617 results on '"False Belief"'
Search Results
2. Knowledge, false belief, and reductio
- Author
-
Matt Leonard
- Subjects
Reductio ad absurdum ,Philosophy ,False belief ,Health Policy ,Subject (philosophy) ,Inference ,Proposition ,Psychology ,Epistemology - Abstract
Recently, a number of cases have been proposed which seem to show that – contrary to widely held opinion – a subject can inferentially come to know some proposition p from an inference which relies...
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
3. REDES SOCIALES, VEJEZ Y POBREZA EN EL DEPARTAMENTO DEL QUINDÍO: El funcionamiento de las redes sociales de personas mayores de 60 años en condición de pobreza
- Author
-
César Augusto Gómez-Santos and Leonardo Iván Quintana-Urrea
- Subjects
H1-99 ,Extreme poverty ,education.field_of_study ,Poverty ,pobreza ,False belief ,Welfare economics ,Population ,General Medicine ,General Works ,Social sciences (General) ,Social group ,apoyo social ,redes sociales ,T1-995 ,Elderly people ,Sociology ,apoyo informal ,envejecimiento ,education ,Vejez ,Technology (General) ,Selection system - Abstract
En este trabajo se da cuenta del funcionamiento de las redes sociales de las personas mayores de 60 años que residen en contextos urbanos y que se encuentran en situación de pobreza o de pobreza extrema. En él se presenta parte de los resultados del proyecto del mismo nombre que ha sido financiado por la Vicerrectoría de investigaciones de la Universidad del Quindío. La muestra para el estudio ha sido constituida por 591 personas mayores de 60 años que están clasificados en los niveles 1 y 2 del Sistema de Selección de Beneficiarios (SISBEN), a quienes se les ha indagado acerca de la percepción que tienen del funcionamiento de sus redes sociales informales tanto familiares como no familiares, así como los apoyos que reciben y que provienen de las redes formales de apoyo. Se evidenció a través de la realización de este trabajo que las redes sociales se afectan debido a las precarias condiciones de vida que los adultos mayores presentan, pero que a pesar de ello las transferencias y los apoyos recíprocos ayudan a cubrir algunas de las necesidades que experimenta este grupo poblacional. Se desvirtúa además la falsa creencia de que los adultos mayores juegan un papel pasivo como receptor de ayudas de parte de las redes sociales, contrario a lo que se piensa se constata la reciprocidad de los intercambios de las personas mayores a pesar de las condiciones de pobreza, llegando a ser en muchos casos un proveedor de ayudas hacia los demás miembros de la red social.
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
4. True Beliefs versus Reality with autism
- Author
-
Samuel, Steven
- Subjects
FOS: Psychology ,non-mental representations ,false belief ,Social Psychology ,Cognitive Psychology ,true belief ,Psychology ,autism ,Social and Behavioral Sciences ,theory of mind - Abstract
A test of whether true beliefs and related non-mental representations are processed differently from facts, and whether autistic traits modulate any such difference.
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
5. Replication attempt of Rubio-Fernandez & Geurts (2013)
- Author
-
Priewasser, Beate and Fowles, Franziska
- Subjects
false belief ,theory of mind - Abstract
Direct replication of Rubio-Fernandez and Geurts' (2013) modified False Belief Task and a Close replication of their comparison between a traditional False Belief Task and their modified version
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
6. Investigating the neural substrates of the continued influence of misinformation
- Author
-
Lewandowsky, Stephan and Gordon, Andrew
- Subjects
memory ,false belief ,memory updating ,continued influence ,fMRI ,ComputingMilieux_COMPUTERSANDSOCIETY ,correction ,misinformation ,retraction - Abstract
This is an updated registration of the project that can be found at https://osf.io/te8hq/ - Our earlier registration contained the wrong p value for the cluster-significance threshold (page 11) Our aim is to gain a better understanding of the cognitive mechanisms underlying continued reliance on misinformation. To this end we will present subjects with brief fictional news reports that may or may not require their memory to be updated. Reliance on misinformation will be assessed through a decision task involving the clarification of a picture as a 'match' or 'mismatch' for the preceding report.
