30 results on '"Bundesen, C."'
Search Results
2. Theory of Visual Attention (TVA) applied to mice in the 5-choice serial reaction time task
- Author
-
Fitzpatrick, C. M., Caballero-Puntiverio, M., Gether, U., Habekost, T., Bundesen, C., Vangkilde, S., Woldbye, D. P. D., Andreasen, J. T., and Petersen, A.
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
3. Phasic alerting increases visual attention capacity in younger but not in older individuals
- Author
-
Wiegand, I.M., Petersen, A., Bundesen, C., Habekost, T., Wiegand, I.M., Petersen, A., Bundesen, C., and Habekost, T.
- Abstract
Contains fulltext : 221774.pdf (publisher's version ) (Open Access), In the present study, we investigated effects of phasic alerting on visual attention in younger and older adults. We modelled parameters of visual attention based on the computational Theory of Visual Attention (TVA) and measured event-related lateralizations (ERLs) in a partial report task, in which half of the displays were preceded by an auditory warning cue. Younger adults showed an alertness-related visual processing facilitation: TVA parameter sensory effectiveness a, a measure of visual processing capacity, was significantly increased, and latencies of visual ERLs were significantly reduced following the warning cue. By contrast, older adults did not benefit from the alerting cue: TVA parameter sensory effectiveness a and ERL latencies did not differ between conditions with and without cues. The findings indicate age-related changes in the brain network underlying alertness and attention, which governs the responsiveness to external cues and is critical for general cognitive functioning in aging.
- Published
- 2017
4. Behavioral and brain measures of phasic alerting effects on visual attention
- Author
-
Wiegand, I.M., Petersen, A., Finke, K., Bundesen, C., Lansner, J., Habekost, T., Wiegand, I.M., Petersen, A., Finke, K., Bundesen, C., Lansner, J., and Habekost, T.
- Abstract
Contains fulltext : 221773.pdf (publisher's version ) (Open Access), In the present study, we investigated effects of phasic alerting on visual attention in a partial report task, in which half of the displays were preceded by an auditory warning cue. Based on the computational Theory of Visual Attention (TVA), we estimated parameters of spatial and non-spatial aspects of visual attention and measured event-related lateralizations (ERLs) over visual processing areas. We found that the TVA parameter sensory effectiveness a, which is thought to reflect visual processing capacity, significantly increased with phasic alerting. By contrast, the distribution of visual processing resources according to task relevance and spatial position, as quantified in parameters top-down control alpha and spatial bias w(index), was not modulated by phasic alerting. On the electrophysiological level, the latencies of ERLs in response to the task displays were reduced following the warning cue. These results suggest that phasic alerting facilitates visual processing in a general, unselective manner and that this effect originates in early stages of visual information processing.
- Published
- 2017
5. Health-related quality of life in older patients surviving ICU treatment for COVID-19: results from an international observational study of patients older than 70 years
- Author
-
Soliman, Ivo, Leaver, Susannah, Flaatten, Hans, Fjølner, Jesper, Wernly, Bernhard, Bruno, Raphael, Artigas, Antonio, Bollen Pinto, Bernardo, Schefold, Joerg, Beil, Michael, Sviri, Sigal, van Heerden, Peter Vernon, Szczeklik, Wojciech, Elhadi, Muhammed, Joannidis, Michael, Oeyen, Sandra, Zafeiridis, Tilemachos, Wollborn, Jakob, Banzo, Maria Jose Arche, Fuest, Kristina, Marsh, Brian, Andersen, Finn, Moreno, Rui, Boumendil, Ariane, Guidet, Bertrand, Jung, Christian, de Lange, Dylan, Eller, Philipp, Mesotten, Dieter, Reper, Pascal, Swinnen, Walter, Brix, Helene, Brushoej, Jens, Villefrance, Maja, Nedergaard, Helene Korvenius, Bjerregaard, Anders Thais, Balleby, Ida Riise, Andersen, Kasper, Hansen, Maria Aagaard, Uhrenholt, Stine, Bundgaard, Helle, Mohamed, Aliae Ar Hussein, Salah, Rehab, Ali, Yasmin Khairy Nasreldin Mohamed, Wassim, Kyrillos, Elgazzar, Yumna, Tharwat, Samar, Azzam, Ahmed, Habib, Ayman Abdelmawgoad, Abosheaishaa, Hazem Maarouf, Azab, Mohammed, Galbois, Arnaud, Charron, Cyril, Guerot, Emmanuel, Besch, Guillaume, Rigaud, Jean-Philippe, Maizel, Julien, Djibré, Michel, Burtin, Philippe, Garcon, Pierre, Nseir, Saad, Valette, Xavier, Alexandru, Nica, Marin, Nathalie, Vaissiere, Marie, Plantefeve, Gaëtan, Vanderlinden, Thierry, Jurcisin, Igor, Megarbane, Bruno, Caillard, Anais, Valent, Arnaud, Garnier, Marc, Besset, Sebastien, Oziel, Johanna, Raphaelen, Jean-Herlé, Dauger, Stéphane, Dumas, Guillaume, Goncalves, Bruno, Piton, Gaël, Bruno, Raphael Romano, Kelm, Malte, Wolff, Georg, Barth, Eberhard, Goebel, Ulrich, Kunstein, Anselm, Schuster, Michael, Welte, Martin, Lutz, Matthias, Meybohm, Patrick, Steiner, Stephan, Poerner, Tudor, Haake, Hendrik, Schaller, Stefan, Kindgen-Milles, Detlef, Meyer, Christian, Kurt, Muhammed, Kuhn, Karl Friedrich, Randerath, Winfried, Dindane, Zouhir, Kabitz, Hans-Joachim, Voigt, Ingo, Shala, Gonxhe, Faltlhauser, Andreas, Rovina, Nikoletta, Aidoni, Zoi, Chrisanthopoulou, Evangelia, Papadogoulas, Antonios, Gurjar, Mohan, Mahmoodpoor, Ata, Ahmed, Abdullah Khudhur, Elsaka, Ahmed, Comellini, Vittoria, Rabha, Ahmed, Ahmed, Hazem, Namendys-Silva, Silvio, Ghannam, Abdelilah, Groenendijk, Martijn, Zegers, Marieke, Cornet, Alex, Evers, Mirjam, Haas, Lenneke, Dormans, Tom, Dieperink, Willem, Romundstad, Luis, Sjøbø, Britt, Strietzel, Hans Frank, Olasveengen, Theresa, Hahn, Michael, Czuczwar, Miroslaw, Gawda, Ryszard, Klimkiewicz, Jakub, de Lurdessantos, Maria Campos, Gordinho, André, Santos, Henrique, Assis, Rui, Oliveira, Ana Isabel Pinho, Badawy, Mohamed Raafat, Perez-Torres, David, Gomà, Gemma, Villamayor, Mercedes Ibarz, Mira, Angela Prado, Cubero, Patricia Jimeno, Rivera, Susana Arias, Tomasa, Teresa, Iglesias, David, Vázquez, Eric Mayor, Aldecoa, Cesar, Ferreira, Aida Fernández, Zalba-Etayo, Begoña, Canas-Perez, Isabel, Tamayo-Lomas, Luis, Diaz-Rodriguez, Cristina, Sancho, Susana, Priego, Jesús, Abualqumboz, Enas, Hilles, Momin Majed Yousuf, Saleh, Mahmoud, Ben-Hamouda, Nawfel, Roberti, Andrea, Dullenkopf, Alexander, Fleury, Yvan, Pinto, Bernardo Bollen, Al-Sadawi, Mohammed, Serck, Nicolas, Dewaele, Elisabeth, Kumar, Pritpal, Bundesen, Camilla, Innes, Richard, Gooch, James, Cagova, Lenka, Potter, Elizabeth, Reay, Michael, Davey, Miriam, Humphreys, Sally, Berlemont, Caroline