19 results on '"Katz, Mindy"'
Search Results
2. Dose-response relationship between late-life physical activity and incident dementia: A pooled analysis of 10 cohort studies of memory in an international consortium
- Author
-
Wu, Wanqing, Ding, Ding, Zhao, Qianhua, Xiao, Zhenxu, Luo, Jianfeng, Ganguli, Mary, Hughes, Tiffany F, Jacobsen, Erin, Haan, Mary N, van Dang, Kristine, Lima-Costa, Maria Fernanda, Blay, Sergio Luis, de Castro-Costa, Erico, Ng, Tze Pin, Gwee, Xinyi, Gao, Qi, Gureje, Oye, Ojagbemi, Akin, Bello, Toyin, Shahar, Suzana, Ludin, Arimi Fitri Mat, Rivan, Nurul Fatin Malek, Scarmeas, Nikolaos, Anastasiou, Costas A, Yannakoulia, Mary, Brodaty, Henry, Crawford, John D, Lipton, Richard B, Derby, Carol A, Katz, Mindy J, Lipnicki, Darren M, Sachdev, Perminder S, and for Cohort Studies of Memory in an International Consortium (COSMIC)
- Subjects
dose-response ,Aging ,Prevention ,Rehabilitation ,Clinical Sciences ,Neurosciences ,physical activity ,cohort ,population-based ,Brain Disorders ,Cohort Studies ,for Cohort Studies of Memory in an International Consortium ,Risk Factors ,Clinical Research ,Geriatrics ,Neurological ,Acquired Cognitive Impairment ,Humans ,Dementia ,pooled analysis ,Proportional Hazards Models ,Aged ,dementia - Abstract
IntroductionThough consistent evidence suggests that physical activity may delay dementia onset, the duration and amount of activity required remains unclear.MethodsWe harmonized longitudinal data of 11,988 participants from 10 cohorts in eight countries to examine the dose-response relationship between late-life physical activity and incident dementia among older adults.ResultsUsing no physical activity as a reference, dementia risk decreased with duration of physical activity up to 3.1 to 6.0hours/week (hazard ratio [HR] 0.88, 95% confidence interval [CI] 0.67 to 1.15 for 0.1 to 3.0hours/week; HR 0.68, 95% CI 0.52 to 0.89 for 3.1 to 6.0hours/week), but plateaued with higher duration. For the amount of physical activity, a similar pattern of dose-response curve was observed, with an inflection point of 9.1 to 18.0 metabolic equivalent value (MET)-hours/week (HR 0.92, 95% CI 0.70 to 1.22 for 0.1 to 9.0 MET-hours/week; HR 0.70, 95% CI 0.53 to 0.93 for 9.1 to 18.0 MET-hours/week).DiscussionThis cross-national analysis suggests that performing 3.1 to 6.0hours of physical activity and expending 9.1 to 18.0/MET-hours of energy per week may reduce dementia risk.
- Published
- 2023
3. Associations between personality traits and cognitive decline in older persons with and without dementia using coordinated integrative data analysis
- Author
-
Graham, Eileen, James, Bryan, Jackson, Kathryn, Beam, Christopher, Pedersen, Nancy, Reynolds, Chandra, Katz, Mindy, Lipton, Richard, Boyle, Patricia, Wilson, Robert, Bennett, David, Willroth, Emily, Mroczek, Dan, and Luo, Jing
- Subjects
Social and Behavioral Sciences - Abstract
There are considerable individual differences in the rates of cognitive decline across later adulthood. Personality traits are one set of factors that may account for some of these differences. The current project explores whether personality traits are associated with trajectories of cognitive decline, and whether the associations are different before and after a diagnosis of dementia. The data will be analyzed using linear mixed effects regression. Across these study aims is a focus on replicability and generalizability. Each of these questions will be addressed in four independent longitudinal studies of aging (EAS, MAP, ROS, SATSA), then meta-analyzed, thus providing an estimate of the replicability of our results.
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
4. Visual memory tests enhance the identification of amnestic MCI cases at greater risk of Alzheimer’s disease
- Author
-
Oltra-Cucarella, Javier, Sanchez-SanSegundo, Miriam, Lipnicki, Darren M., Crawford, John D., Lipton, Richard B., Katz, Mindy J., Zammit, Andrea R., Scarmeas, Nikolaos, Dardiotis, Efthimios, Kosmidis, Mary H., Guaita, Antonio, Vaccaro, Roberta, Kim, Ki Woong, Han, Ji Won, Kochan, Nicole A., Brodaty, Henry, Pérez-Vicente, José A., Cabello-Rodríguez, Luis, Sachdev, Perminder S., Ferrer-Cascales, Rosario, Cohort Studies of Memory in an International Consortium (COSMIC), Universidad de Alicante. Departamento de Psicología de la Salud, and Psicología Aplicada a la Salud y Comportamiento Humano (PSYBHE)
- Subjects
medicine.medical_specialty ,Disease ,Audiology ,behavioral disciplines and activities ,Article ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,Visual memory ,Memory ,Normal cognition ,mental disorders ,Medicine ,Cognitive impairment ,Progression ,030214 geriatrics ,business.industry ,Hazard ratio ,Neuropsychology ,Mild cognitive impairment ,Psychiatry and Mental health ,Clinical Psychology ,Increased risk ,Personalidad, Evaluación y Tratamiento Psicológico ,Geriatrics and Gerontology ,Verbal memory ,business ,Alzheimer’s disease ,Gerontology ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery - Abstract
Objectives: To investigate whether amnestic mild cognitive impairment (aMCI) identified with visual memory tests conveys an increased risk of Alzheimer’s disease (risk-AD) and if the risk-AD differs from that associated with aMCI based on verbal memory tests. Participants: 4,771 participants aged 70.76 (SD = 6.74, 45.4% females) from five community-based studies, each a member of the international COSMIC consortium and from a different country, were classified as having normal cognition (NC) or one of visual, verbal, or combined (visual and verbal) aMCI using international criteria and followed for an average of 2.48 years. Hazard ratios (HR) and individual patient data (IPD) meta-analysis analyzed the risk-AD with age, sex, education, single/multiple domain aMCI, and Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) scores as covariates. Results: All aMCI groups (n = 760) had a greater risk-AD than NC (n = 4,011; HR range = 3.66 – 9.25). The risk-AD was not different between visual (n = 208, 17 converters) and verbal aMCI (n = 449, 29 converters, HR = 1.70, 95%CI: 0.88, 3.27, p = 0.111). Combined aMCI (n = 103, 12 converters, HR = 2.34, 95%CI: 1.13, 4.84, p = 0.023) had a higher risk-AD than verbal aMCI. Age and MMSE scores were related to the risk-AD. The IPD meta-analyses replicated these results, though with slightly lower HR estimates (HR range = 3.68, 7.43) for aMCI vs. NC. Conclusions: Although verbal aMCI was most common, a significant proportion of participants had visual-only or combined visual and verbal aMCI. Compared with verbal aMCI, the risk-AD was the same for visual aMCI and higher for combined aMCI. Our results highlight the importance of including both verbal and visual memory tests in neuropsychological assessments to more reliably identify aMCI. Funding for COSMIC comes from a National Health and Medical Research Council of Australia Program Grant (ID 1093083), the National Institute on Aging of the National Institutes of Health (under Award No. RF1AG057531), and philanthropic contributions to the Dementia Momentum Fund (UNSW Project ID PS38235). Andrea R. Zammit was supported by the National Institute on Aging of the National Institutes of Health (under Award No. K01AG054700). HELIAD study was supported by the following grants: IIRG-09133014 from the Alzheimer’s Association; 189 10276/8/9/2011 from the ESPA-EU program Excellence Grant (ARISTEIA), which is co-funded by the European Social Fund and Greek National resources, and ΔΥ2β/ικ.51657/14.4.2009 from the Ministry for Health and Social Solidarity (Greece). KLOSCAD was funded by a grant from the Korean Health Technology R&D Project, Ministry of Health and Welfare, Republic of Korea (Grant No. HI09C1379 [A092077]). EAS was supported in part by National Institutes of Health grants (NIA 2 P01 AG03949), the Leonard and Sylvia Marx Foundation, and the Czap Foundation.