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
7. Bilingualism & Theory of Mind
- Author
-
Samuel, Steven
- Subjects
Social Psychology ,Bilingualism ,Theory of Mind ,Cognitive Psychology ,Experimental Analysis of Behavior ,Linguistics ,Applied Linguistics ,Social and Behavioral Sciences ,FOS: Psychology ,Psycholinguistics and Neurolinguistics ,Second Language Effect ,FOS: Languages and literature ,Psychology ,Egocentric Bias ,False Belief - Abstract
Testing whether bilingualism and/or the prior embodiment of an agent attenuates the attribution of one's own uncertainty, based on privileged information, to that person.
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
8. Convergent validity of a multi-modal assessment of alexithymia and its relationship with an extensive battery of social cognition
- Author
-
Rösch, Sarah, Puhlmann, Lara, and Preckel, Katrin
- Subjects
Alexithymia ,Director's Task ,Social Cognition ,False Belief ,Dominance analysis ,exploratory factor analysis ,Psychiatry and Psychology ,RMET ,Social and Behavioral Sciences ,FOS: Psychology ,Cyberball ,Medicine and Health Sciences ,Psychology ,TAS ,TSIA ,BVAQ - Abstract
This project aims to advance our understanding of the construct alexithymia through two research goals: Firstly, identify the factors underlying multi-modally assessed alexithymia (self-report and interview). Secondly, examine the relation between the factors underlying alexithymia and an extensive battery of social cognition (comprised of the recognition of other people's affective mental states, representation of other people's cognitive mental states, visual perspective-taking, and affective distress of social rejection).
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
9. Embodiment, Bilingualism, & Theory of Mind
- Author
-
Samuel, Steven
- Subjects
FOS: Psychology ,false belief ,FOS: Languages and literature ,Cognitive Psychology ,Psychology ,Linguistics ,bilingualism ,Social and Behavioral Sciences ,egocentrism ,embodiment ,theory of mind - Abstract
A study to investigate the influences of prior embodiment and bilingualism on the ability to take another's mental perspective and limit egocentric biases.
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
10. Minimally Verbal False Belief Task (Unseen Displacement)
- Author
-
Pyers, Jennie and Gale, Elaine
- Subjects
False Belief ,Minimally Verbal False Belief Task ,Theory of Mind - Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
11. Do autistic adults spontaneously reason about belief? Further investigation
- Author
-
Partington, Hannah, White, Sarah, and Wu, Ruihan
- Subjects
Implicit mentalizing ,Anticipatory-looking ,Theory of mind ,Autism ,False belief ,Eye-tracking ,Spontaneous mentalizing - Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
12. Can distributional semantics explain performance on the false belief task?
- Author
-
Trott, Sean, Jones, Cameron, Chang, Tyler, Michaelov, James, and Bergen, Ben
- Subjects
Computational Linguistics ,FOS: Psychology ,false belief ,Psycholinguistics and Neurolinguistics ,Artificial Intelligence and Robotics ,Computer Sciences ,FOS: Languages and literature ,Physical Sciences and Mathematics ,Cognitive Psychology ,neural language model ,Psychology ,Linguistics ,Social and Behavioral Sciences - Abstract
In a previous pre-registration (https://osf.io/agqwv), we asked whether GPT-3, a neural language model, showed sensitivity to implied belief states. We found that it did: in a false belief task paradigm, GPT-3 assigned significantly different probability estimates to different search locations as a function of a character's belief states. In other words, GPT-3 was "sensitive" to false belief information, in the sense that its predictions about upcoming words/locations changed according to previous language in the passage that indicated whether the character knew or didn't know about an object's location change. The goal of the present work is to ask whether, and to what extent, human behavior on this task is explained by GPT-3's output, as opposed to the key experimental manipulation of Condition. The theoretical motivation for this work is to test the hypothesis that human behavior on the false belief task requires some sort of *additional* cognitive resource or information source that cannot be obtained from distributional statistics alone. Thus, GPT-3's output serves as a kind of "baseline" to ask about the *sufficiency* (in principle) of distributional statistics, as captured by GPT-3, in explaining sensitivity to false belief on this task. The hypothesis space is as follows: H0 (null): Human behavior is not sensitive to the false belief manipulation. H1 (baseline): Human sensitivity to false belief does not exceed GPT-3’s sensitivity. In other words, distributional statistics are sufficient to explain human behavior on the task, and Condition does not explain additional variance. H2 (>baseline): Human behavior is sensitive to false belief, above and beyond variance in GPT-3 log-odds. Distributional statistics are *insufficient* to explain human behavior on the task, because Condition explains additional variance.