Hauw, Chousterman, Benjamin Glenn, Dépret, François, Ferre, Alexis, Vettoretti, Lucie, Thevenin, Didier, Milovanovic, Milena, Simon, Philipp, Lorenz, Marco, Stoll, Sandra Emily, Dubler, Simon, Mulita, Francesk, Kondili, Eumorifa, Andrianopoulos, Ioannis, Meynaar, Iwan, Cornet, Alexander Daniel, Sjøbøe, Britt, Kluzik, Anna, Zatorski, Paweł, Drygalski, Tomasz, Solek-Pastuszka, Joanna, Onichimowski, Dariusz, Stefaniak, Jan, Stefanska-Wronka, Karina, Zabul, Ewa, Cardoso, Filipe Sousa, Banzo, Maria José Arche, Tomasa-Irriguible, Teresa Maria, Mira, Ángela Prado, Arias-Rivera, Susana, Frutos-Vivar, Fernando, Lopez-Cuenca, Sonia, de Gopegui, Pablo Ruiz, Abidi, Nour, Chau, Ivan, Pugh, Richard, Smuts, Sara, Institut Pierre Louis d'Epidémiologie et de Santé Publique (iPLESP), Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM)-Sorbonne Université (SU), Service de Réanimation Médicale [CHU Saint-Antoine], Assistance publique - Hôpitaux de Paris (AP-HP) (AP-HP)-CHU Saint-Antoine [AP-HP], Assistance publique - Hôpitaux de Paris (AP-HP) (AP-HP)-Sorbonne Université (SU)-Sorbonne Université (SU), Mécanismes physiopathologiques et conséquences des calcifications vasculaires - UR UPJV 7517 (MP3CV), Université de Picardie Jules Verne (UPJV)-CHU Amiens-Picardie, Optimisation thérapeutique en Neuropsychopharmacologie (OPTeN (UMR_S_1144 / U1144)), Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale (INSERM)-Université Paris Cité (UPCité), Bouchard, Mélanie, Critical care, Anesthesiology, Peri-operative and Emergency medicine (CAPE), COVIP-study group, Eller, P., Joannidis, M., Mesotten, D., Reper, P., Oeyen, S., Swinnen, W., Brix, H., Brushoej, J., Villefrance, M., Nedergaard, H.K., Bjerregaard, A.T., Balleby, I.R., Andersen, K., Hansen, M.A., Uhrenholt, S., Bundgaard, H., Fjølner, J., Mohamed, AAH, Salah, R., Ali, YKNM, Wassim, K., Elgazzar, Y.A., Tharwat, S., Azzam, A.Y., Habib, A.A., Abosheaishaa, H.M., Azab, M.A., Leaver, S., Galbois, A., Guidet, B., Charron, C., Guerot, E., Besch, G., Rigaud, J.P., Maizel, J., Djibré, M., Burtin, P., Garcon, P., Nseir, S., Valette, X., Alexandru, N., Marin, N., Vaissiere, M., Plantefeve, G., Vanderlinden, T., Jurcisin, I., Megarbane, B., Caillard, A., Valent, A., Garnier, M., Besset, S., Oziel, J., Raphaelen, J.H., Dauger, S., Dumas, G., Goncalves, B., Piton, G., Jung, C., Bruno, R.R., Kelm, M., Wolff, G., Barth, E., Goebel, U., Kunstein, A., Schuster, M., Welte, M., Lutz, M., Meybohm, P., Steiner, S., Poerner, T., Haake, H., Schaller, S., Kindgen-Milles, D., Meyer, C., Kurt, M., Kuhn, K.F., Randerath, W., Wollborn, J., Dindane, Z., Kabitz, H.J., Voigt, I., Shala, G., Faltlhauser, A., Rovina, N., Aidoni, Z., Chrisanthopoulou, E., Papadogoulas, A., Gurjar, M., Mahmoodpoor, A., Ahmed, A.K., Marsh, B., Elsaka, A., Sviri, S., Comellini, V., Rabha, A., Ahmed, H., Namendys-Silva, S.A., Ghannam, A., Groenendijk, M., Zegers, M., de Lange, D., Cornet, A., Evers, M., Haas, L., Dormans, T., Dieperink, W., Romundstad, L., Sjøbø, B., Andersen, F.H., Strietzel, H.F., Olasveengen, T., Hahn, M., Czuczwar, M., Gawda, R., Klimkiewicz, J., de LurdesSantos, M.C., Gordinho, A., Santos, H., Assis, R., Oliveira, AIP, Badawy, M.R., Perez-Torres, D., Gomà, G., Villamayor, M.I., Mira, A.P., Cubero, P.J., Rivera, S.A., Tomasa, T., Iglesias, D., Vázquez, E.M., Aldecoa, C., Ferreira, A.F., Zalba-Etayo, B., Canas-Perez, I., Tamayo-Lomas, L., Diaz-Rodriguez, C., Sancho, S., Priego, J., Abualqumboz, EMY, Hilles, MMY, Saleh, M., Ben-HAmouda, N., Roberti, A., Dullenkopf, A., Fleury, Y., Pinto, B.B., Schefold, J.C., Al-Sadawi, M., Serck, N., Dewaele, E., Kumar, P., Bundesen, C., Innes, R., Gooch, J., Cagova, L., Potter, E., Reay, M., Davey, M., Humphreys, S., Berlemont, C.H., Chousterman, B.G., Dépret, F., Ferre, A., Vettoretti, L., Thevenin, D., Milovanovic, M., Simon, P., Lorenz, M., Stoll, S.E., Dubler, S., Fuest, K., Mulita, F., Kondili, E., Andrianopoulos, I., Meynaar, I., Cornet, A.D., Sjøbøe, B., Kluzik, A., Zatorski, P., Drygalski, T., Szczeklik, W., Solek-Pastuszka, J., Onichimowski, D., Stefaniak, J., Stefanska-Wronka, K., Zabul, E., Cardoso, F.S., Banzo, MJA, Tomasa-Irriguible, T.M., Mira, Á.P., Arias-Rivera, S., Frutos-Vivar, F., Lopez-Cuenca, S., de Gopegui, P.R., Abidi, N., Chau, I., Pugh, R., and Smuts, S.
- Subjects
Aging ,Activities of Daily Living ,Aged ,COVID-19 ,Humans ,Intensive Care Units ,Prospective Studies ,Quality of Life ,SARS-CoV-2 ,Intensive Care Unit (ICU) ,Older people ,Survival ,frailty ,[SDV]Life Sciences [q-bio] ,SOCIETY ,610 Medicine & health ,General Medicine ,INTENSIVE-CARE ,humanities ,Healthcare improvement science Radboud Institute for Health Sciences [Radboudumc 18] ,[SDV] Life Sciences [q-bio] ,Medicine and Health Sciences ,Geriatrics and Gerontology - Abstract
Background health-related quality of life (HRQoL) is an important patient-centred outcome in patients surviving ICU admission for COVID-19. It is currently not clear which domains of the HRQoL are most affected. Objective to quantify HRQoL in order to identify areas of interventions. Design prospective observation study. Setting admissions to European ICUs between March 2020 and February 2021. Subjects patients aged 70 years or older admitted with COVID-19 disease. Methods collected determinants include SOFA-score, Clinical Frailty Scale (CFS), number and timing of ICU procedures and limitation of care, Katz Activities of Daily Living (ADL) dependence score. HRQoL was assessed at 3 months after ICU admission with the Euro-QoL-5D-5L questionnaire. An outcome of ≥4 on any of Euro-QoL-5D-5L domains was considered unfavourable. Results in total 3,140 patients from 14 European countries were included in this study. Three months after inclusion, 1,224 patients (39.0%) were alive and the EQ-5D-5L from was obtained. The CFS was associated with an increased odds ratio for an unfavourable HRQoL outcome after 3 months; OR 1.15 (95% confidence interval (CI): 0.71–1.87) for CFS 2 to OR 4.33 (95% CI: 1.57–11.9) for CFS ≧ 7. The Katz ADL was not statistically significantly associated with HRQoL after 3 months. Conclusions in critically ill old intensive care patients suffering from COVID-19, the CFS is associated with the subjectively perceived quality of life. The CFS on admission can be used to inform patients and relatives on the risk of an unfavourable qualitative outcome if such patients survive.