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
5. Additional file 1 of Estimating prevalence of subjective cognitive decline in and across international cohort studies of aging: a COSMIC study
- Author
-
Röhr, Susanne, Pabst, Alexander, Riedel-Heller, Steffi G., Jessen, Frank, Yuda Turana, Handajani, Yvonne S., Brayne, Carol, Matthews, Fiona E., Stephan, Blossom C. M., Lipton, Richard B., Katz, Mindy J., Cuiling Wang, Guerchet, Maëlenn, Pierre-Marie Preux, Mbelesso, Pascal, Ritchie, Karen, Marie-Laure Ancelin, Carrière, Isabelle, Guaita, Antonio, Davin, Annalisa, Vaccaro, Roberta, Kim, Ki Woong, Han, Ji Won, Suh, Seung Wan, Shahar, Suzana, Normah C. Din, Vanoh, Divya, Boxtel, Martin Van, Köhler, Sebastian, Ganguli, Mary, Jacobsen, Erin P., Snitz, Beth E., Anstey, Kaarin J., Cherbuin, Nicolas, Kumagai, Shuzo, Sanmei Chen, Narazaki, Kenji, Tze Pin Ng, Gao, Qi, Xinyi Gwee, Brodaty, Henry, Kochan, Nicole A., Trollor, Julian, Lobo, Antonio, López-Antón, Raúl, Santabárbara, Javier, Crawford, John D., Lipnicki, Darren M., and Perminder S. Sachdev
- Subjects
Data_FILES - Abstract
Additional file 1.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
6. Data S1. Supporting Information, per2259-sup-0001-Data_S1 - Trajectories of Big Five Personality Traits: A Coordinated Analysis of 16 Longitudinal Samples
- Author
-
Graham, Eileen K., Weston, Sara J., Gerstorf, Denis, Yoneda, Tomiko B., Booth, Tom, Beam, Christopher R., Petkus, Andrew J., Drewelies, Johanna, Hall, Andrew N., Bastarache, Emily D., Estabrook, Ryne, Katz, Mindy J., Turiano, Nicholas A., Ulman Lindenberger, Smith, Jacqui, Wagner, Gert G., Pedersen, Nancy L., Allemand, Mathias, Avron Spiro, Deeg, Dorly J.H., Johansson, Boo, Piccinin, Andrea M., Lipton, Richard B., K. Warner Schaie, Willis, Sherry, Reynolds, Chandra A., Deary, Ian J., Hofer, Scott M., and Mroczek, Daniel K.
- Subjects
FOS: Psychology ,170199 Psychology not elsewhere classified - Abstract
Data S1. Supporting Information, per2259-sup-0001-Data_S1 for Trajectories of Big Five Personality Traits: A Coordinated Analysis of 16 Longitudinal Samples by Graham Eileen K., Weston Sara J., Gerstorf Denis, Yoneda Tomiko B., Booth Tom, Beam Christopher R., Petkus Andrew J., Drewelies Johanna, Hall Andrew N., Bastarache Emily D., Estabrook Ryne, Katz Mindy J., Turiano Nicholas A., Lindenberger Ulman, Smith Jacqui, Wagner Gert G., Pedersen Nancy L., Allemand Mathias, Spiro AvronIII, Deeg Dorly J.H., Johansson Boo, Piccinin Andrea M., Lipton Richard B., Schaie K. Warner, Willis Sherry, Reynolds Chandra A., Deary Ian J., Hofer Scott M. and Mroczek Daniel K. in European Journal of Personality
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
7. A Coordinated Analysis of Big-Five Trait Change Across 16 Longitudinal Samples
- Author
-
Willis, Sherry, Piccinin , Andrea, Hofer , Scott, Gerstorf, Denis, Yoneda , Tomiko, Piccinin, Andrea, Booth , Tom, Petkus, Andrew, Rutsohn , Joshua, Estabrook , Ryne, Katz , Mindy, Turiano , Nick, Lindenberger, Ulman, Smith, Jacqui, Drewelies, Johanna, Wagner , Gert, Pedersen, Nancy, Allemand, Mathias, Spiro , Avron, Deeg, Dorly, Johansson, Boo, Sliwinski , Martin, Lipton, Richard, Graham , Eileen, Beam , Christopher, Mroczek , Dan, Hofer, Scott, Deary , Ian, and Schaie, K.
- Subjects
PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Developmental Psychology|Language Aquisition ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Developmental Psychology ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Developmental Psychology|Death, Dying, and Grieving ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Developmental Psychology|Toddlerhood/Preschool Period ,Developmental psychology ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Developmental Psychology|Middle & Late Childhood ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Developmental Psychology|Gene-environment Interaction ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Developmental Psychology|Middle Adulthood ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Developmental Psychology|Adolescence ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Developmental Psychology|Emotional Development ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Developmental Psychology|Self-concept and Identity ,bepress|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Psychology|Child Psychology ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Developmental Psychology|Infancy ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Developmental Psychology|Prenatal Development ,Big Five personality traits ,bepress|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Psychology|Developmental Psychology ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Developmental Psychology|Early Adulthood ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Social and Personality Psychology ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Developmental Psychology|Early Childhood ,Personality change ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Developmental Psychology|Motor Development ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Developmental Psychology|Cognitive Development ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences| Social and Personality Psychology ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Developmental Psychology|Social Development ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Developmental Psychology|Personality Development ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Developmental Psychology|Attachment ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Developmental Psychology|Moral Development ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Developmental Psychology|Perceptual Development ,Integrative data analysis ,bepress|Social and Behavioral Sciences ,Trait ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Developmental Psychology|Old Age ,bepress|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Psychology|Personality and Social Contexts ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Developmental Psychology|Physical Development ,Psychology ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Developmental Psychology|Aging - Abstract
This study assessed change in the Big Five personality traits. We conducted a coordinated integrative data analysis (IDA) using data from 16 studies including over 60,000 respondents to examine trajectories of change in the traits of neuroticism, extraversion, openness, conscientiousness, and agreeableness. Coordinating models across multiple study sites, we fit nearly identical multi-level linear growth curve models to assess and compare the extent of trait change over time. Quadratic change was assessed in 8 studies with four or more measurement occasions. Across studies, the linear trajectory models revealed stability for agreeableness and decreases for the other four five traits. The non-linear trajectories suggest a U-shaped curve for neuroticism, and an inverted-U for extraversion. Meta-analytic summaries indicate that the fixed effects are heterogeneous, and that the variability in traits is partially explained by baseline age and country of origin. We conclude from our study that neuroticism, extraversion, conscientiousness, and openness go down over time, while agreeableness remains relatively stable.