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
13. Do dogs understand false beliefs of humans?
- Author
-
Lonardo, Lucrezia, Marchand, Sarah, Völter, Christoph, and Huber, Ludwig
- Subjects
FOS: Psychology ,dogs ,false belief ,comparative cognition ,Psychology ,social cognition ,Social and Behavioral Sciences ,human activities ,Comparative Psychology ,theory of mind - Abstract
With the present study, we aim at investigating whether pet dogs (Canis familiaris) understand human false beliefs and whether they adjust their behaviour accordingly. In particular, we are going to address dogs’ ability to distinguish between true and false beliefs of a human experimenter (the “player”) in a novel non-verbal task: a social game with rules established by the human player. In a post-test condition, we will target explicitly dogs’ reaction to the player’s ignorance in the same task. The game is designed to elicit dogs’ ability to take into account the human player’s mental states because the “correct” response that dogs should exhibit to get a reward will depend on the player’s beliefs. Dogs will first learn a go/no-go task during a short training phase. In this, the presence of food in one opaque container (the “go box”) will be associated to the “go” response: dogs have to retrieve the food autonomously from the box within 10 seconds. The presence of food in a second opaque container (the “wait box”) will be associated to the “no-go” response: dogs have to wait for 10 seconds without touching any of the boxes in order to get a different piece of food from the player. If dogs make a mistake (moving when food is in the wait box or not moving for 10 seconds when the food is in the go box), the player will remove both boxes from the dogs’ reach. Once dogs have reached predefined learning criteria (see below), they will be ready for the test phase, in which test trials will be interspersed among training trials to keep up dogs’ motivation and to re-establish the rules of the game. During test trials, dogs will witness for the first time that a second experimenter, “the hider”, transfers the reward from the wait to the go box either in the presence (true belief condition) or in the absence (false belief condition) of the player.
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
14. Can We Boost Preschoolers’ Inhibitory Performance Just by Changing the Way They Respond?
- Author
-
Daniel J. Carroll, Andrew Simpson, and Emma Blakey
- Subjects
Counterfactual thinking ,False belief ,Mean age ,Correct response ,Education ,Child, Preschool ,Pediatrics, Perinatology and Child Health ,Inhibitory control ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,Arrow ,Humans ,Child ,Psychology ,Problem Solving ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Changing the way children make their response appears to sometimes, but not always, boost their inhibitory control – though interpreting existing findings is hampered by inconsistent methods and results. This study investigated the effects of delaying, and changing, the means of responding. Ninety-six preschoolers (mean age 46 months) completed tasks assessing inhibitory control, counterfactual reasoning, strategic reasoning, and false belief understanding. Children responded either immediately or after a delay, and either by pointing with their finger, or with a hand-held arrow. Delaying boosted performance on all tasks except false belief understanding; arrow-pointing only improved strategic reasoning. It is suggested that delay helps children work out the correct response; it is unlikely to help on tasks where this requirement is absent.