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
6. The association of the Activities of Daily Living and the outcome of old intensive care patients suffering from COVID-19
- Author
-
Bruno, Raphael Romano, Wernly, Bernhard, Flaatten, Hans, Fjølner, Jesper, Artigas, Antonio, Baldia, Philipp Heinrich, Binneboessel, Stephan, Bollen Pinto, Bernardo, Schefold, Joerg C., Wolff, Georg, Kelm, Malte, Beil, Michael, Sviri, Sigal, van Heerden, Peter Vernon, Szczeklik, Wojciech, Elhadi, Muhammed, Joannidis, Michael, Oeyen, Sandra, Kondili, Eumorfia, Marsh, Brian, Wollborn, Jakob, Andersen, Finn H., Moreno, Rui, Leaver, Susannah, Boumendil, Ariane, De Lange, Dylan W., Guidet, Bertrand, Jung, Christian, Eller, Philipp, Mesotten, Dieter, Reper, Pascal, Swinnen, Walter, Serck, Nicolas, Dewaele, Elisabeth, Brix, Helene, Brushoej, Jens, Kumar, Pritpal, Nedergaard, Helene Korvenius, Balleby, Ida Riise, Bundesen, Camilla, Hansen, Maria Aagaard, Uhrenholt, Stine, Bundgaard, Helle, Gooch, James, Cagova, Lenka, Potter, Elizabeth, Reay, Michael, Davey, Miriam, Abusayed, Mohammed Abdelshafy, Humphreys, Sally, Galbois, Arnaud, Charron, Cyril, Berlemont, Caroline Hauw, Besch, Guillaume, Rigaud, Jean-Philippe, Maizel, Julien, Djibré, Michel, Burtin, Philippe, Garcon, Pierre, Nseir, Saad, Valette, Xavier, Alexandru, Nica, Marin, Nathalie, Vaissiere, Marie, Plantefeve, Gaëtan, Vanderlinden, Thierry, Jurcisin, Igor, Megarbane, Buno, Chousterman, Benjamin Glenn, Dépret, François, Garnier, Marc, Besset, Sebastien, Oziel, Johanna, Ferre, Alexis, Dauger, Stéphane, Dumas, Guillaume, Goncalves, Bruno, Vettoretti, Lucie, Thevenin, Didier, Schaller, Stefan, Kurt, Muhammed, Faltlhauser, Andreas, Meyer, Christian, Milovanovic, Milena, Lutz, Matthias, Shala, Gonxhe, Haake, Hendrik, Randerath, Winfried, Kunstein, Anselm, Meybohm, Patrick, Steiner, Stephan, Barth, Eberhard, Poerner, Tudor, Simon, Philipp, Lorenz, Marco, Dindane, Zouhir, Kuhn, Karl Friedrich, Welte, Martin, Voigt, Ingo, Kabitz, Hans-Joachim, Goebel, Ulrich, Stoll, Sandra Emily, Kindgen-Milles, Detlef, Dubler, Simon, Fuest, Kristina, Schuster, Michael, Papadogoulas, Antonios, Mulita, Francesk, Rovina, Nikoletta, Aidoni, Zoi, Chrisanthopoulou, Evangelia, Andrianopoulos, Ioannis, Groenendijk, Martijn, Evers, Mirjam, van Lelyveld-Haas, Lenneke, Meynaar, Iwan, Cornet, Alexander Daniel, Zegers, Marieke, Dieperink, Willem, Dormans, Tom, Hahn, Michael, Sjøbøe, Britt, Strietzel, Hans Frank, Olasveengen, Theresa, Romundstad, Luis, Kluzik, Anna, Zatorski, Paweł, Drygalski, Tomasz, Klimkiewicz, Jakub, Solek-pastuszka, Joanna, Onichimowski, Dariusz, Czuczwar, Miroslaw, Gawda, Ryszard, Stefaniak, Jan, Stefanska-Wronka, Karina, Zabul, Ewa, Oliveira, Ana Isabel Pinho, Assis, Rui, de Lurdes Campos Santos, Maria, Santos, Henrique, Cardoso, Filipe Sousa, Gordinho, André, Banzo, Maria José Arche, Zalba-Etayo, Begoña, Cubero, Patricia Patricia, Priego, Jesús, Gomà, Gemma, Tomasa-Irriguible, Teresa Maria, Sancho, Susana, Ferreira, Aida Fernández, Vázquez, Eric Mayor, Mira, Ángela Prado, Ibarz, Mercedes, Iglesias, David, Arias-Rivera, Susana, Frutos-Vivar, Fernando, Lopez-Cuenca, Sonia, Aldecoa, Cesar, Perez-Torres, David, Canas-Perez, Isabel, Tamayo-Lomas, Luis, Diaz-Rodriguez, Cristina, de Gopegui, Pablo Ruiz, Ben-Hamouda, Nawfel, Roberti, Andrea, Fleury, Yvan, Abidi, Nour, Chau, Ivan, Dullenkopf, Alexander, Pugh, Richard, Smuts, Sara, COVIP study group, [missing], Critical care, Anesthesiology, Peri-operative and Emergency medicine (CAPE), NOVA Medical School|Faculdade de Ciências Médicas (NMS|FCM), COVIP study group, Eller, P., Joannidis, M., Mesotten, D., Reper, P., Oeyen, S., Swinnen, W., Serck, N., Dewaele, E., Brix, H., Brushoej, J., Kumar, P., Nedergaard, H.K., Balleby, I.R., Bundesen, C., Hansen, M.A., Uhrenholt, S., Bundgaard, H., Fjølner, J., Gooch, J., Cagova, L., Potter, E., Reay, M., Davey, M., Abusayed, M.A., Humphreys, S., Galbois, A., Guidet, B., Charron, C., Berlemont, C.H., Besch, G., Rigaud, J.P., Maizel, J., Djibré, M., Burtin, P., Garcon, P., Nseir, S., Valette, X., Alexandru, N., Marin, N., Vaissiere, M., Plantefeve, G., Vanderlinden, T., Jurcisin, I., Megarbane, B., Chousterman, B.G., Dépret, F., Garnier, M., Besset, S., Oziel, J., Ferre, A., Dauger, S., Dumas, G., Goncalves, B., Vettoretti, L., Thevenin, D., Schaller, S., Kurt, M., Faltlhauser, A., Meyer, C., Milovanovic, M., Lutz, M., Shala, G., Haake, H., Randerath, W., Kunstein, A., Meybohm, P., Steiner, S., Barth, E., Poerner, T., Simon, P., Lorenz, M., Dindane, Z., Kuhn, K.F., Welte, M., Voigt, I., Kabitz, H.J., Wollborn, J., Goebel, U., Stoll, S.E., Kindgen-Milles, D., Dubler, S., Jung, C., Fuest, K., Schuster, M., Papadogoulas, A., Mulita, F., Rovina, N., Aidoni, Z., Chrisanthopoulou, E., Kondili, E., Andrianopoulos, I., Groenendijk, M., Evers, M., van Lelyveld-Haas, L., Meynaar, I., Cornet, A.D., Zegers, M., Dieperink, W., De Lange, D.W., Dormans, T., Hahn, M., Sjøbøe, B., Strietzel, H.F., Olasveengen, T., Romundstad, L., Andersen, F.H., Kluzik, A., Zatorski, P., Drygalski, T., Szczeklik, W., Klimkiewicz, J., Solek-Pastuszka, J., Onichimowski, D., Czuczwar, M., Gawda, R., Stefaniak, J., Stefanska-Wronka, K., Zabul, E., Oliveira, AIP, Assis, R., de Lurdes Campos Santos, M., Santos, H., Cardoso, F.S., Gordinho, A., Banzo, MJA, Zalba-Etayo, B., Cubero, P.P., Priego, J., Gomà, G., Tomasa-Irriguible, T.M., Sancho, S., Ferreira, A.F., Vázquez, E.M., Mira, Á.P., Ibarz, M., Iglesias, D., Arias-Rivera, S., Frutos-Vivar, F., Lopez-Cuenca, S., Aldecoa, C., Perez-Torres, D., Canas-Perez, I., Tamayo-Lomas, L., Diaz-Rodriguez, C., de Gopegui, P.R., Ben-Hamouda, N., Roberti, A., Fleury, Y., Abidi, N., Schefold, J.C., Chau, I., Dullenkopf, A., Pugh, R., and Smuts, S.