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
8. Personality Predicts Mortality Risk: An Integrative Data Analysis of 15 International Longitudinal Studies
- Author
-
Turiano , Nick, Bendayan, Rebecca, Bastarache, Emily, Zelinski, Elizabeth, Elleman, Lorien, Piccinin, Andrea, Hofer, Scott, Pedersen, Nancy, Spiro , Avron, Yoneda , Tomiko, Deeg, Dorly, Herd, Pamela, Reynolds, Chandra, Muniz-Terrera, Graciela, Lipton, Richard, Roan, Carol, Schaie, K., Rutsohn , Joshua, Mroczek , Dan, Kuh, Diana, Piccinin , Andrea, Bennet, David, Willis, Sherry, Barnes, Lisa, Hofer , Scott, Katz , Mindy, Batterham, Philip, Johansson, Boo, Gerstorf , Denis, Sharp, Emily, Graham , Eileen, APH - Aging & Later Life, and Epidemiology and Data Science
- Subjects
PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Developmental Psychology|Language Aquisition ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Developmental Psychology ,Social and Behavioral Sciences ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Developmental Psychology|Toddlerhood/Preschool Period ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Health Psychology|Mental Health ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Developmental Psychology|Middle & Late Childhood ,0302 clinical medicine ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Developmental Psychology|Adolescence ,Personality and Social Contexts ,Psychology ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Developmental Psychology|Self-concept and Identity ,bepress|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Psychology|Child Psychology ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Developmental Psychology|Prenatal Development ,Big Five personality traits ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Health Psychology|Illness ,bepress|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Psychology|Developmental Psychology ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Developmental Psychology|Early Adulthood ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Social and Personality Psychology ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Developmental Psychology|Early Childhood ,General Psychology ,media_common ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Developmental Psychology|Cognitive Development ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences| Social and Personality Psychology ,05 social sciences ,Explained variation ,Neuroticism ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Developmental Psychology|Attachment ,FOS: Psychology ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Developmental Psychology|Moral Development ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Developmental Psychology|Perceptual Development ,bepress|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Psychology|Health Psychology ,bepress|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Psychology|Personality and Social Contexts ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Developmental Psychology|Physical Development ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Health Psychology|Treatment ,Social psychology ,Clinical psychology ,Agreeableness ,Social Psychology ,media_common.quotation_subject ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Health Psychology|Stress ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Developmental Psychology|Death, Dying, and Grieving ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Health Psychology|Prevention ,050105 experimental psychology ,Article ,03 medical and health sciences ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Health Psychology ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Developmental Psychology|Gene-environment Interaction ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Developmental Psychology|Middle Adulthood ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Developmental Psychology|Emotional Development ,Openness to experience ,Personality ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Developmental Psychology|Infancy ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Developmental Psychology|Motor Development ,Health Psychology ,Extraversion and introversion ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Developmental Psychology|Social Development ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Developmental Psychology|Personality Development ,Conscientiousness ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Health Psychology|Health-related Behavior ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Health Psychology|Social health ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences ,Developmental Psychology ,bepress|Social and Behavioral Sciences ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Developmental Psychology|Old Age ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Developmental Psychology|Aging ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery - Abstract
This study examined the Big Five personality traits as predictors of mortality risk, and smoking as a mediator of that association. Replication was built into the fabric of our design: we used a Coordinated Analysis with 15 international datasets, representing 44,094 participants. We found that high neuroticism and low conscientiousness, extraversion, and agreeableness were consistent predictors of mortality across studies. Smoking had a small mediating effect for neuroticism. Country and baseline age explained variation in effects: studies with older baseline age showed a pattern of protective effects (HR
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
9. A Coordinated Analysis of Big-Five Trait Change Across 14 Longitudinal Studies
- Author
-
Graham, Eileen, Gerstorf, Denis, Yoneda, Tomiko, Piccinin, Andrea, Booth, Tom, Beam, Christopher, Petkus, Andrew, Rutsohn, Joshua, Estabrook, Ryne, Katz, Mindy, Turiano, Nick, Lindenberger, Ulman, Smith, Jacqui, Drewelies, Johanna, Wagner, Gert, Pedersen, Nancy, Allemand, Mathias, Spiro, Avron, Deeg, Dorly, Johansson, Boo, Sliwinski, Martin, Lipton, Richard, Schaie, K., Willis, Sherry, Deary, Ian, Hofer, Scott, and Mroczek, Dan
- Subjects
FOS: Psychology ,Developmental Psychology ,Personality and Social Contexts ,Psychology ,Social and Behavioral Sciences - Abstract
This study assessed change in the Big Five personality traits. We conducted a coordinated integrative data analysis (IDA) using data from 14 studies including 47,190 respondents to examine trajectories of change in the traits of neuroticism, extraversion, openness, conscientiousness, and agreeableness. Coordinating models across multiple study sites, we fit nearly identical multi-level linear growth curve models to assess and compare the extent of trait change over time. Quadratic change was assessed in 8 studies with four or more measurement occasions. Across studies, the linear trajectory models revealed stability for agreeableness and decreases for the other four five traits. The non-linear trajectories suggest a U-shaped curve for neuroticism, and an inverted-U for extraversion. Meta-analytic summaries indicate that the fixed effects are heterogeneous, and that the variability in traits is partially explained by baseline age and country of origin. We conclude from our study that neuroticism, extraversion, conscientiousness, and openness go down over time, while agreeableness remains relatively stable.
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
10. COSMIC (Cohort Studies of Memory in an International Consortium): An international consortium to identify risk and protective factors and biomarkers of cognitive ageing and dementia
- Author
-
Sachdev, Perminder, Lipnicki, Darren, Kochan, Nicole, Crawford, John, Rockwood, Kenneth, Xiao, Shifu, Li, Juan, Li, Xia, Brayne, Carol, Matthews, Fiona, Stephan, Blossom, Lipton, Richard, Katz, Mindy, Ritchie, Karen, Carrière, Isabelle, Ancelin, Marie-Laure, Seshadri, Sudha, Au, Rhoda, Beiser, Alexa, Lam, Linda, Wong, Candy, Fung, Ada, Kim, Ki, Han, Ji, Kim, Tae, Petersen, Ronald, Roberts, Rosebud, Mielke, Michelle, Ganguli, Mary, Dodge, Hiroko, Hughes, Tiffany, Anstey, Kaarin, Cherbuin, Nicolas, Butterworth, Peter, Ng, Tze, Gao, Qi, Reppermund, Simone, Brodaty, Henry, Meguro, Kenichi, Schupf, Nicole, Manly, Jennifer J., Stern, Yaakov, Lobo, Antonio, Lopez-Anton, Raúl, and Santabárbara, Javier
- Subjects
FOS: Computer and information sciences ,Bioinformatics ,FOS: Clinical medicine ,Neurosciences ,Mild cognitive impairment ,Older people - Abstract
Background: A large number of longitudinal studies of population-based ageing cohorts are in progress internationally, but the insights from these studies into the risk and protective factors for cognitive ageing and conditions like mild cognitive impairment and dementia have been inconsistent. Some of the problems confounding this research can be reduced by harmonising and pooling data across studies. COSMIC (Cohort Studies of Memory in an International Consortium) aims to harmonise data from international cohort studies of cognitive ageing, in order to better understand the determinants of cognitive ageing and neurocognitive disorders. Methods/Design: Longitudinal studies of cognitive ageing and dementia with at least 500 individuals aged 60 years or over are eligible and invited to be members of COSMIC. There are currently 17 member studies, from regions that include Asia, Australia, Europe, and North America. A Research Steering Committee has been established, two meetings of study leaders held, and a website developed. The initial attempts at harmonising key variables like neuropsychological test scores are in progress. Discussion: The challenges of international consortia like COSMIC include efficient communication among members, extended use of resources, and data harmonisation. Successful harmonisation will facilitate projects investigating rates of cognitive decline, risk and protective factors for mild cognitive impairment, and biomarkers of mild cognitive impairment and dementia. Extended implications of COSMIC could include standardised ways of collecting and reporting data, and a rich cognitive ageing database being made available to other researchers. COSMIC could potentially transform our understanding of the epidemiology of cognitive ageing, and have a world-wide impact on promoting successful ageing.
- Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
11. Estimating prevalence of subjective cognitive decline in and across international cohort studies of aging: a COSMIC study
- Author
-
Röhr, Susanne, Pabst, Alexander, Riedel-Heller, Steffi G, Jessen, Frank, Turana, Yuda, Handajani, Yvonne S, Brayne, Carol, Matthews, Fiona E, Stephan, Blossom CM, Lipton, Richard B, Katz, Mindy J, Wang, Cuiling, Guerchet, Maëlenn, Preux, Pierre-Marie, Mbelesso, Pascal, Ritchie, Karen, Ancelin, Marie-Laure, Carrière, Isabelle, Guaita, Antonio, Davin, Annalisa, Vaccaro, Roberta, Kim, Ki Woong, Han, Ji Won, Suh, Seung Wan, Shahar, Suzana, Din, Normah C, Vanoh, Divya, Van Boxtel, Martin, Köhler, Sebastian, Ganguli, Mary, Jacobsen, Erin P, Snitz, Beth E, Anstey, Kaarin J, Cherbuin, Nicolas, Kumagai, Shuzo, Chen, Sanmei, Narazaki, Kenji, Ng, Tze Pin, Gao, Qi, Gwee, Xinyi, Brodaty, Henry, Kochan, Nicole A, Trollor, Julian, Lobo, Antonio, López-Antón, Raúl, Santabárbara, Javier, Crawford, John D, Lipnicki, Darren M, Sachdev, Perminder S, and For Cohort Studies Of Memory In An International Consortium (COSMIC)
- Subjects
Male ,Aging ,Epidemiology ,Data harmonization ,Individual participant data ,Middle Aged ,Neuropsychological Tests ,3. Good health ,Cohort Studies ,Prevalence ,Subjective cognitive decline ,Humans ,Cognitive Dysfunction ,Female ,Cohort study - Abstract
BACKGROUND: Subjective cognitive decline (SCD) is recognized as a risk stage for Alzheimer's disease (AD) and other dementias, but its prevalence is not well known. We aimed to use uniform criteria to better estimate SCD prevalence across international cohorts. METHODS: We combined individual participant data for 16 cohorts from 15 countries (members of the COSMIC consortium) and used qualitative and quantitative (Item Response Theory/IRT) harmonization techniques to estimate SCD prevalence. RESULTS: The sample comprised 39,387 cognitively unimpaired individuals above age 60. The prevalence of SCD across studies was around one quarter with both qualitative harmonization/QH (23.8%, 95%CI = 23.3-24.4%) and IRT (25.6%, 95%CI = 25.1-26.1%); however, prevalence estimates varied largely between studies (QH 6.1%, 95%CI = 5.1-7.0%, to 52.7%, 95%CI = 47.4-58.0%; IRT: 7.8%, 95%CI = 6.8-8.9%, to 52.7%, 95%CI = 47.4-58.0%). Across studies, SCD prevalence was higher in men than women, in lower levels of education, in Asian and Black African people compared to White people, in lower- and middle-income countries compared to high-income countries, and in studies conducted in later decades. CONCLUSIONS: SCD is frequent in old age. Having a quarter of older individuals with SCD warrants further investigation of its significance, as a risk stage for AD and other dementias, and of ways to help individuals with SCD who seek medical advice. Moreover, a standardized instrument to measure SCD is needed to overcome the measurement variability currently dominant in the field.
