10 results on '"Colasante, Tyler"'
Search Results
2. Bidirectional Associations of Adolescents’ Momentary Social Media Use and Negative Emotions
- Author
-
Colasante, Tyler, Faulkner, Katie, Kharbotli, Dana, Malti, Tina, and Hollenstein, Tom
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
3. Kindness for Peace
- Author
-
Gibhardt, Sina, Colasante, Tyler, Malti, Tina, Christie, Daniel J., Series Editor, and Niemiec, Ryan M., editor
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
4. Play My Math: Second Development Cycle of an EdTech Tool Supporting the Teaching and Learning of Fractions Through Music in Algebraic Notation
- Author
-
Roldan-Roa, Eric, Roldan-Roa, Érika B., Raave, Doris Kristina, van Herwegen, Jo, Polytimou, Nina, Mukherjee, Sayan, Colasante, Tyler, Malti, Tina, Mori, Julia, Goos, Gerhard, Series Editor, Hartmanis, Juris, Founding Editor, Bertino, Elisa, Editorial Board Member, Gao, Wen, Editorial Board Member, Steffen, Bernhard, Editorial Board Member, Yung, Moti, Editorial Board Member, Cheng, Yu-Ping, editor, Pedaste, Margus, editor, Bardone, Emanuele, editor, and Huang, Yueh-Min, editor
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
5. Physiological Dysregulation in Children With and Without Externalizing Difficulties: Novel Insights From Intensive Longitudinal Data
- Author
-
Kil, Hali, Sibalis, Annabel, Colasante, Tyler, Jambon, Marc, Acland, Erinn, Suri, Anjali, Malti, Tina, and Andrade, Brendan F.
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
6. Kindness
- Author
-
Malti, Tina, primary and Colasante, Tyler, additional
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
7. Fostering Prosociality in Refugee Children: An Intervention With Rohingya Children.
- Author
-
Callaghan, Tara, Colasante, Tyler, Muhammad, Saifullah, Corbit, John, Yavuz‐Muren, Melis, Raffaele, Charles, Akter, Rozina, Al Janaideh, Redab, Duan, Tz‐Yu, Didkowsky, Nora, Beuze, Jean‐Nicolas, Homer, Bruce, Cameron, Catherine Ann, and Malti, Tina
- Subjects
- *
PSYCHOLOGICAL resilience , *REFUGEE camps , *RESEARCH funding , *PSYCHOLOGY of refugees , *SOCIAL skills , *BIRTHPLACES , *CHILD behavior - Abstract
Prosocial behavior is a distinguishing characteristic of human nature. Although prosocial behaviors emerge early in development, contextual factors play an important role in how these behaviors are manifested over development. A large body of research focuses on the trajectory of prosocial development across diverse cultures and investigating contexts that foster it. Against this backdrop of developmental research endeavoring to understand and enhance the cooperative side of humanity, is the catastrophic impact of profoundly negative forces on social‐emotional development for children forced to flee from violent conflict. Close to half a million Rohingya children, whose families were forced to flee genocide in Myanmar, now live in the largest refugee camp in the world. To examine the resilience of human prosociality in the face of extreme adversity, we documented initial levels of prosociality in Rohingya refugee children living in a mega‐camp (Cox's Bazar, Bangladesh) and the extent to which those levels were improved following a multifaceted intervention designed to foster prosociality. The research was a partnership between Rohingya community members with lived experience, humanitarian practitioners, and developmental researchers. A sample of 152 Rohingya children (5–12 years) participated in pre‐ and postintervention assessments of prosocial behaviors and related cognitive‐affective processes. The 10‐day collaboration‐based intervention was implemented between November 2021 and January 2022 by Rohingya researchers. Birthplace was used as a proxy measure of trauma level. Children born in Myanmar (N = 88) directly experienced relatively higher levels of trauma (genocide, forced migration) than children who were born in the camp after their families fled from Myanmar (N = 64). Children were individually tested pre‐ and postintervention with a task battery, including a helping (Origami) and