10 results on '"Andy P Field"'
Search Results
2. Anxiety Disorders in Children and Adolescents
- Author
-
Wendy K. Silverman, Andy P. Field, Wendy K. Silverman, and Andy P. Field
- Subjects
- Adolescent psychotherapy, Child development, Child psychotherapy, Anxiety in children, Anxiety in adolescence, Children
- Abstract
Anxiety disorders are among the most prevalent mental health problems in childhood and adolescence. This fully revised new edition is an authoritative guide to the understanding and assessment of anxiety disorders in the young. The first section covers historical and conceptual issues, including cognitive and developmental processes, clinical and theoretical models, phenomenology and classification, and evidence-based assessment. Subsequent sections cover the biology of child and adolescent anxiety, and environmental influences including traumatic events, parenting and the impact of the peer group. The final section addresses prevention and treatment of anxiety. All chapters incorporate new advances in the field, explicitly differentiate between children and adolescents, and incorporate a developmental perspective. Written and edited by an international team of leading experts in the field, this is a key text for researchers, practitioners, students and clinical trainees with interests in child and adolescent anxiety.
- Published
- 2011
3. Preface
- Author
-
Wendy K. Silverman and Andy P. Field
- Subjects
medicine.medical_specialty ,Psychotherapist ,medicine ,Child and adolescent psychiatry ,Anxiety ,medicine.symptom ,Psychiatry ,Neuropsychiatry ,Psychology - Published
- 2011
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
4. Anxiety Disorders in Children and Adolescents
- Author
-
Andy P. Field and Wendy K. Silverman
- Subjects
Child and adolescent ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Child and adolescent psychiatry ,medicine ,Anxiety ,medicine.symptom ,Neuropsychiatry ,Psychology ,Psychiatry ,Clinical psychology - Abstract
Anxiety disorders are among the most prevalent mental health problems in childhood and adolescence. This fully revised new edition is an authoritative guide to the understanding and assessment of anxiety disorders in the young. The first section covers historical and conceptual issues, including cognitive and developmental processes, clinical and theoretical models, phenomenology and classification, and evidence-based assessment. Subsequent sections cover the biology of child and adolescent anxiety, and environmental influences including traumatic events, parenting and the impact of the peer group. The final section addresses prevention and treatment of anxiety. All chapters incorporate new advances in the field, explicitly differentiate between children and adolescents, and incorporate a developmental perspective. Written and edited by an international team of leading experts in the field, this is a key text for researchers, practitioners, students and clinical trainees with interests in child and adolescent anxiety.
- Published
- 2011
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
5. The role of learning in the etiology of child and adolescent fear and anxiety
- Author
-
Andy P. Field and Helena M. Purkis
- Subjects
Phobias ,Social anxiety ,medicine ,Anxiety ,Fear conditioning ,medicine.symptom ,medicine.disease ,Psychology ,Anxiety disorder ,Post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) ,Developmental psychology ,Arousal ,Associative learning - Abstract
Hush little baby, don't say a word; And never mind that noise you heard. It's just the beast under your bed. In the closet, in your head. (Metallica, 1991, track 1) Children are so prone to fear that it has been seen as a normal part of childhood development (see Muris & Field, Chapter 4, this volume). These normal fears are likely to be the precursors of more persistent and severe anxiety (Field & Davey, 2001) because the age of onset of anxiety disorders broadly follows the developmental pattern of non-clinical fears. As reviewed by Muris and Field, infants tend to fear novel stimuli within their immediate environment such as loud noise, objects, and separation from a caretaker (Campbell, 1986), in mid-childhood (6–8 years old) fears are focused towards ghosts and animals, then self-injury in late childhood (Bauer, 1976), and social evaluation in pre-adolescence and adolescence (Westenberg, Drewes, Goedhart, Siebelink, & Treffers, 2004). Similarly, phobias concerning environmental threats (e.g., heights, water) typically originate in infancy (Menzies & Clarke, 1993a, 1993b), then specific phobias emerge in middle childhood (5–6 years) followed by generalized anxiety in late childhood and social anxiety in pre-adolescence (Costello, Egger, Copeland, Erkanli, & Angold, Chapter 3, this volume). This broad correspondence between normal fears and the onset of anxiety disorders raises three questions pertinent to explaining the etiology of anxiety disorders: (1) are these normal fears innate or learnt? (2) What process underlies whether a fear develops into an anxiety disorder? And (3) what variables moderate this process? This chapter attempts to explore these questions by evaluating the contribution of learning processes in the development of both normal fears and anxiety disorders. What are fear and anxiety? Lang, Davis, and Ohman (2000) characterize fear as a reaction to a specific threat, with increasing proximity resulting in escape or avoidance; anxiety, however, is characterized by a more diffuse state with less explicit, more generalized cues and involves increased physiological arousal but without necessarily leading to organized functional behavior. Throughout this chapter, we talk primarily about models of “fear” because it is convenient to describe the mechanisms of acquisition as operating at an individual stimulus level. The difference between whether a child acquires “fear” or “anxiety” lies in the extent to which they have learning experiences about a specific group of related stimuli (fear) or a diffuse array of situations (anxiety). However, the underlying mechanisms are similar. There is also a distinction to be made between the intensity of “normal” fear and anxiety responses and those that characterize anxiety disorders.
- Published
- 2011
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
6. Research into anxiety of childhood: playing catch-up (to Olympic standard)
- Author
-
Andy P. Field, Cathy Creswell, Sam Cartwright-Hatton, and Shirley Reynolds
- Subjects
Clinical Psychology ,Medical education ,medicine ,Anxiety ,General Medicine ,Sister ,medicine.symptom ,Psychology ,Developmental psychology - Abstract
This special issue is the culmination of an ESRC seminar series grant awarded to the authors of this editorial. We named the seminar series CATTS (Child Anxiety, Theory and Treatment Seminars) and it took the form of six highly stimulating, one-day seminars on the subject of child anxiety, with participants from clinical and academic backgrounds and from Great Britain, Europe, the USA and Australia. Most of the authors in this publication, and a sister special issue in Cognitions and Emotion (2008), participated in the CATTS series.