12. Open Practices Disclosure Form, per2259-sup-0002-Open_Practices_Disclosure_Form - Trajectories of Big Five Personality Traits: A Coordinated Analysis of 16 Longitudinal Samples
- Author
-
Graham, Eileen K., Weston, Sara J., Gerstorf, Denis, Yoneda, Tomiko B., Booth, Tom, Beam, Christopher R., Petkus, Andrew J., Drewelies, Johanna, Hall, Andrew N., Bastarache, Emily D., Estabrook, Ryne, Katz, Mindy J., Turiano, Nicholas A., Ulman Lindenberger, Smith, Jacqui, Wagner, Gert G., Pedersen, Nancy L., Allemand, Mathias, Avron Spiro, Deeg, Dorly J.H., Johansson, Boo, Piccinin, Andrea M., Lipton, Richard B., K. Warner Schaie, Willis, Sherry, Reynolds, Chandra A., Deary, Ian J., Hofer, Scott M., and Mroczek, Daniel K.
- Subjects
FOS: Psychology ,170199 Psychology not elsewhere classified ,16. Peace & justice - Abstract
Open Practices Disclosure Form, per2259-sup-0002-Open_Practices_Disclosure_Form for Trajectories of Big Five Personality Traits: A Coordinated Analysis of 16 Longitudinal Samples by Graham Eileen K., Weston Sara J., Gerstorf Denis, Yoneda Tomiko B., Booth Tom, Beam Christopher R., Petkus Andrew J., Drewelies Johanna, Hall Andrew N., Bastarache Emily D., Estabrook Ryne, Katz Mindy J., Turiano Nicholas A., Lindenberger Ulman, Smith Jacqui, Wagner Gert G., Pedersen Nancy L., Allemand Mathias, Spiro AvronIII, Deeg Dorly J.H., Johansson Boo, Piccinin Andrea M., Lipton Richard B., Schaie K. Warner, Willis Sherry, Reynolds Chandra A., Deary Ian J., Hofer Scott M. and Mroczek Daniel K. in European Journal of Personality
13. Estimating prevalence of subjective cognitive decline in and across international cohort studies of aging: a COSMIC study
- Author
-
Röhr, Susanne, Pabst, Alexander, Riedel-Heller, Steffi G., Jessen, Frank, Turana, Yuda, Handajani, Yvonne S., Brayne, Carol, Matthews, Fiona E., Stephan, Blossom C. M., Lipton, Richard B., Katz, Mindy J., Wang, Cuiling, Guerchet, Maëlenn, Preux, Pierre-Marie, Mbelesso, Pascal, Ritchie, Karen, Ancelin, Marie-Laure, Carrière, Isabelle, Guaita, Antonio, Davin, Annalisa, Vaccaro, Roberta, Kim, Ki Woong, Han, Ji Won, Suh, Seung Wan, Shahar, Suzana, Din, Normah C., Vanoh, Divya, Van Boxtel, Martin, Köhler, Sebastian, Ganguli, Mary, Jacobsen, Erin P., Snitz, Beth E., Anstey, Kaarin J., Cherbuin, Nicolas, Kumagai, Shuzo, Chen, Sanmei, Narazaki, Kenji, Ng, Tze Pin, Gao, Qi, Gwee, Xinyi, Brodaty, Henry, Kochan, Nicole A., Trollor, Julian, Lobo, Antonio, López-Antón, Raúl, Santabárbara, Javier, Crawford, John D., Lipnicki, Darren M., and Sachdev, Perminder S.
- Subjects
Epidemiology ,Research ,Prevalence ,Subjective cognitive decline ,Individual participant data ,Data harmonization ,Cohort study ,3. Good health - Abstract
Background: Subjective cognitive decline (SCD) is recognized as a risk stage for Alzheimer’s disease (AD) and other dementias, but its prevalence is not well known. We aimed to use uniform criteria to better estimate SCD prevalence across international cohorts. Methods: We combined individual participant data for 16 cohorts from 15 countries (members of the COSMIC consortium) and used qualitative and quantitative (Item Response Theory/IRT) harmonization techniques to estimate SCD prevalence. Results: The sample comprised 39,387 cognitively unimpaired individuals above age 60. The prevalence of SCD across studies was around one quarter with both qualitative harmonization/QH (23.8%, 95%CI = 23.3–24.4%) and IRT (25.6%, 95%CI = 25.1–26.1%); however, prevalence estimates varied largely between studies (QH 6.1%, 95%CI = 5.1–7.0%, to 52.7%, 95%CI = 47.4–58.0%; IRT: 7.8%, 95%CI = 6.8–8.9%, to 52.7%, 95%CI = 47.4–58.0%). Across studies, SCD prevalence was higher in men than women, in lower levels of education, in Asian and Black African people compared to White people, in lower- and middle-income countries compared to high-income countries, and in studies conducted in later decades. Conclusions: SCD is frequent in old age. Having a quarter of older individuals with SCD warrants further investigation of its significance, as a risk stage for AD and other dementias, and of ways to help individuals with SCD who seek medical advice. Moreover, a standardized instrument to measure SCD is needed to overcome the measurement variability currently dominant in the field.