two sharing tasks (Dictator Game [DG], Forced Choice sharing) measuring prosocial behavior. Assessments of related cognitive‐affective processes included measures of empathic responding and emotion perspective‐taking in story tasks (Imagine, Judgment) and executive function (EF) skills (Younger: Hearts & Flowers; Older: Dimensional Change Card Sorting). Small group intervention sessions conducted over 10 days targeted these prosocial behaviors and cognitive‐affective processes and were based on collaborative activities, emotion perspective taking and EF skills training with the same partner throughout the intervention phase. We used latent change modeling to examine initial levels (preintervention) and intervention‐related changes in these measures from pre‐ to postintervention. Prosocial responding was found across all measures (preintervention) and improvements (pre‐ to postintervention change) were apparent across most measures. Age and birthplace variables were significant predictors of initial levels and intervention‐related change. Initial levels: Regarding age, older children (9–12 years) showed higher levels than younger children (5–8 years) of sharing in the Forced Choice task but lower levels in the DG. Older children also showed higher levels of empathic responding when asked to report how they would feel and respond to another person's misfortune in the Imagine task. Regarding birthplace, prior to the intervention camp‐born children showed higher levels than Myanmar‐born children of helping in the Origami task and reported more behavioral responses indicating how they would respond to misfortune in the Imagine task. In contrast, Myanmar‐born children had higher levels of sharing in the DG and consistently chose equality over inequality in the Forced Choice sharing task, even when their partner would receive more, indicating a pattern of generosity in these children. Myanmar‐born children had lower levels than camp‐born children on EF measures. Intervention‐related change: Regarding age, older but not younger children were more likely to increase choices for equality over inequality on the Forced Choice sharing task following the intervention. Regarding birthplace and helping, camp‐born children increased behaviors that helped their partner make origami shapes themselves ("how‐to" helping), whereas Myanmar‐born children increased behavior that took over folding for their partner ("do‐for" helping). For sharing tasks, Myanmar‐born but not camp‐born children increased sharing in the DG and showed an increased pattern of generosity in Forced Choice sharing task. In the Imagine story task, children born in Myanmar were more likely than those born in camp to increase empathic responding (i.e., imagining how they would feel). Children born in Myanmar showed less improvement on EF measures than children born in the camp. Taken together, these findings provide evidence that in a context of extreme adversity, Rohingya children exhibited prosociality and benefitted from a multifaceted intervention. Our research adds credence to the view that human prosociality is a fundamental characteristic of humanity that not only survives but can be enhanced in even the most adverse of childhood environments. Our multifaceted intervention, which was implemented within a collaborative social context and targeted prosocial behaviors and related cognitive‐affective processes, was designed to be easily implemented within existing psychosocial support programs in refugee contexts. As the numbers of children affected by violent conflict and forced migration rise alarmingly worldwide, there is a critical need to expand research partnerships that aim to improve developmental outcomes for these millions of children. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
8. Biological basis of temperament: Respiratory sinus arrhythmia and inhibitory control across childhood.
- Author
-
Yavuz, H. Melis, primary, Galarneau, Emma, additional, Speidel, Ruth, additional, Colasante, Tyler, additional, and Malti, Tina, additional