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
15. False Belief Understanding in Deaf Children With Cochlear Implants
- Author
-
Joanna Wysocka, Małgorzata Zgoda, Maciej Haman, Elżbieta Włodarczyk, Magdalena Krysztofiak, Joanna Wójcik, Agnieszka Pluta, and Karolina Golec
- Subjects
medicine.medical_specialty ,Deception ,Empirical Manuscript ,Deafness ,Audiology ,Education ,030507 speech-language pathology & audiology ,03 medical and health sciences ,Speech and Hearing ,Theory of mind ,Assistive technology ,medicine ,Cognitive development ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Child ,False belief ,Communication ,05 social sciences ,Sentence comprehension test ,Cochlear Implantation ,AcademicSubjects/SOC02080 ,Comprehension ,Cochlear Implants ,Child, Preschool ,Listening comprehension ,0305 other medical science ,Psychology ,Sentence ,050104 developmental & child psychology - Abstract
Theory of mind (ToM) is crucial for social interactions. Previous research has indicated that deaf and hard-of-hearing children born into hearing families (DoH) are at risk of delayed ToM development. However, it is unclear whether this is the case for DoH children who receive cochlear implants (CIs) before and around the second year of life. The present study aimed to investigate false belief understanding (FBU) in DoH children with CIs. The relationships between false belief task (FBT) performance, sentence comprehension, age at implantation, duration of CI use, and Speech Recognition Threshold were explored. A total of 94 children with typical levels of hearing (TH) and 45 DoH children (age range: 3–8), who received their first CI between 6 and 27 months of age, were tested on the FBT and a sentence comprehension test. Results showed that 4- and 5-year-old children with CIs performed significantly worse than their peers with TH on the FBT; 6- to 8-year-old children with CIs performed similarly to age-matched children with TH. Age at implantation and duration of CI use were correlated with sentence comprehension but not with the FBT. The results indicated that FBU was delayed until the age of 6 years in most of children with CIs.
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
16. Advanced theory of mind in children with mild intellectual disability and deaf or hard of hearing children: A two‐year longitudinal study in middle childhood
- Author
-
Joanna Smogorzewska and Christopher Osterhaus
- Subjects
medicine.medical_specialty ,Longitudinal study ,Deception ,Theory of Mind ,Deafness ,Audiology ,Middle childhood ,050105 experimental psychology ,Typically developing ,Hearing ,Developmental Neuroscience ,Intellectual Disability ,Theory of mind ,Intellectual disability ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,medicine ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Longitudinal Studies ,Child ,Aged ,False belief ,Communication ,05 social sciences ,medicine.disease ,Large sample ,Persons With Hearing Impairments ,Psychology ,050104 developmental & child psychology - Abstract
The present study investigates the development of advanced theory of mind (AToM) among typically developing (TD) children, children with mild intellectual disability (MID), and deaf or hard of hearing (DHH) children. The 2-year longitudinal study comprised three waves and included a large sample of children from Poland in middle childhood aged around 7.5-9.5 years (N = 779; M = 7.7, SD = 0.92 at wave 1). The analysis of children's understanding of second-order false belief and the Faux-Pas Recognition Test showed that TD children outperformed children with MID and DHH children on both measures. At 7.5 years, almost 60% of the TD children correctly solved the second-order false belief task; correct performance at 7.5 years in children with MID and DHH children was 27 and 38% respectively. Two years later, correct performance rose to 80% (TD children), 45% (children with MID), and 63% (DHH children). Despite these differences, the speed of AToM development did not differ across the groups. The development of faux-pas recognition followed a non-linear pattern, with TD children showing no further significant development after mid-elementary school. Our findings show differences in AToM development between TD children, children with MID, and DHH children, and they suggest that children's development of AToM may follow different developmental pathways, depending on the aspect of AToM under study.