14. Age-related cognitive decline and associations with sex, education and apolipoprotein E genotype across ethnocultural groups and geographic regions: a collaborative cohort study
- Author
-
Lipnicki, Darren M, Crawford, John D, Dutta, Rajib, Thalamuthu, Anbupalam, Kochan, Nicole A, Andrews, Gavin, Lima-Costa, M Fernanda, Castro-Costa, Erico, Brayne, Carol, Matthews, Fiona E, Stephan, Blossom CM, Lipton, Richard B, Katz, Mindy J, Ritchie, Karen, Scali, Jacqueline, Ancelin, Marie-Laure, Scarmeas, Nikolaos, Yannakoulia, Mary, Dardiotis, Efthimios, Lam, Linda CW, Wong, Candy HY, Fung, Ada WT, Guaita, Antonio, Vaccaro, Roberta, Davin, Annalisa, Kim, Ki Woong, Han, Ji Won, Kim, Tae Hui, Anstey, Kaarin J, Cherbuin, Nicolas, Butterworth, Peter, Scazufca, Marcia, Kumagai, Shuzo, Chen, Sanmei, Narazaki, Kenji, Ng, Tze Pin, Gao, Qi, Reppermund, Simone, Brodaty, Henry, Lobo, Antonio, Lopez-Anton, Raúl, Santabárbara, Javier, Sachdev, Perminder S, and Cohort Studies Of Memory In An International Consortium (COSMIC)
- Subjects
Aged, 80 and over ,Male ,Genotype ,Age Factors ,Middle Aged ,Cohort Studies ,Apolipoproteins E ,Sex Factors ,Risk Factors ,Educational Status ,Humans ,Cognitive Dysfunction ,Female ,Longitudinal Studies ,10. No inequality ,Aged - Abstract
BACKGROUND: The prevalence of dementia varies around the world, potentially contributed to by international differences in rates of age-related cognitive decline. Our primary goal was to investigate how rates of age-related decline in cognitive test performance varied among international cohort studies of cognitive aging. We also determined the extent to which sex, educational attainment, and apolipoprotein E ε4 allele (APOE*4) carrier status were associated with decline. METHODS AND FINDINGS: We harmonized longitudinal data for 14 cohorts from 12 countries (Australia, Brazil, France, Greece, Hong Kong, Italy, Japan, Singapore, Spain, South Korea, United Kingdom, United States), for a total of 42,170 individuals aged 54-105 y (42% male), including 3.3% with dementia at baseline. The studies began between 1989 and 2011, with all but three ongoing, and each had 2-16 assessment waves (median = 3) and a follow-up duration of 2-15 y. We analyzed standardized Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) and memory, processing speed, language, and executive functioning test scores using linear mixed models, adjusted for sex and education, and meta-analytic techniques. Performance on all cognitive measures declined with age, with the most rapid rate of change pooled across cohorts a moderate -0.26 standard deviations per decade (SD/decade) (95% confidence interval [CI] [-0.35, -0.16], p < 0.001) for processing speed. Rates of decline accelerated slightly with age, with executive functioning showing the largest additional rate of decline with every further decade of age (-0.07 SD/decade, 95% CI [-0.10, -0.03], p = 0.002). There was a considerable degree of heterogeneity in the associations across cohorts, including a slightly faster decline (p = 0.021) on the MMSE for Asians (-0.20 SD/decade, 95% CI [-0.28, -0.12], p < 0.001) than for whites (-0.09 SD/decade, 95% CI [-0.16, -0.02], p = 0.009). Males declined on the MMSE at a slightly slower rate than females (difference = 0.023 SD/decade, 95% CI [0.011, 0.035], p < 0.001), and every additional year of education was associated with a rate of decline slightly slower for the MMSE (0.004 SD/decade less, 95% CI [0.002, 0.006], p = 0.001), but slightly faster for language (-0.007 SD/decade more, 95% CI [-0.011, -0.003], p = 0.001). APOE*4 carriers declined slightly more rapidly than non-carriers on most cognitive measures, with processing speed showing the greatest difference (-0.08 SD/decade, 95% CI [-0.15, -0.01], p = 0.019). The same overall pattern of results was found when analyses were repeated with baseline dementia cases excluded. We used only one test to represent cognitive domains, and though a prototypical one, we nevertheless urge caution in generalizing the results to domains rather than viewing them as test-specific associations. This study lacked cohorts from Africa, India, and mainland China. CONCLUSIONS: Cognitive performance declined with age, and more rapidly with increasing age, across samples from diverse ethnocultural groups and geographical regions. Associations varied across cohorts, suggesting that different rates of cognitive decline might contribute to the global variation in dementia prevalence. However, the many similarities and consistent associations with education and APOE genotype indicate a need to explore how international differences in associations with other risk factors such as genetics, cardiovascular health, and lifestyle are involved. Future studies should attempt to use multiple tests for each cognitive domain and feature populations from ethnocultural groups and geographical regions for which we lacked data.
15. Rare coding variants in PLCG2, ABI3, and TREM2 implicate microglial-mediated innate immunity in Alzheimer's disease
- Author
-
Sims, Rebecca, Van Der Lee, Sven J, Naj, Adam C, Bellenguez, Céline, Badarinarayan, Nandini, Jakobsdottir, Johanna, Kunkle, Brian W, Boland, Anne, Raybould, Rachel, Bis, Joshua C, Martin, Eden R, Grenier-Boley, Benjamin, Heilmann-Heimbach, Stefanie, Chouraki, Vincent, Kuzma, Amanda B, Sleegers, Kristel, Vronskaya, Maria, Ruiz, Agustin, Graham, Robert R, Olaso, Robert, Hoffmann, Per, Grove, Megan L, Vardarajan, Badri N, Hiltunen, Mikko, Nöthen, Markus M, White, Charles C, Hamilton-Nelson, Kara L, Epelbaum, Jacques, Maier, Wolfgang, Choi, Seung-Hoan, Beecham, Gary W, Dulary, Cécile, Herms, Stefan, Smith, Albert V, Funk, Cory C, Derbois, Céline, Forstner, Andreas J, Ahmad, Shahzad, Li, Hongdong, Bacq, Delphine, Harold, Denise, Satizabal, Claudia L, Valladares, Otto, Squassina, Alessio, Thomas, Rhodri, Brody, Jennifer A, Qu, Liming, Sánchez-Juan, Pascual, Morgan, Taniesha, Wolters, Frank J, Zhao, Yi, Garcia, Florentino Sanchez, Denning, Nicola, Fornage, Myriam, Malamon, John, Naranjo, Maria Candida Deniz, Majounie, Elisa, Mosley, Thomas H, Dombroski, Beth, Wallon, David, Lupton, Michelle K, Dupuis, Josée, Whitehead, Patrice, Fratiglioni, Laura, Medway, Christopher, Jian, Xueqiu, Mukherjee, Shubhabrata, Keller, Lina, Brown, Kristelle, Lin, Honghuang, Cantwell, Laura B, Panza, Francesco, McGuinness, Bernadette, Moreno-Grau, Sonia, Burgess, Jeremy D, Solfrizzi, Vincenzo, Proitsi, Petra, Adams, Hieab H, Allen, Mariet, Seripa, Davide, Pastor, Pau, Cupples, L Adrienne, Price, Nathan D, Hannequin, Didier, Frank-García, Ana, Levy, Daniel, Chakrabarty, Paramita, Caffarra, Paolo, Giegling, Ina, Beiser, Alexa S, Giedraitis, Vilmantas, Hampel, Harald, Garcia, Melissa E, Wang, Xue, Lannfelt, Lars, Mecocci, Patrizia, Eiriksdottir, Gudny, Crane, Paul K, Pasquier, Florence, Boccardi, Virginia, Henández, Isabel, Barber, Robert C, Scherer, Martin, Tarraga, Lluis, Adams, Perrie M, Leber, Markus, Chen, Yuning, Albert, Marilyn S, Riedel-Heller, Steffi, Emilsson, Valur, Beekly, Duane, Braae, Anne, Schmidt, Reinhold, Blacker, Deborah, Masullo, Carlo, Schmidt, Helena, Doody, Rachelle S, Spalletta, Gianfranco, Longstreth, WT, Fairchild, Thomas J, Bossù, Paola, Lopez, Oscar L, Frosch, Matthew P, Sacchinelli, Eleonora, Ghetti, Bernardino, Yang, Qiong, Huebinger, Ryan M, Jessen, Frank, Li, Shuo, Kamboh, M Ilyas, Morris, John, Sotolongo-Grau, Oscar, Katz, Mindy