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
9. Machiavellian Behavior and Social–Emotional Functioning in Middle Childhood and Early Adolescence.
- Author
-
Jambon, Marc, Colasante, Tyler, and Malti, Tina
- Subjects
- *
HEALTH literacy , *RECOGNITION (Psychology) , *STATISTICAL correlation , *RISK assessment , *VIOLENCE , *RESEARCH funding , *PARENT-child relationships , *SKIN physiology , *ANGER , *MANIPULATIVE behavior , *EMOTIONS , *SELF-control , *PARENT attitudes , *DESCRIPTIVE statistics , *AGGRESSION (Psychology) , *LONGITUDINAL method , *CAREGIVERS , *SOCIAL skills , *GUILT (Psychology) , *RESEARCH , *INTERPERSONAL relations , *PSYCHOSOCIAL functioning , *ANTISOCIAL personality disorders , *ADOLESCENCE , *CHILDREN - Abstract
Machiavellianism is an antisocial interpersonal style involving the use of manipulative, deceptive, and coercive behaviors in the pursuit of self-interest. Although widely studied as a "dark" personality trait in adults, relatively little is known about the developmental correlates of Machiavellian tendencies earlier in life. The present study addressed this knowledge gap by examining associations between Machiavellian behavior and three theoretically relevant social–emotional domains—prosocial emotions, emotion recognition skills, and self-control—in a community sample of 7- and 11-year-old Canadian children (N = 300, 50% female). Of particular interest was the extent to which individual differences in social–emotional capacities were uniquely associated with Machiavellian behavior after controlling for aggression, a relatively well-studied antisocial behavior in childhood. Parents reported on children's Machiavellian behavior and aggression. Social–emotional correlates were assessed via parent and child reports, behavioral tasks, and physiological assessments. Machiavellian behavior and aggression were similarly associated with lower parent-reported prosocial emotions and self-control. Machiavellian (but not aggressive) behavior was associated with lower child-reported guilt (a prosocial emotion), higher resting skin conductance levels (reflecting a general tendency for higher arousal), and better anger recognition. Semipartial correlational analyses indicated that these patterns of association with Machiavellian behaviors held independent of aggression. These findings indicate that, by middle childhood, Machiavellian behavior constitutes a distinct form of antisocial conduct that is associated with a unique social–emotional risk profile. Public Significance Statement: By middle childhood, Machiavellian behaviors such as manipulation, deception, and coercion are distinct from aggression in terms of their occurrence and associations with other domains of social–emotional functioning (e.g., prosocial emotions, emotion recognition ability, self-control). This suggests that traditional prevention and intervention programs targeting aggressive and violent tendencies may be less effective at curbing more subtle and self-serving forms of antisocial conduct. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
10. Empathy, Sympathy, and Emotion Regulation: A Meta-Analytic Review.
- Author
-
Yavuz, H. Melis, Colasante, Tyler, Galarneau, Emma, and Malti, Tina
- Subjects
- *
EMPATHY , *EMOTION regulation , *SYMPATHY , *SINUS arrhythmia , *RESEARCH personnel - Abstract
Empathy, sympathy, and emotion regulation are core components of social–emotional development. Regulating vicariously induced negative emotions is thought to support feeling empathy and sympathy for others in need, but empirical evidence for such effects is mixed. Moreover, despite the longstanding conceptual distinction between empathy and sympathy, most researchers refer to and measure these constructs interchangeably. This meta-analysis examined associations between emotion regulation and empathy and/or sympathy in childhood and adolescence, as well as potential methodological, study, and sample moderators. Analyses were conducted on 58 studies (75 effect sizes; N = 25,831). There was a significant and positive overall association between emotion regulation and empathy/sympathy (r =.19, p <.001); however, most scales assessing empathy were conflated with other constructs. Accounting for conflation, emotion regulation was related to sympathy (r =.24, p <.001) but not empathy (r =.04, p =.38). Moreover, the association between emotion regulation and empathy/sympathy was not significant when baseline respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA) and RSA reactivity were examined as proxies of emotion regulation. Results were largely similar across sample characteristics (e.g., age group, sex, and culture). Conceptual implications for differentiating empathy and sympathy are discussed, as well as methodological considerations to maximize the quality and clarity of research on emotion regulation, empathy, and sympathy. Public Significance Statement: Empathy, sympathy, and emotion regulation are regarded as interrelated cornerstones of social–emotional development. This meta-analysis shows that higher emotion regulation is associated with more sympathy for others in need but not with more empathic sharing of others' emotions. It provides substantial empirical support for differentiating empathy from sympathy to better understand and promote social–emotional capacities in children and adolescents. Overall, the results imply that the two pillars of social–emotional development (emotion regulation and sympathy) are interrelated, and emotion regulation might be a key factor to promote sympathy (instead of empathy) in childhood and adolescence. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.