- Published
- 2008
7. Conditioning models of childhood anxiety
- Author
-
Andy P. Field and Graham C. L. Davey
- Subjects
Phobias ,Childhood development ,Rapid rate ,medicine ,Childhood anxiety ,Normative ,Classical conditioning ,Anxiety ,medicine.symptom ,medicine.disease ,Psychology ,Developmental psychology ,Agoraphobia - Abstract
Little miss Muffet, sat on a tuffet – Eating her curds and whey. Down came a spider, that sat down beside her and frightened miss Muffet away Children seem particularly prone to fear, so much so that fear has been seen as a normal part of childhood development. It is documented in the research into childhood fear that during infancy children tend to fear stimuli within their immediate environment such as loud noise, objects and separation from a caretaker, but that as the child matures these fears adjust to incorporate anticipatory events and abstract stimuli (Campbell, 1986). Recent work has indicated that general fearfulness decreases as age increases and that this decrease continues at a fairly rapid rate until the beginning of adolescence (Gullone & King, 1997). Mild fears in children often appear and disappear spontaneously and follow a predictable course. For example, Bauer (1976) reported that younger children (4–8 years old) typically fear ghosts and animals whilst older children (10–12 years) are more likely to fear self-injury. These short-lived fears are part of a normal pattern of development, frequently have an obvious adaptive significance, and reflect the everyday experiences of the child. These normative fears are at their highest during the first 11–14 years of life but then stabilize, leaving only pervasive fears and phobias (Draper & James, 1985; Gullone & King, 1997). A phobia is a fear that is out of proportion to the demands of the situation that evokes it, it cannot be rationalized, is involuntary, and leads to avoidance of the situation (Marks, 1969).
- Published
- 2000
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
8. A Neglectful Parenting Style Moderates the Effect of the Verbal Threat Information Pathway on Children's Heart Rate Responses to Novel Animals.
- Author
-
Khanya Price-Evans and Andy P. Field
- Subjects
- *
ANXIETY , *CHILDREN , *PARENTING , *CHILD rearing , *PARENT-child relationships - Abstract
AbstractParenting styles are associated with anxiety in children. Part of this association can be explained by how parenting interacts with known pathways to anxiety. Although parenting interacts with the verbal threat information pathway to create anxiogenic cognitions in children, it is not known whether parenting styles mediate the physiological component of the anxiety emotion. An experiment is reported in which 6?10-year-old children (N= 54) completed parenting styles, and anxiety beliefs questionnaires. They were then given threat, positive or no verbal information about three novel animals before being asked to place their hands in boxes they believed these animals inhabited. Their average heart rate during the approach was recorded. The results suggest that a neglectful maternal parenting style mediates the effect that verbal threat information has on physiological responses. However, a punitive maternal parenting style, maternal warmth, overprotection, and accurate monitoring were not found to have a significant effect. Paternal parenting styles were not found to have any significant effect. This experiment adds to the existing evidence demonstrating that parenting practices can mediate components of acquired anxiety emotions. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2008
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
9. What Happens When Verbal Threat Information and Vicarious Learning Combine?
- Author
-
Chris Askew, Hannah Kessock-Philip, and Andy P. Field
- Subjects
RESEARCH ,FEAR ,COGNITION ,CHILDREN ,LEARNING - Abstract
AbstractRecent research has shown that the verbal information and vicarious learning pathways to fear create long term fear cognitions and can create cognitive biases and avoidance in children. However, it is unlikely that these pathways operate in isolation in the aetiology of childhood fear and the interaction between these pathways is untested. Three preliminary experiments are reported that explore the combined effect of verbal threat information and vicarious learning on self-reported fear beliefs in 7?9-year-old children. Results showed that prior negative information significantly facilitated the effect of negative vicarious learning on children's fear beliefs (Experiment 1); however, there was not a significant combined effect of verbal threat information and vicarious learning when they the information was presented during (Experiment 2) or after (Experiment 3) vicarious learning. These results support the idea that verbal information can affect CS-US associations formed in subsequent vicarious learning events, but contradict the proposal that it can change fear beliefs already acquired through vicarious learning by changing how a person evaluates the vicarious learning episode. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2008
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
10. Parent-Child Relationships and the Verbal Information Pathway to Fear in Children: Two Preliminary Experiments.
- Author
-
Andy P. Field, Jess E. Ball, Nicola J. Kawycz, and Harriett Moore
- Subjects
- *
PARENT-child relationships , *PARENTING , *FEAR in children , *ANXIETY in children , *INTERPERSONAL relations , *CHILD psychology - Abstract
Parenting styles and the parent-child bond are associated with anxiety in children. Despite this association, little is known about the mechanism through which parenting has its effect. One possibility is that parenting interacts with other known pathways to fear. Two experiments are reported that look at the interaction between parenting styles and parent-child relationships and the verbal information pathway to fear. In Experiment 1, a punitive maternal parenting style was found to interact with the effect of threat information about a novel animal in 6–9-year-old children. Maternal warmth, neglect, overprotection and accurate monitoring were not found to have a significant effect. Experiment 2 showed that children reporting a greater number of negative interactions with their fathers had greater increases in fear beliefs about novel animals after both threat and no information. The quality of mother-child relationships did not significantly interact with the verbal information pathway. These experiments offer preliminary evidence that parenting practices influence how children react to negative information, which offers some insight into the potential causes of the association between parenting and anxiety in children. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2007
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.