J, Corcoran, Chris, Dunstan, Melanie, Braddel, Amy, Thomas, Charlene, Meggy, Alun, Marshall, Rachel, Gerrish, Amy, Chapman, Jade, Aguilar, Miquel, Taylor, Sarah, Hill, Matt, Fairén, Mònica Díez, Hodges, Angela, Vellas, Bruno, Soininen, Hilkka, Kloszewska, Iwona, Daniilidou, Makrina, Uphill, James, Patel, Yogen, Hughes, Joseph T, Lord, Jenny, Turton, James, Hartmann, Annette M, Cecchetti, Roberta, Fenoglio, Chiara, Serpente, Maria, Arcaro, Marina, Caltagirone, Carlo, Orfei, Maria Donata, Ciaramella, Antonio, Pichler, Sabrina, Mayhaus, Manuel, Gu, Wei, Lleó, Alberto, Fortea, Juan, Blesa, Rafael, Barber, Imelda S, Brookes, Keeley, Cupidi, Chiara, Maletta, Raffaele Giovanni, Carrell, David, Sorbi, Sandro, Moebus, Susanne, Urbano, Maria, Pilotto, Alberto, Kornhuber, Johannes, Bosco, Paolo, Todd, Stephen, Craig, David, Johnston, Janet, Gill, Michael, Lawlor, Brian, Lynch, Aoibhinn, Fox, Nick C, Hardy, John, ARUK Consortium, Albin, Roger L, Apostolova, Liana G, Arnold, Steven E, Asthana, Sanjay, Atwood, Craig S, Baldwin, Clinton T, Barnes, Lisa L, Barral, Sandra, Beach, Thomas G, Becker, James T, Bigio, Eileen H, Bird, Thomas D, Boeve, Bradley F, Bowen, James D, Boxer, Adam, Burke, James R, Burns, Jeffrey M, Buxbaum, Joseph D, Cairns, Nigel J, Cao, Chuanhai, Carlson, Chris S, Carlsson, Cynthia M, Carney, Regina M, Carrasquillo, Minerva M, Carroll, Steven L, Diaz, Carolina Ceballos, Chui, Helena C, Clark, David G, Cribbs, David H, Crocco, Elizabeth A, DeCarli, Charles, Dick, Malcolm, Duara, Ranjan, Evans, Denis A, Faber, Kelley M, Fallon, Kenneth B, Fardo, David W, Farlow, Martin R, Ferris, Steven, Foroud, Tatiana M, Galasko, Douglas R, Gearing, Marla, Geschwind, Daniel H, Gilbert, John R, Graff-Radford, Neill R, Green, Robert C, Growdon, John H, Hamilton, Ronald L, Harrell, Lindy E, Honig, Lawrence S, Huentelman, Matthew J, Hulette, Christine M, Hyman, Bradley T, Jarvik, Gail P, Abner, Erin, Jin, Lee-Way, Jun, Gyungah, Karydas, Anna, Kaye, Jeffrey A, Kim, Ronald, Kowall, Neil W, Kramer, Joel H, LaFerla, Frank M, Lah, James J, Leverenz, James B, Levey, Allan I, Li, Ge, Lieberman, Andrew P, Lunetta, Kathryn L, Lyketsos, Constantine G, Marson, Daniel C, Martiniuk, Frank, Mash, Deborah C, Masliah, Eliezer, McCormick, Wayne C, McCurry, Susan M, McDavid, Andrew N, McKee, Ann C, Mesulam, Marsel, Miller, Bruce L, Miller, Carol A, Miller, Joshua W, Morris, John C, Murrell, Jill R, Myers, Amanda J, O'Bryant, Sid, Olichney, John M, Pankratz, Vernon S, Parisi, Joseph E, Paulson, Henry L, Perry, William, Peskind, Elaine, Pierce, Aimee, Poon, Wayne W, Potter, Huntington, Quinn, Joseph F, Raj, Ashok, Raskind, Murray, Reisberg, Barry, Reitz, Christiane, Ringman, John M, Roberson, Erik D, Rogaeva, Ekaterina, Rosen, Howard J, Rosenberg, Roger N, Sager, Mark A, Saykin, Andrew J, Schneider, Julie A, Schneider, Lon S, Seeley, William W, Smith, Amanda G, Sonnen, Joshua A, Spina, Salvatore, Stern, Robert A, Swerdlow, Russell H, Tanzi, Rudolph E, Thornton-Wells, Tricia A, Trojanowski, John Q, Troncoso, Juan C, Van Deerlin, Vivianna M, Van Eldik, Linda J, Vinters, Harry V, Vonsattel, Jean Paul, Weintraub, Sandra, Welsh-Bohmer, Kathleen A, Wilhelmsen, Kirk C, Williamson, Jennifer, Wingo, Thomas S, Woltjer, Randall L, Wright, Clinton B, Yu, Chang-En, Yu, Lei, Garzia, Fabienne, Golamaully, Feroze, Septier, Gislain, Engelborghs, Sebastien, Vandenberghe, Rik, De Deyn, Peter P, Fernadez, Carmen Muñoz, Benito, Yoland Aladro, Thonberg, Hakan, Forsell, Charlotte, Lilius, Lena, Kinhult-Stählbom, Anne, Kilander, Lena, Brundin, RoseMarie, Concari, Letizia, Helisalmi, Seppo, Koivisto, Anne Maria, Haapasalo, Annakaisa, Dermecourt, Vincent, Fievet, Nathalie, Hanon, Olivier, Dufouil, Carole, Brice, Alexis, Ritchie, Karen, Dubois, Bruno, Himali, Jayanadra J, Keene, C Dirk, Tschanz, JoAnn, Fitzpatrick, Annette L, Kukull, Walter A, Norton, Maria, Aspelund, Thor, Larson, Eric B, Munger, Ron, Rotter, Jerome I, Lipton, Richard B, Bullido, María J, Hofman, Albert, Montine, Thomas J, Coto, Eliecer, Boerwinkle, Eric, Petersen, Ronald C, Alvarez, Victoria, Rivadeneira, Fernando, Reiman, Eric M, Gallo, Maura, O'Donnell, Christopher J, Reisch, Joan S, Bruni, Amalia Cecilia, Royall, Donald R, Dichgans, Martin, Sano, Mary, Galimberti, Daniela, St George-Hyslop, Peter, Scarpini, Elio, Tsuang, Debby W, Mancuso, Michelangelo, Bonuccelli, Ubaldo, Winslow, Ashley R, Daniele, Antonio, Wu, Chuang-Kuo, GERAD/PERADES, CHARGE, Peters, Oliver, Nacmias, Benedetta, Riemenschneider, Matthias, Heun, Reinhard, Brayne, Carol, Rubinsztein, David C, Bras, Jose, Guerreiro, Rita, Al-Chalabi, Ammar, Shaw, Christopher E, Collinge, John, Mann, David, Tsolaki, Magda, Clarimón, Jordi, Sussams, Rebecca, Lovestone, Simon, O'Donovan, Michael C, Owen, Michael J, Behrens, Timothy W, Mead, Simon, Goate, Alison M, Uitterlinden, Andre G, Holmes, Clive, Cruchaga, Carlos, Ingelsson, Martin, Bennett, David A, Powell, John, Golde, Todd E, Graff, Caroline, De Jager, Philip L, Morgan, Kevin, Ertekin-Taner, Nilufer, Combarros, Onofre, Psaty, Bruce M, Passmore, Peter, Younkin, Steven G, Berr, Claudine, Gudnason, Vilmundur, Rujescu, Dan, Dickson, Dennis W, Dartigues, Jean-François, DeStefano, Anita L, Ortega-Cubero, Sara, Hakonarson, Hakon, Campion, Dominique, Boada, Merce, Kauwe, John Keoni, Farrer, Lindsay A, Van Broeckhoven, Christine, Ikram, M Arfan, Jones, Lesley, Haines, Jonathan L, Tzourio, Christophe, Launer, Lenore J, Escott-Price, Valentina, Mayeux, Richard, Deleuze, Jean-François, Amin, Najaf, Holmans, Peter A, Pericak-Vance, Margaret A, Amouyel, Philippe, Van Duijn, Cornelia M, Ramirez, Alfredo, Wang, Li-San, Lambert, Jean-Charles, Seshadri, Sudha, Williams, Julie, and Schellenberg, Gerard D
- Subjects
Membrane Glycoproteins ,Genotype ,Sequence Homology, Amino Acid ,Phospholipase C gamma ,Gene Expression Profiling ,Polymorphism, Single Nucleotide ,Immunity, Innate ,Linkage Disequilibrium ,3. Good health ,Gene Frequency ,Alzheimer Disease ,Case-Control Studies ,Odds Ratio ,Humans ,Exome ,Genetic Predisposition to Disease ,Amino Acid Sequence ,Microglia ,Protein Interaction Maps ,Receptors, Immunologic ,Adaptor Proteins, Signal Transducing - Abstract
We identified rare coding variants associated with Alzheimer's disease in a three-stage case-control study of 85,133 subjects. In stage 1, we genotyped 34,174 samples using a whole-exome microarray. In stage 2, we tested associated variants (P < 1 × 10-4) in 35,962 independent samples using de novo genotyping and imputed genotypes. In stage 3, we used an additional 14,997 samples to test the most significant stage 2 associations (P < 5 × 10-8) using imputed genotypes. We observed three new genome-wide significant nonsynonymous variants associated with Alzheimer's disease: a protective variant in PLCG2 (rs72824905: p.Pro522Arg, P = 5.38 × 10-10, odds ratio (OR) = 0.68, minor allele frequency (MAF)cases = 0.0059, MAFcontrols = 0.0093), a risk variant in ABI3 (rs616338: p.Ser209Phe, P = 4.56 × 10-10, OR = 1.43, MAFcases = 0.011, MAFcontrols = 0.008), and a new genome-wide significant variant in TREM2 (rs143332484: p.Arg62His, P = 1.55 × 10-14, OR = 1.67, MAFcases = 0.0143, MAFcontrols = 0.0089), a known susceptibility gene for Alzheimer's disease. These protein-altering changes are in genes highly expressed in microglia and highlight an immune-related protein-protein interaction network enriched for previously identified risk genes in Alzheimer's disease. These genetic findings provide additional evidence that the microglia-mediated innate immune response contributes directly to the development of Alzheimer's disease.