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
17. Early mindreading scale: From joint attention to false-belief understanding
- Author
-
Arkadiusz Białek, Julian Zubek, Mateusz Blukacz, Marta Białecka-Pikul, Małgorzata Stępień-Nycz, and Magdalena Kosno
- Subjects
Joint attention ,Social Psychology ,False belief ,05 social sciences ,false-belief understanding ,050109 social psychology ,mindreading ability ,Developmental psychology ,Scale (social sciences) ,continuous development ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,latent trait modelling ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Psychology ,050104 developmental & child psychology ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
The current research aims at constructing a developmentally sensitive mindreading scale (i.e., a battery of tasks measuring different aspects of mindreading ability in children from 1 to 3.5 years of age). Over 300 Polish children were tested at six-month intervals with 48 different tasks designed to measure mindreading ability (for a total of six measurement occasions, T1-T6; NT1 = 357; MageT1 = 52,30 weeks; SD = 1,73; 201 (56%) boys, 156 (44%) girls). Using latent trait modelling, a latent mindreading factor was established. Support was found for satisfactory reliability of the scale that ultimately consisted of 31 tasks. The tasks were age-matched, as the information curves for latent factors for each of the six age ranges and the reliability of the composite constructs were mostly acceptable. The validity was also confirmed using correlational analyses, and the continuous development of mindreading was observed over the tested age ranges: from joint attention, through using gestures in communication and visual perspective taking, to false-belief understanding. The proposed scale is suitable for use in the investigation of the general pattern of the development of, and individual differences in, mindreading ability.
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
18. The impact of theory of mind barriers in interpreting illustrations used in primary school early readers: four brief case studies of false-belief scenarios
- Author
-
Kelley Donner
- Subjects
Early childhood education ,Visual Arts and Performing Arts ,False belief ,Communication ,Visual literacy ,Primary education ,medicine.disease ,Education ,Theory of mind ,Mathematics education ,Learning to read ,medicine ,Autism ,Psychology ,Inclusion (education) - Abstract
Theory of mind skills are critical to understanding and interpreting many illustrations in primary school literature especially those which are used in material for the purpose of learning to read....
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
19. The Effects of Mother’s Parenting Attitude and Humor Sense on Children’s False Belief Understanding
- Author
-
Yumi Kim
- Subjects
False belief ,Sense of humor ,Emotional expression ,Psychology ,Social psychology - Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
20. Little pranksters: Inhibitory control mediates the association between false belief understanding and practical joking in young children
- Author
-
Zhenlin Wang and Lamei Wang
- Subjects
Developmental Neuroscience ,Social Psychology ,Joke ,False belief ,Theory of mind ,Inhibitory control ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,Life-span and Life-course Studies ,Association (psychology) ,Psychology ,Social psychology ,Social Sciences (miscellaneous) ,Education - Abstract
To successfully pull a practical joke on someone, children need to understand that their victims do not know what they themselves know, be able to intentionally manipulate others’ beliefs, and maintain a straight face to safeguard the integrity of the joke. This study examined the relationship between children’s developing theory of mind (ToM), inhibitory control, and their ability to pull a practical joke. Ninety-five children between ages 2 and 6 participated in, among other measures, a practical joke task that required them to knowingly give one of the experimenters a gift box containing a rubber insect. Results showed that children’s ability to pull a practical joke was significantly related to their age, false belief understanding (FBU), inhibitory control, and verbal ability. Children with more siblings were more likely to successfully pull a practical joke. Most importantly, inhibitory control was shown to mediate the relation between FBU and practical joking. The findings provide evidence that practical joking as an example of ToM use is effortful.
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
21. A Cooperation Advantage for Theory of Mind in Children and Adults
- Author
-
Lily Tsoi, Adam Waytz, J. Kiley Hamlin, Andrew Scott Baron, and Liane Lee Young
- Subjects
Social Psychology ,False belief ,Theory of mind ,05 social sciences ,Social change ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Psychology ,050105 experimental psychology ,050104 developmental & child psychology ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Three studies test whether people engage in mental state reasoning or theory of mind (ToM) differently across two fundamental social contexts: cooperation and competition. Study 1 examines how children with an emerging understanding of false beliefs deploy ToM across these contexts. We find that young preschool children are better able to plant false beliefs in others' minds in a cooperative versus competitive context; this difference does not emerge for other cognitive capacities tested (e.g., executive functioning, memory). Studies 2a and 2b reveal the same systematic difference in adults' ToM for cooperation and competition, even after accounting for relevant predictors (e.g., preference for a task condition, feelings about deception). Together, these findings provide initial evidence for enhanced ToM for cooperation versus competition in early development and also adulthood.