- Subjects
IMPACT ,MORTALITY ,ICU ,Medicine and Health Sciences ,ILL ELDERLY-PATIENTS ,610 Medicine & health ,ddc:610 ,Critical Care and Intensive Care Medicine ,610 Medizin und Gesundheit ,FRAILTY ,human activities ,Healthcare improvement science Radboud Institute for Health Sciences [Radboudumc 18] - Abstract
Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL. This study was endorsed by the ESICM. Free support for running the electronic database and was granted from the dep. of Epidemiology, University of Aarhus, Denmark. Bruno et al. Annals of Intensive Care (2022) 12:26 Page 10 of 11 The support of the study in France by a grant from Fondation Assistance Publique-Hôpitaux de Paris pour la recherche is greatly appreciated. In Norway, the study was supported by a grant from the Health Region West. In addition, the study was supported by a grant from the European Open Science Cloud (EOSC). EOSCsecretariat.eu has received funding from the European Union’s Horizon Programme call H2020-INFRAEOSC-05-2018-2019, grant agreement number 831644. This work was supported by the Collaborative Research Center SFB 1116 (German Research Foundation, DFG) and by the Forschungskommission of the Medical Faculty of the Heinrich-Heine-University Düsseldorf and No. 2020–21 to RRB for a Clinician Scientist Track. No (industry) sponsorship has been received for this investigator-initiated study. PURPOSE: Critically ill old intensive care unit (ICU) patients suffering from Sars-CoV-2 disease (COVID-19) are at increased risk for adverse outcomes. This post hoc analysis investigates the association of the Activities of Daily Living (ADL) with the outcome in this vulnerable patient group. METHODS: The COVIP study is a prospective international observational study that recruited ICU patients ≥ 70 years admitted with COVID-19 (NCT04321265). Several parameters including ADL (ADL; 0 = disability, 6 = no disability), Clinical Frailty Scale (CFS), SOFA score, intensive care treatment, ICU- and 3-month survival were recorded. A mixed-effects Weibull proportional hazard regression analyses for 3-month mortality adjusted for multiple confounders. RESULTS: This pre-specified analysis included 2359 patients with a documented ADL and CFS. Most patients evidenced independence in their daily living before hospital admission (80% with ADL = 6). Patients with no frailty and no disability showed the lowest, patients with frailty (CFS ≥ 5) and disability (ADL
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
7. Lactate is associated with mortality in very old intensive care patients suffering from COVID-19 : results from an international observational study of 2860 patients
- Author
-
Raphael Romano Bruno, Bernhard Wernly, Hans Flaatten, Jesper Fjølner, Antonio Artigas, Bernardo Bollen Pinto, Joerg C. Schefold, Stephan Binnebössel, Philipp Heinrich Baldia, Malte Kelm, Michael Beil, Sivri Sigal, Peter Vernon van Heerden, Wojciech Szczeklik, Muhammed Elhadi, Michael Joannidis, Sandra Oeyen, Tilemachos Zafeiridis, Jakob Wollborn, Maria José Arche Banzo, Kristina Fuest, Brian Marsh, Finn H. Andersen, Rui Moreno, Susannah Leaver, Ariane Boumendil, Dylan W. De Lange, Bertrand Guidet, Christian Jung, the COVIP Study Group, COVIP Study Group, Eller, P., Joannidis, M., Mesotten, D., Reper, P., Oeyen, S., Swinnen, W., Serck, N., Dewaele, E., Chapeta, E., Brix, H., Brushoej, J., Kumar, P., Nedergaard, H.K., Johnsen, T.K., Bundesen, C., Hansen, M.A., Uhrenholt, S., Bundgaard, H., Fjølner, J., Innes, R., Gooch, J., Cagova, L., Potter, E., Reay, M., Davey, M., Abusayed, M.A., Humphreys, S., Collins, A., Aujayeb, A., Leaver, S., Khaliq, W., Habib, A.A., Azab, M.A., Wassim, K., Elgazzar, Y.A., Salah, R., Abosheaishaa, H.M., Hussein Mohamed, AAR, Azzam, A.Y., Tharwat, S., Ali, YKNM, Elmandouh, O., Galal, I., Abu-Elfatth, A., Motawea, K., Elbahnasawy, M., Shehata, M., Tayeb, M., Osman, N., Abdel-Elsalam, W., Hussein, A.M., Aldhalia, A., Galbois, A., Guidet, B., Charron, C., Berlemont, C.H., Besch, G., Rigaud, J.P., Maizel, J., Djibré, M., Burtin, P., Garcon, P., Nseir, S., Valette, X., Alexandru, N., Marin, N., Vaissiere, M., Plantefeve, G., Mentec, H., Vanderlinden, T., Jurcisin, I., Megarbane, B., Chousterman, B.G., Dépret, F., Garnier, M., Besset, S., Oziel, J., Ferre, A., Dauger, S., Dumas, G., Goncalves, B., Vettoretti, L., Thevenin, D., Schaller, S., Kurt, M., Faltlhauser, A., Meyer, C., Milovanovic, M., Lutz, M., Shala, G., Haake, H., Randerath, W., Kunstein, A., Meybohm, P., Steiner, S., Barth, E., Poerner, T., Simon, P., Lorenz, M., Dindane, Z., Kuhn, K.F., Welte, M., Voigt, I., Kabitz, H.J., Wollborn, J., Goebel, U., Stoll, S.E., Kindgen-Milles, D., Dubler, S., Jung, C., Fuest, K., Schuster, M., Papadogoulas, A., Mulita, F., Rovina, N., Aidoni, Z., Chrisanthopoulou, E., Kondili, E., Andrianopoulos, I., Gurjar, M., Mahmoodpoor, A., Hussein, R., Al-Juaifari, M.A., Karantenachy, AKA, Sviri, S., Elsaka, A., Marsh, B., Comellini, V., Al-Ali, F., Almani, S., Khamees, A.A., Al-Shami, K., El Din, I.S., Abubaker, T., Ahmed, H., Rabha, A., Alhadi, A., Emhamed, M., Abdeewi, S., Abusalama, A., Huwaysh, M., Alghati, E.A., Ghannam, A., Namendys-Sylva, S.A., Groenendijk, M., Evers, M., Van Lelyveld-Haas, L., Meynaar, I., Cornet, A.D., Zegers, M., Dieperink, W., De Lange, D., Dormans, T., Hahn, M., Sjøbøe, B., Strietzel, H.F., Olasveengen, T., Romundstad, L., Andersen, F.H., Massoud, JGG, Khan, A.G., Al-Qasrawi, S., Amro, S., Kluzik, A., Zatorski, P., Drygalski, T., Szczeklik, W., Klimkiewicz, J., Solek-Pastuszka, J., Onichimowski, D., Czuczwar, M., Gawda, R., Stefaniak, J., Stefanska-Wronka, K., Zabul, E., Oliveira, AIP, Assis, R., De Lurdes Campos Santos, M., Santos, H., Cardoso, F.S., Gordinho, A., Grintescu, I.M., Tomescu, D., Badawy, M.R., José Arche Banzo, M., Zalba-Etayo, B., Cubero, P.J., Priego, J., Gomà, G., Tomasa-Irriguible, T.M., Sancho, S., Ferreira, A.F., Vázquez, E.M., Mira, Á.P., Ibarz, M., Iglesias, D., Arias-Rivera, S., Frutos-Vivar, F., Lopez-Cuenca, S., Aldecoa, C., Perez-Torres, D., Canas-Perez, I., Tamayo-Lomas, L., Diaz-Rodriguez, C., De Gopegui, P.R., Saleh, M., Hilles, MMY, Abualqumboz, EMY, Ben-Hamouda, N., Roberti, A., Fleury, Y., Abidi, N., Schefold, J.C., Chau, I., Dullenkopf, A., Chaaban, M.K., Shebani, M.M., Hmaideh, A., Shaher, A., Sahin, A.S., Saracoglu, K.T., Al-Sadawi, M., Pugh, R., Smuts, S., and Al-Saban, RAM
- Subjects
SEPSIS ,RC86-88.9 ,IMPACT ,Research ,SEPTIC SHOCK ,LEVEL ,Medicine and Health Sciences ,Medical emergencies. Critical care. Intensive care. First aid ,610 Medicine & health ,ELDERLY-PATIENTS ,Healthcare improvement science Radboud Institute for Health Sciences [Radboudumc 18] - Abstract
Purpose Lactate is an established prognosticator in critical care. However, there still is insufficient evidence about its role in predicting outcome in COVID-19. This is of particular concern in older patients who have been mostly affected during the initial surge in 2020. Methods This prospective international observation study (The COVIP study) recruited patients aged 70 years or older (ClinicalTrials.gov ID: NCT04321265) admitted to an intensive care unit (ICU) with COVID-19 disease from March 2020 to February 2021. In addition to serial lactate values (arterial blood gas analysis), we recorded several parameters, including SOFA score, ICU procedures, limitation of care, ICU- and 3-month mortality. A lactate concentration ≥ 2.0 mmol/L on the day of ICU admission (baseline) was defined as abnormal. The primary outcome was ICU-mortality. The secondary outcomes 30-day and 3-month mortality. Results In total, data from 2860 patients were analyzed. In most patients (68%), serum lactate was lower than 2 mmol/L. Elevated baseline serum lactate was associated with significantly higher ICU- and 3-month mortality (53% vs. 43%, and 71% vs. 57%, respectively, p