16. Determinants of cognitive performance and decline in 20 diverse ethno-regional groups: A COSMIC collaboration cohort study
- Author
-
Lipnicki, Darren M, Makkar, Steve R, Crawford, John D, Thalamuthu, Anbupalam, Kochan, Nicole A, Lima-Costa, Maria Fernanda, Castro-Costa, Erico, Ferri, Cleusa Pinheiro, Brayne, Carol, Stephan, Blossom, Llibre-Rodriguez, Juan J, Llibre-Guerra, Jorge J, Valhuerdi-Cepero, Adolfo J, Lipton, Richard B, Katz, Mindy J, Derby, Carol A, Ritchie, Karen, Ancelin, Marie-Laure, Carrière, Isabelle, Scarmeas, Nikolaos, Yannakoulia, Mary, Hadjigeorgiou, Georgios M, Lam, Linda, Chan, Wai-Chi, Fung, Ada, Guaita, Antonio, Vaccaro, Roberta, Davin, Annalisa, Kim, Ki Woong, Han, Ji Won, Suh, Seung Wan, Riedel-Heller, Steffi G, Roehr, Susanne, Pabst, Alexander, Van Boxtel, Martin, Köhler, Sebastian, Deckers, Kay, Ganguli, Mary, Jacobsen, Erin P, Hughes, Tiffany F, Anstey, Kaarin J, Cherbuin, Nicolas, Haan, Mary N, Aiello, Allison E, Dang, Kristina, Kumagai, Shuzo, Chen, Tao, Narazaki, Kenji, Ng, Tze Pin, Gao, Qi, Nyunt, Ma Shwe Zin, Scazufca, Marcia, Brodaty, Henry, Numbers, Katya, Trollor, Julian N, Meguro, Kenichi, Yamaguchi, Satoshi, Ishii, Hiroshi, Lobo, Antonio, Lopez-Anton, Raul, Santabárbara, Javier, Leung, Yvonne, Lo, Jessica W, Popovic, Gordana, Sachdev, Perminder S, and For Cohort Studies Of Memory In An International Consortium (COSMIC)
- Subjects
2. Zero hunger ,Aged, 80 and over ,Male ,Smoking ,Age Factors ,Comorbidity ,Middle Aged ,Risk Assessment ,3. Good health ,Stroke ,Cognition ,Risk Factors ,Diabetes Mellitus ,Ethnicity ,Humans ,Cognitive Dysfunction ,Female ,Exercise ,Health Education ,Aged - Abstract
BACKGROUND: With no effective treatments for cognitive decline or dementia, improving the evidence base for modifiable risk factors is a research priority. This study investigated associations between risk factors and late-life cognitive decline on a global scale, including comparisons between ethno-regional groups. METHODS AND FINDINGS: We harmonized longitudinal data from 20 population-based cohorts from 15 countries over 5 continents, including 48,522 individuals (58.4% women) aged 54-105 (mean = 72.7) years and without dementia at baseline. Studies had 2-15 years of follow-up. The risk factors investigated were age, sex, education, alcohol consumption, anxiety, apolipoprotein E ε4 allele (APOE*4) status, atrial fibrillation, blood pressure and pulse pressure, body mass index, cardiovascular disease, depression, diabetes, self-rated health, high cholesterol, hypertension, peripheral vascular disease, physical activity, smoking, and history of stroke. Associations with risk factors were determined for a global cognitive composite outcome (memory, language, processing speed, and executive functioning tests) and Mini-Mental State Examination score. Individual participant data meta-analyses of multivariable linear mixed model results pooled across cohorts revealed that for at least 1 cognitive outcome, age (B = -0.1, SE = 0.01), APOE*4 carriage (B = -0.31, SE = 0.11), depression (B = -0.11, SE = 0.06), diabetes (B = -0.23, SE = 0.10), current smoking (B = -0.20, SE = 0.08), and history of stroke (B = -0.22, SE = 0.09) were independently associated with poorer cognitive performance (p < 0.05 for all), and higher levels of education (B = 0.12, SE = 0.02) and vigorous physical activity (B = 0.17, SE = 0.06) were associated with better performance (p < 0.01 for both). Age (B = -0.07, SE = 0.01), APOE*4 carriage (B = -0.41, SE = 0.18), and diabetes (B = -0.18, SE = 0.10) were independently associated with faster cognitive decline (p < 0.05 for all). Different effects between Asian people and white people included stronger associations for Asian people between ever smoking and poorer cognition (group by risk factor interaction: B = -0.24, SE = 0.12), and between diabetes and cognitive decline (B = -0.66, SE = 0.27; p < 0.05 for both). Limitations of our study include a loss or distortion of risk factor data with harmonization, and not investigating factors at midlife. CONCLUSIONS: These results suggest that education, smoking, physical activity, diabetes, and stroke are all modifiable factors associated with cognitive decline. If these factors are determined to be causal, controlling them could minimize worldwide levels of cognitive decline. However, any global prevention strategy may need to consider ethno-regional differences.
17. Open Practices Disclosure Form, per2259-sup-0002-Open_Practices_Disclosure_Form - Trajectories of Big Five Personality Traits: A Coordinated Analysis of 16 Longitudinal Samples
- Author
-
Graham, Eileen K., Weston, Sara J., Gerstorf, Denis, Yoneda, Tomiko B., Booth, Tom, Beam, Christopher R., Petkus, Andrew J., Drewelies, Johanna, Hall, Andrew N., Bastarache, Emily D., Estabrook, Ryne, Katz, Mindy J., Turiano, Nicholas A., Ulman Lindenberger, Smith, Jacqui, Wagner, Gert G., Pedersen, Nancy L., Allemand, Mathias, Avron Spiro, Deeg, Dorly J.H., Johansson, Boo, Piccinin, Andrea M., Lipton, Richard B., K. Warner Schaie, Willis, Sherry, Reynolds, Chandra A., Deary, Ian J., Hofer, Scott M., and Mroczek, Daniel K.
