials, the cursor was unpredictably translated by 20 mm following movement initiation. The results showed that even the youngest children visually monitor the initial impulse of goaldirected movements. This monitoring and the error correction process that it triggers seem automatic because it occurs even when the cursor jump is not consciously detected. Finally, it appears that this process does not fully mature before late childhood, which suggests that a putative dedicated channel for processing visual hand information develops during childhood. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.