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
8. Diabetes in urban Guinea-Bissau; patient characteristics, mortality and prevalence of undiagnosed dysglycemia.
- Author
-
Byberg S, Bundesen C, Rudolf F, Haraldsdottir TL, Indjai L, Barai R, Beck-Nielsen H, Sodemann M, Jensen DM, and Bjerregaard-Andersen M
- Subjects
- Adult, Aged, Blood Glucose, Cause of Death, Female, Guinea-Bissau epidemiology, Humans, Male, Middle Aged, Prevalence, Risk Factors, Diabetes Mellitus, Type 2 epidemiology, Diabetes Mellitus, Type 2 mortality
- Abstract
Background: The burden of diabetes mellitus in Sub-Saharan Africa is growing rapidly, and yet the prevalence and patient characteristics are still largely unknown., Objectives: We analyzed clinical and demographic characteristics of Type 2 diabetes (T2DM) patients attending a diabetes clinic in Guinea-Bissau from February 2008 to April 2014, and estimated the prevalence and risk factors of unknown-impaired fasting plasma glucose (FPG) and diabetes, as well as excess mortality associated with T2DM., Methods: We characterized T2DM patients attending the national diabetes clinic in Bissau. Diabetes was diagnosed based on FPG. We matched T2DM patients 1:1 with non-diabetes community controls on age and sex to determine relevant risk factors for T2DM using logistic regression. Furthermore, we matched patients 1:6 with community controls to assess long-term survival (until February 2019) in a Cox regression using calendar time as the underlying timescale. Verbal autopsies determined the cause of death among T2DM patients and controls., Results: The mean age among T2DM was 50.6 (SD 11.1), and the mean FPG at first consultation was high (13.2 mmol/L (SD 5.1)). Ethnicity, family history of diabetes, hypertension, and anthropometrics differed among T2DM patients, community controls with impaired FPG, and healthy controls. Family history of diabetes (OR = 5.65, 95% CI: 3.10-10.3) and elevated waist circumference (2.33, 1.26-4.29) were significant risk factors for T2DM. 20.4% (59/289) of community controls had abnormal FPG. T2DM patients had an excess mortality hazard ratio of 3.53 (95%CI: 1.92-6.52). Deaths caused by bacterial infections, including foot ulcers, were more common among T2DM patients, compared with community controls (54% (7/13) vs. 19% (5/27) (P = 0.02))., Conclusion: Several risk factors including were associated with T2DM in Guinea-Bissau. We found a high prevalence of elevated FPG among randomly selected community controls. In combination with higher mortality among T2DM patients, an urgent need for better treatment options and increased awareness.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
9. Comparing exponential race and signal detection models of encoding stimuli into visual short-term memory.
- Author
-
Larsen A, Markussen B, and Bundesen C
- Subjects
- Adult, Female, Humans, Male, Young Adult, Attention physiology, Memory, Short-Term physiology, Models, Psychological, Pattern Recognition, Visual physiology, Psychomotor Performance physiology, Signal Detection, Psychological physiology
- Abstract
The exponential race model embodied in the theory of visual attention (TVA) and the power law generalization of the sample size model (SSPL) provide competing accounts of the mechanisms that determine how exposure duration, set size, and attention influence how many items enter visual short-term memory (VSTM). In the exponential race model, items compete for entry into VSTM in a processing race with exponentially distributed processing times. The most recent version of the sample size model assumes that target sensitivity measured by d ' increases monotonically as a function of exposure duration and decreases as a power function of set size. Here we compare the two models in a new experiment with letters and Gabor patches and with data from five previously published experiments. This was done by applying TVA to the two-alternative forced-choice method (2AFC), which forms the basis of the experimental work on the sample size model. Both models fitted individual participants' proportions of correct trials quite well, and overall the fits by the two models were almost indistinguishable. This was confirmed by formal pairwise comparison of TVA and SSPL by the Akaike information criterion and the Bayesian information criterion measures. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights reserved).
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
10. A Poisson random walk model of response times.
- Author
-
Blurton SP, Kyllingsbæk S, Nielsen CS, and Bundesen C
- Subjects
- Choice Behavior, Humans, Pattern Recognition, Visual, Attention, Decision Making, Models, Psychological, Models, Statistical, Poisson Distribution, Reaction Time, Visual Perception
- Abstract
Based on the simple what first comes to mind rule, the theory of visual attention (TVA; Bundesen, 1990) provides a comprehensive account of visual attention that has been successful in explaining performance in visual categorization for a variety of attention tasks. If the stimuli to be categorized are mutually confusable, a response rule based on the amount of evidence collected over a longer time seems more appropriate. In this paper, we extend the idea of a simple race to continuous sampling of evidence in favor of a certain response category. The resulting Poisson random walk model is a TVA-based response time model in which categories are reported based on the amount of evidence obtained. We demonstrate that the model provides an excellent account for response time distributions obtained in speeded visual categorization tasks. The model is mathematically tractable, and its parameters are well founded and easily interpretable. We also provide an extension of the Poisson random walk to any number of response alternatives. We tested the model in experiments with speeded and nonspeeded binary responses and a speeded response task with multiple report categories. The Poisson random walk model agreed very well with the data. A thorough investigation of processing rates revealed that the perceptual categorizations described by the Poisson random walk were the same as those obtained from TVA. The Poisson random walk model could therefore provide a unifying account of attention and response times. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights reserved).
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
11. Distinguishing between parallel and serial processing in visual attention from neurobiological data.
- Author
-
Li K, Kadohisa M, Kusunoki M, Duncan J, Bundesen C, and Ditlevsen S
- Abstract
Serial and parallel processing in visual search have been long debated in psychology, but the processing mechanism remains an open issue. Serial processing allows only one object at a time to be processed, whereas parallel processing assumes that various objects are processed simultaneously. Here, we present novel neural models for the two types of processing mechanisms based on analysis of simultaneously recorded spike trains using electrophysiological data from prefrontal cortex of rhesus monkeys while processing task-relevant visual displays. We combine mathematical models describing neuronal attention and point process models for spike trains. The same model can explain both serial and parallel processing by adopting different parameter regimes. We present statistical methods to distinguish between serial and parallel processing based on both maximum likelihood estimates and decoding the momentary focus of attention when two stimuli are presented simultaneously. Results show that both processing mechanisms are in play for the simultaneously recorded neurons, but neurons tend to follow parallel processing in the beginning after the onset of the stimulus pair, whereas they tend to serial processing later on., Competing Interests: The authors declare they have no competing interests., (© 2020 The Authors.)