- Subjects
FOS: Psychology ,170199 Psychology not elsewhere classified ,16. Peace & justice - Abstract
Open Practices Disclosure Form, per2259-sup-0002-Open_Practices_Disclosure_Form for Trajectories of Big Five Personality Traits: A Coordinated Analysis of 16 Longitudinal Samples by Graham Eileen K., Weston Sara J., Gerstorf Denis, Yoneda Tomiko B., Booth Tom, Beam Christopher R., Petkus Andrew J., Drewelies Johanna, Hall Andrew N., Bastarache Emily D., Estabrook Ryne, Katz Mindy J., Turiano Nicholas A., Lindenberger Ulman, Smith Jacqui, Wagner Gert G., Pedersen Nancy L., Allemand Mathias, Spiro AvronIII, Deeg Dorly J.H., Johansson Boo, Piccinin Andrea M., Lipton Richard B., Schaie K. Warner, Willis Sherry, Reynolds Chandra A., Deary Ian J., Hofer Scott M. and Mroczek Daniel K. in European Journal of Personality
18. Age-related cognitive decline and associations with sex, education and apolipoprotein E genotype across ethnocultural groups and geographic regions: a collaborative cohort study
- Author
-
Lipnick, Darren M., Crawford, John D., Dutta, Rajib, Thalamuthu, Anbupalam, Kochan, Nicole A., Andrews, Gavin, Lima-Costa, M. Fernanda, Castro-Costa, Erico, Brayne, Carol, Matthews, Fiona E., Stephan, Blossom C. M., Lipton, Richard B., Katz, Mindy J., Ritchie, Karen, Scali, Jacqueline, Ancelin, Marie-Laure, Scarmeas, Nikolaos, Yannakoulia, Mary, Dardiotis, Efthimios, Lam, Linda C. W., Wong, Candy H. Y., Fung, Ada W. T., Guaita, Antonio, Vaccaro, Roberta, Davin, Annalisa, Kim, Ki Woong, Han, Ji Won, Kim, Tae Hui, Anstey, Kaarin J., Cherbuin, Nicolas, Butterworth, Peter, Scazufca, Marcia, Kumagai, Shuzo, Chen, Sanmei, Narazaki, Kenji, Ng, Tze Pin, Gao, Qi, Reppermund, Simone, Brodaty, Henry, Lobo, Antonio, Lopez-Anton, Rau´L, Santaba´Rbara, Javier, Sachdev, Perminder S., and Cohort Studies Of Memory In An International Consortium
- Subjects
Aging ,Apolipoprotein E gene ,FOS: Biological sciences ,Genetics ,Cognitive neuroscience ,10. No inequality ,3. Good health - Abstract
Background The prevalence of dementia varies around the world, potentially contributed to by international differences in rates of age-related cognitive decline. Our primary goal was to investigate how rates of age-related decline in cognitive test performance varied among international cohort studies of cognitive aging. We also determined the extent to which sex, educational attainment, and apolipoprotein E ε4 allele (APOE*4) carrier status were associated with decline. Methods and findings We harmonized longitudinal data for 14 cohorts from 12 countries (Australia, Brazil, France, Greece, Hong Kong, Italy, Japan, Singapore, Spain, South Korea, United Kingdom, United States), for a total of 42,170 individuals aged 54–105 y (42% male), including 3.3% with dementia at baseline. The studies began between 1989 and 2011, with all but three ongoing, and each had 2–16 assessment waves (median = 3) and a follow-up duration of 2–15 y. We analyzed standardized Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) and memory, processing speed, language, and executive functioning test scores using linear mixed models, adjusted for sex and education, and meta-analytic techniques. Performance on all cognitive measures declined with age, with the most rapid rate of change pooled across cohorts a moderate -0.26 standard deviations per decade (SD/decade) (95% confidence interval [CI] [-0.35, -0.16], p < 0.001) for processing speed. Rates of decline accelerated slightly with age, with executive functioning showing the largest additional rate of decline with every further decade of age (-0.07 SD/decade, 95% CI [-0.10, -0.03], p = 0.002). There was a considerable degree of heterogeneity in the associations across cohorts, including a slightly faster decline (p = 0.021) on the MMSE for Asians (-0.20 SD/decade, 95% CI [-0.28, -0.12], p < 0.001) than for whites (-0.09 SD/decade, 95% CI [-0.16, -0.02], p = 0.009). Males declined on the MMSE at a slightly slower rate than females (difference = 0.023 SD/decade, 95% CI [0.011, 0.035], p < 0.001), and every additional year of education was associated with a rate of decline slightly slower for the MMSE (0.004 SD/decade less, 95% CI [0.002, 0.006], p = 0.001), but slightly faster for language (-0.007 SD/decade more, 95% CI [-0.011, -0.003], p = 0.001). APOE*4 carriers declined slightly more rapidly than non-carriers on most cognitive measures, with processing speed showing the greatest difference (-0.08 SD/decade, 95% CI [-0.15, -0.01], p = 0.019). The same overall pattern of results was found when analyses were repeated with baseline dementia cases excluded. We used only one test to represent cognitive domains, and though a prototypical one, we nevertheless urge caution in generalizing the results to domains rather than viewing them as test-specific associations. This study lacked cohorts from Africa, India, and mainland China. Conclusions Cognitive performance declined with age, and more rapidly with increasing age, across samples from diverse ethnocultural groups and geographical regions. Associations varied across cohorts, suggesting that different rates of cognitive decline might contribute to the global variation in dementia prevalence. However, the many similarities and consistent associations with education and APOE genotype indicate a need to explore how international differences in associations with other risk factors such as genetics, cardiovascular health, and lifestyle are involved. Future studies should attempt to use multiple tests for each cognitive domain and feature populations from ethnocultural groups and geographical regions for which we lacked data.
19. Age-related cognitive decline and associations with sex, education and apolipoprotein E genotype across ethnocultural groups and geographic regions: a collaborative cohort study
- Author
-
Lipnicki, Darren M, Crawford, John D, Dutta, Rajib, Thalamuthu, Anbupalam, Kochan, Nicole A, Andrews, Gavin, Fernanda Lima-Costa, M, Castro-Costa, Erico, Brayne, Carol, Matthews, Fiona E, Stephan, Blossom CM, Lipton, Richard B, Katz, Mindy J, Ritchie, Karen, Scali, Jacqueline, Ancelin, Marie-Laure, Scarmeas, Nikolaos, Yannakoulia, Mary, Dardiotis, Efthimios, Lam, Linda CW, Wong, Candy HY, Fung, Ada WT, Guaita, Antonio, Vaccaro, Roberta, Davin, Annalisa, Kim, Ki Woong, Han, Ji Won, Kim, Tae Hui, Anstey, Kaarin J, Cherbuin, Nicolas, Butterworth, Peter, Scazufca, Marcia, Kumagai, Shuzo, Chen, Sanmei, Narazaki, Kenji, Ng, Tze Pin, Gao, Qi, Reppermund, Simone, Brodaty, Henry, Lobo, Antonio, Lopez-Anton, Raul, Santabarbara, Javier, Sachdev, Perminder S, and Int, Cohort Studies Memory
- Subjects
10. No inequality ,3. Good health - Abstract
Background The prevalence of dementia varies around the world, potentially contributed to by international differences in rates of age-related cognitive decline. Our primary goal was to investigate how rates of age-related decline in cognitive test performance varied among international cohort studies of cognitive aging. We also determined the extent to which sex, educational attainment, and apolipoprotein E ε4 allele (APOE*4) carrier status were associated with decline. Methods and findings We harmonized longitudinal data for 14 cohorts from 12 countries (Australia, Brazil, France, Greece, Hong Kong, Italy, Japan, Singapore, Spain, South Korea, United Kingdom, United States), for a total of 42,170 individuals aged 54–105 y (42% male), including 3.3% with dementia at baseline. The studies began between 1989 and 2011, with all but three ongoing, and each had 2–16 assessment waves (median = 3) and a follow-up duration of 2–15 y. We analyzed standardized Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) and memory, processing speed, language, and executive functioning test scores using linear mixed models, adjusted for sex and education, and meta-analytic techniques. Performance on all cognitive measures declined with age, with the most rapid rate of change pooled across cohorts a moderate -0.26 standard deviations per decade (SD/decade) (95% confidence interval [CI] [-0.35, -0.16], p < 0.001) for processing speed. Rates of decline accelerated slightly with age, with executive functioning showing the largest additional rate of decline with every further decade of age (-0.07 SD/decade, 95% CI [-0.10, -0.03], p = 0.002). There was a considerable degree of heterogeneity in the associations across cohorts, including a slightly faster decline (p = 0.021) on the MMSE for Asians (-0.20 SD/decade, 95% CI [-0.28, -0.12], p < 0.001) than for whites (-0.09 SD/decade, 95% CI [-0.16, -0.02], p = 0.009). Males declined on the MMSE at a slightly slower rate than females (difference = 0.023 SD/decade, 95% CI [0.011, 0.035], p < 0.001), and every additional year of education was associated with a rate of decline slightly slower for the MMSE (0.004 SD/decade less, 95% CI [0.002, 0.006], p = 0.001), but slightly faster for language (-0.007 SD/decade more, 95% CI [-0.011, -0.003], p = 0.001). APOE*4 carriers declined slightly more rapidly than non-carriers on most cognitive measures, with processing speed showing the greatest difference (-0.08 SD/decade, 95% CI [-0.15, -0.01], p = 0.019). The same overall pattern of results was found when analyses were repeated with baseline dementia cases excluded. We used only one test to represent cognitive domains, and though a prototypical one, we nevertheless urge caution in generalizing the results to domains rather than viewing them as test-specific associations. This study lacked cohorts from Africa, India, and mainland China. Conclusions Cognitive performance declined with age, and more rapidly with increasing age, across samples from diverse ethnocultural groups and geographical regions. Associations varied across cohorts, suggesting that different rates of cognitive decline might contribute to the global variation in dementia prevalence. However, the many similarities and consistent associations with education and APOE genotype indicate a need to explore how international differences in associations with other risk factors such as genetics, cardiovascular health, and lifestyle are involved. Future studies should attempt to use multiple tests for each cognitive domain and feature populations from ethnocultural groups and geographical regions for which we lacked data.
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.