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
12. Visual attention in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder before and after stimulant treatment.
- Author
-
Low AM, Vangkilde S, le Sommer J, Fagerlund B, Glenthøj B, Jepsen JRM, Bundesen C, Petersen A, and Habekost T
- Subjects
- Adult, Cognition, Denmark, Female, Humans, Male, Memory, Short-Term, Neuropsychological Tests, Prospective Studies, Reaction Time, Young Adult, Attention, Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity drug therapy, Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity psychology, Central Nervous System Stimulants therapeutic use, Methylphenidate therapeutic use
- Abstract
Background: Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder which frequently persists into adulthood. The primary goal of the current study was to (a) investigate attentional functions of stimulant medication-naïve adults with ADHD, and (b) investigate the effects of 6 weeks of methylphenidate treatment on these functions., Methods: The study was a prospective, non-randomized, non-blinded, 6-week follow-up design with 42 stimulant medication-naïve adult patients with ADHD, and 42 age and parental education-matched healthy controls. Assessments included measures of visual attention, based on Bundesen's Theory of Visual Attention (TVA), which yields five precise measures of aspects of visual attention; general psychopathology; ADHD symptoms; dyslexia screening; and estimates of IQ., Results: At baseline, significant differences were found between patients and controls on three attentional parameters: visual short-term memory capacity, threshold of conscious perception, and to a lesser extent visual processing speed. Secondary analyses revealed no significant correlations between TVA parameter estimates and severity of ADHD symptomatology. At follow-up, significant improvements were found specifically for visual processing speed; this improvement had a large effect size, and remained when controlling for re-test effects, IQ, and dyslexia screen performance. There were no significant correlations between changes in visual processing speed and changes in ADHD symptomatology., Conclusions: ADHD in adults may be associated with deficits in three distinct aspects of visual attention. Improvements after 6 weeks of medication are seen specifically in visual processing speed, which could represent an improvement in alertness. Clinical symptoms and visual attentional deficits may represent separate aspects of ADHD in adults.
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
13. A physiologically based nonhomogeneous Poisson counter model of visual identification.
- Author
-
Christensen JH, Markussen B, Bundesen C, and Kyllingsbæk S
- Subjects
- Adult, Female, Humans, Male, Poisson Distribution, Young Adult, Attention physiology, Decision Making physiology, Models, Theoretical, Pattern Recognition, Visual physiology
- Abstract
A physiologically based nonhomogeneous Poisson counter model of visual identification is presented. The model was developed in the framework of a Theory of Visual Attention (Bundesen, 1990; Kyllingsbæk, Markussen, & Bundesen, 2012) and meant for modeling visual identification of objects that are mutually confusable and hard to see. The model assumes that the visual system's initial sensory response consists in tentative visual categorizations, which are accumulated by leaky integration of both transient and sustained components comparable with those found in spike density patterns of early sensory neurons. The sensory response (tentative categorizations) feeds independent Poisson counters, each of which accumulates tentative object categorizations of a particular type to guide overt identification performance. We tested the model's ability to predict the effect of stimulus duration on observed distributions of responses in a nonspeeded (pure accuracy) identification task with eight response alternatives. The time courses of correct and erroneous categorizations were well accounted for when the event-rates of competing Poisson counters were allowed to vary independently over time in a way that mimicked the dynamics of receptive field selectivity as found in neurophysiological studies. Furthermore, the initial sensory response yielded theoretical hazard rate functions that closely resembled empirically estimated ones. Finally, supplied with a Naka-Rushton type contrast gain control, the model provided an explanation for Bloch's law. (PsycINFO Database Record, ((c) 2018 APA, all rights reserved).)
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
14. In defense of limited-processing-capacity models for encoding into visual short-term memory: Comment on Sewell, Lilburn, and Smith (2014).
- Author
-
Bundesen C
- Subjects
- Humans, Memory, Short-Term
- Abstract
Despite claims to the contrary, experimental results by Sewell, Lilburn, and Smith (2014) appear to be consistent with limited-processing-capacity models for encoding into visual short-term memory. (PsycINFO Database Record, ((c) 2018 APA, all rights reserved).)
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
15. Attentional weights in vision as products of spatial and nonspatial components.
- Author
-
Nordfang M, Staugaard C, and Bundesen C
- Subjects
- Adolescent, Female, Humans, Male, Young Adult, Attention physiology, Models, Psychological, Pattern Recognition, Visual physiology, Space Perception physiology
- Abstract
The relationship between visual attentional selection of items in particular spatial locations and selection by nonspatial criteria was investigated in a partial report experiment with report of letters (as many as possible) from brief postmasked exposures of circular arrays of letters and digits. The data were fitted by mathematical models based on Bundesen's (Psychological Review, 97, 523-547, 1990) theory of visual attention (TVA). Both attentional weights of targets (letters) and attentional weights of distractors (digits) showed strong variations across the eight possible target locations, but for each of the ten participants, the ratio of the weight of a distractor at a given location to the weight of a target at the same location was approximately constant. The results were accommodated by revising the weight equation of TVA such that the attentional weight of an object equals a product of a spatial weight component (weight due to being at a particular location) and a nonspatial weight component (weight due to having particular features other than locations), the two components scaling the effects of each other multiplicatively.
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
16. The effect of phasic auditory alerting on visual perception.
- Author
-
Petersen A, Petersen AH, Bundesen C, Vangkilde S, and Habekost T
- Subjects
- Acoustic Stimulation, Adult, Cues, Female, Humans, Male, Models, Psychological, Photic Stimulation, Pupil, Young Adult, Attention, Auditory Perception, Visual Perception
- Abstract
Phasic alertness refers to a short-lived change in the preparatory state of the cognitive system following an alerting signal. In the present study, we examined the effect of phasic auditory alerting on distinct perceptual processes, unconfounded by motor components. We combined an alerting/no-alerting design with a pure accuracy-based single-letter recognition task. Computational modeling based on Bundesen's Theory of Visual Attention was used to examine the effect of phasic alertness on visual processing speed and threshold of conscious perception. Results show that phasic auditory alertness affects visual perception by increasing the visual processing speed and lowering the threshold of conscious perception (Experiment 1). By manipulating the intensity of the alerting cue, we further observed a positive relationship between alerting intensity and processing speed, which was not seen for the threshold of conscious perception (Experiment 2). This was replicated in a third experiment, in which pupil size was measured as a physiological marker of alertness. Results revealed that the increase in processing speed was accompanied by an increase in pupil size, substantiating the link between alertness and processing speed (Experiment 3). The implications of these results are discussed in relation to a newly developed mathematical model of the relationship between levels of alertness and the speed with which humans process visual information., (Copyright © 2017 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.)
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
17. Behavioral and Brain Measures of Phasic Alerting Effects on Visual Attention.
- Author
-
Wiegand I, Petersen A, Finke K, Bundesen C, Lansner J, and Habekost T
- Abstract
In the present study, we investigated effects of phasic alerting on visual attention in a partial report task, in which half of the displays were preceded by an auditory warning cue. Based on the computational Theory of Visual Attention (TVA), we estimated parameters of spatial and non-spatial aspects of visual attention and measured event-related lateralizations (ERLs) over visual processing areas. We found that the TVA parameter sensory effectiveness a , which is thought to reflect visual processing capacity, significantly increased with phasic alerting. By contrast, the distribution of visual processing resources according to task relevance and spatial position, as quantified in parameters top-down control α and spatial bias w
index , was not modulated by phasic alerting. On the electrophysiological level, the latencies of ERLs in response to the task displays were reduced following the warning cue. These results suggest that phasic alerting facilitates visual processing in a general, unselective manner and that this effect originates in early stages of visual information processing.- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
18. Out with the old? The role of selective attention in retaining targets in partial report.
- Author
-
Lindsey DR, Bundesen C, Kyllingsbæk S, Petersen A, and Logan GD
- Subjects
- Adolescent, Adult, Humans, Young Adult, Attention physiology, Choice Behavior physiology, Memory, Short-Term physiology, Pattern Recognition, Visual physiology
- Abstract
In the partial-report task, subjects are asked to report only a portion of the items presented. Selective attention chooses which objects to represent in short-term memory (STM) on the basis of their relevance. Because STM is limited in capacity, one must sometimes choose which objects are removed from memory in light of new relevant information. We tested the hypothesis that the choices among newly presented information and old information in STM involve the same process-that both are acts of selective attention. We tested this hypothesis using a two-display partial-report procedure. In this procedure, subjects had to select and retain relevant letters (targets) from two sequentially presented displays. If selection in perception and retention in STM are the same process, then irrelevant letters (distractors) in the second display, which demanded attention because of their similarity to the targets, should have decreased target report from the first display. This effect was not obtained in any of four experiments. Thus, choosing objects to keep in STM is not the same process as choosing new objects to bring into STM.
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
19. Neurons in Primate Visual Cortex Alternate between Responses to Multiple Stimuli in Their Receptive Field.
- Author
-
Li K, Kozyrev V, Kyllingsbæk S, Treue S, Ditlevsen S, and Bundesen C
- Abstract
A fundamental question concerning representation of the visual world in our brain is how a cortical cell responds when presented with more than a single stimulus. We find supportive evidence that most cells presented with a pair of stimuli respond predominantly to one stimulus at a time, rather than a weighted average response. Traditionally, the firing rate is assumed to be a weighted average of the firing rates to the individual stimuli (response-averaging model) (Bundesen et al., 2005). Here, we also evaluate a probability-mixing model (Bundesen et al., 2005), where neurons temporally multiplex the responses to the individual stimuli. This provides a mechanism by which the representational identity of multiple stimuli in complex visual scenes can be maintained despite the large receptive fields in higher extrastriate visual cortex in primates. We compare the two models through analysis of data from single cells in the middle temporal visual area (MT) of rhesus monkeys when presented with two separate stimuli inside their receptive field with attention directed to one of the two stimuli or outside the receptive field. The spike trains were modeled by stochastic point processes, including memory effects of past spikes and attentional effects, and statistical model selection between the two models was performed by information theoretic measures as well as the predictive accuracy of the models. As an auxiliary measure, we also tested for uni- or multimodality in interspike interval distributions, and performed a correlation analysis of simultaneously recorded pairs of neurons, to evaluate population behavior.
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
20. Responses of Leaky Integrate-and-Fire Neurons to a Plurality of Stimuli in Their Receptive Fields.
- Author
-
Li K, Bundesen C, and Ditlevsen S
- Abstract
A fundamental question concerning the way the visual world is represented in our brain is how a cortical cell responds when its classical receptive field contains a plurality of stimuli. Two opposing models have been proposed. In the response-averaging model, the neuron responds with a weighted average of all individual stimuli. By contrast, in the probability-mixing model, the cell responds to a plurality of stimuli as if only one of the stimuli were present. Here we apply the probability-mixing and the response-averaging model to leaky integrate-and-fire neurons, to describe neuronal behavior based on observed spike trains. We first estimate the parameters of either model using numerical methods, and then test which model is most likely to have generated the observed data. Results show that the parameters can be successfully estimated and the two models are distinguishable using model selection.
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
21. Recent developments in a computational theory of visual attention (TVA).
- Author
-
Bundesen C, Vangkilde S, and Petersen A
- Subjects
- Humans, Neuropsychological Tests, Psychological Theory, Attention physiology, Computer Simulation, Memory, Short-Term physiology, Visual Perception physiology
- Abstract
This article reviews the foundations of the theory of visual attention (TVA) and describes recent developments in the theory. TVA is based on the principle of biased competition: All possible visual categorizations ascribing features to objects compete (race) to become encoded into visual short-term memory before it is filled up. Each of the possible categorizations is supported by sensory evidence, but the competition is biased by multiplication with attentional weights (high weights on important objects) and perceptual biases (toward use of important categories). The way sensory evidence and attentional biases interact is specified in the rate and weight equations of TVA, so TVA represents a mathematical formalization of the biased competition principle. In addition to describing TVA as a psychological theory, we present the neural interpretation of TVA, NTVA., (Copyright © 2014 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.)
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
22. A Componential Analysis of Visual Attention in Children With ADHD.
- Author
-
McAvinue LP, Vangkilde S, Johnson KA, Habekost T, Kyllingsbæk S, Bundesen C, and Robertson IH
- Subjects
- Adolescent, Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity diagnosis, Child, Female, Humans, Male, Attention physiology, Memory, Short-Term physiology
- Abstract
Objective: Inattentive behaviour is a defining characteristic of ADHD. Researchers have wondered about the nature of the attentional deficit underlying these symptoms. The primary purpose of the current study was to examine this attentional deficit using a novel paradigm based upon the Theory of Visual Attention (TVA)., Method: The TVA paradigm enabled a componential analysis of visual attention through the use of a mathematical model to estimate parameters relating to attentional selectivity and capacity. Children's ability to sustain attention was also assessed using the Sustained Attention to Response Task. The sample included a comparison between 25 children with ADHD and 25 control children aged 9-13., Results: Children with ADHD had significantly impaired sustained attention and visual processing speed but intact attentional selectivity, perceptual threshold and visual short-term memory capacity., Conclusion: The results of this study lend support to the notion of differential impairment of attentional functions in children with ADHD., (© 2012 SAGE Publications.)
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
23. Repetition priming in selective attention: A TVA analysis.
- Author
-
Ásgeirsson ÁG, Kristjánsson Á, and Bundesen C
- Subjects
- Adult, Female, Humans, Male, Psychological Theory, Young Adult, Attention physiology, Color Perception physiology, Pattern Recognition, Visual physiology, Perceptual Masking physiology, Psychomotor Performance physiology, Repetition Priming physiology
- Abstract
Current behavior is influenced by events in the recent past. In visual attention, this is expressed in many variations of priming effects. Here, we investigate color priming in a brief exposure digit-recognition task. Observers performed a masked odd-one-out singleton recognition task where the target-color either repeated or changed between subsequent trials. Performance was measured by recognition accuracy over exposure durations. The purpose of the study was to replicate earlier findings of perceptual priming in brief displays and to model those results based on a Theory of Visual Attention (TVA; Bundesen, 1990). We tested 4 different definitions of a generic TVA-model and assessed their explanatory power. Our hypothesis was that priming effects could be explained by selective mechanisms, and that target-color repetitions would only affect the selectivity parameter (α) of our models. Repeating target colors enhanced performance for all 12 observers. As predicted, this was only true under conditions that required selection of a target among distractors, but not when a target was presented alone. Model fits by TVA were obtained with a trial-by-trial maximum likelihood estimation procedure that estimated 4-15 free parameters, depending on the particular model. We draw two main conclusions. Color priming can be modeled simply as a change in selectivity between conditions of repetition or swap of target color. Depending on the desired resolution of analysis; priming can accurately be modeled by a simple four parameter model, where VSTM capacity and spatial biases of attention are ignored, or more fine-grained by a 10 parameter model that takes these aspects into account., (Copyright © 2015 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.)
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
24. Editorial: Theories of visual attention-linking cognition, neuropsychology, and neurophysiology.
- Author
-
Kyllingsbæ S, Vangkilde S, and Bundesen C
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
25. Beyond trial types.
- Author
-
Dyrholm M, Vangkilde S, and Bundesen C
- Subjects
- Attention physiology, Female, Humans, Photic Stimulation, Reaction Time physiology, Visual Perception physiology, Models, Psychological, Research Design
- Abstract
Conventional wisdom on psychological experiments has held that when one or more independent variables are manipulated it is essential that all other conditions are kept constant such that confounding factors can be assumed negligible (Woodworth, 1938). In practice, the latter assumption is often questionable because it is generally difficult to guarantee that all other conditions are constant between any two trials. Therefore, the most common way to check for confounding violations of this assumption is to split the experimental conditions in terms of "trial types" to simulate a reduction of unintended trial-by-trial variation. Here, we pose a method which is more general than the use of trial types: use of mathematical models treating measures of potentially confounding factors and manipulated variables as equals on the single-trial level. We show how the method can be applied with models that subsume under the generalized linear item response theory (GLIRT), which is the case for most of the well-known psychometric models (Mellenbergh, 1994). As an example, we provide a new analysis of a single-letter recognition experiment using a nested likelihood ratio test that treats manipulated and measured variables equally (i.e., in exactly the same way) on the single-trial level. The test detects a confounding interaction with time-on-task as a single-trial measure and yields a substantially better estimate of the effect size of the main manipulation compared with an analysis made in terms of trial types.
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
26. Dissociable spatial and non-spatial attentional deficits after circumscribed thalamic stroke.
- Author
-
Kraft A, Irlbacher K, Finke K, Kaufmann C, Kehrer S, Liebermann D, Bundesen C, and Brandt SA
- Subjects
- Female, Functional Laterality physiology, Humans, Male, Middle Aged, Neuropsychological Tests, Perceptual Disorders etiology, Reaction Time physiology, Stroke complications, Visual Perception physiology, Attention physiology, Perceptual Disorders physiopathology, Space Perception physiology, Stroke physiopathology, Thalamus physiopathology
- Abstract
Thalamic nuclei act as sensory, motor and cognitive relays between multiple subcortical areas and the cerebral cortex. They play a crucial role in cognitive functions such as executive functioning, memory and attention. In the acute period after thalamic stroke attentional deficits are common. The precise functional relevance of specific nuclei or vascular sub regions of the thalamus for attentional sub functions is still unclear. The theory of visual attention (TVA) allows the measurement of four independent attentional parameters (visual short term memory storage capacity (VSTM), visual perceptual processing speed, selective control and spatial weighting). We combined parameter-based assessment based on TVA with lesion symptom mapping in standard stereotactic space in sixteen patients (mean age 41.2 ± 11.0 SD, 6 females), with focal thalamic lesions in the medial (N = 9), lateral (N = 5), anterior (N = 1) or posterior (N = 1) vascular territories of the thalamus. Compared with an age-matched control group of 52 subjects (mean age 40.1 ± 6.4, 35 females), the patients with thalamic lesions were, on the group level, mildly impaired in visual processing speed and VSTM. Patients with lateral thalamic lesions showed a deficit in processing speed while all other TVA parameters were within the normal range. Medial thalamic lesions can be associated with a spatial bias and extinction of targets either in the ipsilesional or the contralesional field. A posterior case with a thalamic lesion of the pulvinar replicated a finding of Habekost and Rostrup (2006), demonstrating a spatial bias to the ipsilesional field, as suggested by the neural theory of visual attention (NTVA) (Bundesen, Habekost, & Kyllingsbæk, 2011). A case with an anterior-medial thalamic lesion showed reduced selective attentional control. We conclude that lesions in distinct vascular sub regions of the thalamus are associated with distinct attentional syndromes (medial = spatial bias, lateral = processing speed)., (Copyright © 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.)
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
27. TMS over the right precuneus reduces the bilateral field advantage in visual short term memory capacity.
- Author
-
Kraft A, Dyrholm M, Kehrer S, Kaufmann C, Bruening J, Kathmann N, Bundesen C, Irlbacher K, and Brandt SA
- Subjects
- Adult, Attention physiology, Female, Functional Laterality physiology, Humans, Male, Photic Stimulation, Young Adult, Memory, Short-Term physiology, Parietal Lobe physiology, Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation, Visual Perception physiology
- Abstract
Background: Several studies have demonstrated a bilateral field advantage (BFA) in early visual attentional processing, that is, enhanced visual processing when stimuli are spread across both visual hemifields. The results are reminiscent of a hemispheric resource model of parallel visual attentional processing, suggesting more attentional resources on an early level of visual processing for bilateral displays [e.g. Sereno AB, Kosslyn SM. Discrimination within and between hemifields: a new constraint on theories of attention. Neuropsychologia 1991;29(7):659-75.]. Several studies have shown that the BFA extends beyond early stages of visual attentional processing, demonstrating that visual short term memory (VSTM) capacity is higher when stimuli are distributed bilaterally rather than unilaterally., Objective/hypothesis: Here we examine whether hemisphere-specific resources are also evident on later stages of visual attentional processing., Methods: Based on the Theory of Visual Attention (TVA) [Bundesen C. A theory of visual attention. Psychol Rev 1990;97(4):523-47.] we used a whole report paradigm that allows investigating visual attention capacity variability in unilateral and bilateral displays during navigated repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) of the precuneus region., Results: A robust BFA in VSTM storage capacity was apparent after rTMS over the left precuneus and in the control condition without rTMS. In contrast, the BFA diminished with rTMS over the right precuneus., Conclusion: This finding indicates that the right precuneus plays a causal role in VSTM capacity, particularly in bilateral visual displays., (Copyright © 2015 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.)
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
28. Components of visual bias: a multiplicative hypothesis.
- Author
-
Bundesen C, Vangkilde S, and Habekost T
- Subjects
- Bayes Theorem, Empirical Research, Humans, Arousal physiology, Attention physiology, Pattern Recognition, Visual physiology, Photic Stimulation methods
- Abstract
Attentional selection can be viewed as having two aspects: selection with respect to particular objects and selection with respect to particular categories. Both aspects are mathematically modeled in the theory of visual attention (TVA). In this paper, we expand the rate equation of the TVA and propose that the visual bias toward seeing an object x as a member of category i is a product of three factors: the expectancy (prior probability) of being presented with members of category i, the subjective importance (utility) of seeing objects in category i as members of that category, and the general level of alertness. Together, the three factors also determine the level of arousal in the visual system. The hypothesized multiplicative interaction between the three components of visual bias seems consistent with the function of an ideal observer and also paves the way for a Bayesian interpretation of the TVA., (© 2015 New York Academy of Sciences.)
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
29. Confusing confusability: on the problems of using psychophysical measures of letter confusability in neuropsychological research.
- Author
-
Starrfelt R, Lindegaard M, and Bundesen C
- Subjects
- Humans, Verbal Behavior, Behavioral Research methods, Dyslexia physiopathology, Language, Neuropsychology methods, Pattern Recognition, Visual, Psychophysics methods, Reading
- Abstract
The effect of letter confusability on reading has received increasing attention over the last decade. Confusability scores for individual letters, derived from older psychophysical studies, have been used to calculate summed confusability scores for whole words, and effects of this variable on normal and alexic reading have been reported. On this basis, letter confusability is now increasingly controlled for in stimulus selection. In this commentary, we try to clarify what letter confusability scores represent and discuss several problems with the way this variable has been treated in neuropsychological research. We conclude that it is premature to control for this variable when selecting stimuli in studies of reading and alexia. Although letter confusability may play a role in (impaired) reading, it remains to be determined how this measure should be calculated, and what effect it may have on word and letter identification.
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
30. Components of attention modulated by temporal expectation.
- Author
-
Sørensen TA, Vangkilde S, and Bundesen C
- Subjects
- Adult, Cues, Female, Humans, Male, Models, Psychological, Photic Stimulation, Probability, Psychological Tests, Time Factors, Young Adult, Anticipation, Psychological, Attention, Memory, Short-Term, Visual Perception
- Abstract
By varying the probabilities that a stimulus would appear at particular times after the presentation of a cue and modeling the data by the theory of visual attention (Bundesen, 1990), Vangkilde, Coull, and Bundesen (2012) provided evidence that the speed of encoding a singly presented stimulus letter into visual short-term memory (VSTM) is modulated by the observer's temporal expectations. We extended the investigation from single-stimulus recognition to whole report (Experiment 1) and partial report (Experiment 2). Cue-stimulus foreperiods were distributed geometrically using time steps of 500 ms. In high expectancy conditions, the probability that the stimulus would appear on the next time step, given that it had not yet appeared, was high, whereas in low expectancy conditions, the probability was low. The speed of encoding the stimuli into VSTM was higher in the high expectancy conditions. In line with the Easterbrook (1959) hypothesis, under high temporal expectancy, the processing was also more focused (selective). First, the storage capacity of VSTM was lower, so that fewer stimuli were encoded into VSTM. Second, the distribution of attentional weights across stimuli was less even: The efficiency of selecting targets rather than distractors for encoding into VSTM was higher, as was the spread of the attentional weights of the target letters., (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2015 APA, all rights reserved.)
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.