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
22. Verbal mediation of theory of mind in verbal adolescents with autism spectrum disorder
- Author
-
Inge-Marie Eigsti and Christina A. Irvine
- Subjects
050101 languages & linguistics ,Linguistics and Language ,genetic structures ,Logical reasoning ,False belief ,Mediation (Marxist theory and media studies) ,05 social sciences ,nutritional and metabolic diseases ,Cognition ,medicine.disease ,behavioral disciplines and activities ,Article ,050105 experimental psychology ,Language and Linguistics ,Linguistics ,Education ,Developmental psychology ,Nonverbal communication ,Autism spectrum disorder ,Theory of mind ,mental disorders ,medicine ,Autism ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Psychology - Abstract
This study tests the role of verbal mediation during theory of mind processing in autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Adolescents with ASD or typical development completed a false belief task while simultaneously performing a verbal or nonverbal load task. There was no group difference in false belief accuracy; however, under verbal load, the ASD group was relatively less efficient, with slower reaction times, in false belief compared to true belief trials. Faster false belief task performance under verbal but not nonverbal load was associated with pragmatic language ability for the ASD group only. Results were consistent with the theory that there are two (implicit, nonverbal and explicit, verbal) processes that support cognitive reasoning about other people’s minds (Apperly & Butterfill, 2009), and that people with ASD rely more on the explicit system. Verbal mediation may be critical for false belief understanding in individuals with ASD, but not typical development.
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
23. Epistemic Norms, the False Belief Requirement, and Love
- Author
-
J. Spencer Atkins
- Subjects
Philosophy ,False belief ,Psychology ,Epistemology - Abstract
Many authors have argued that epistemic rationality sometimes comes into conflict with our relationships. Although Sarah Stroud and Simon Keller argue that friendships sometimes require bad epistemic agency, their proposals do not go far enough. I argue here for a more radical claim—romantic love sometimes requires we form beliefs that are false. Lovers stand in a special position with one another; they owe things to one another that they do not owe to others. Such demands hold for beliefs as well. Two facets of love ground what I call the false belief requirement , or the demand to form false beliefs when it is for the good of the beloved: the demand to love for the right reasons and the demand to refrain from doxastic wronging. Since truth is indispensable to epistemic rationality, the requirement to believe falsely, consequently, undermines truth norms. I demonstrate that, when the false belief requirement obtains, there is an irreconcilable conflict between love and truth norms of epistemic rationality: we must forsake one, at least at the time, for the other.
- Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
24. How children approach the false belief test: social development, pragmatics, and the assembly of Theory of Mind
- Author
-
Marco Fenici
- Subjects
Philosophy of mind ,Comprehension ,Philosophy ,False belief ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,Theory of mind ,Social change ,Critical factors ,Pragmatics ,Psychology ,On Language ,humanities ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Evidence from the knowledge access task and the diverse belief task suggests that, before age four, children may find it difficult to attribute false beliefs to others, despite demonstrating a basic comprehension of the concept of belief. Challenging this view, this article assumes a sociopragmatic perspective on language to argue that even children younger than four may not understand at all the concept of belief but may nevertheless master naively the pragmatics of belief reports in specific conversational contexts. The proposal suggests a novel interpretation of both the reasons behind younger children’s difficulty with (elicited-response) false belief tasks, and the critical factors enabling children’s success in them. On the one hand, it proposes that younger children fail (elicited-response) false belief tasks because they do not understand the importance of focusing on an agent’s (verbally ascribed) mental states to infer her practical commitments. On the other hand, it suggests that children’s active engagement in conversations where the caregiver credits an agent with a belief is the critical factor integrating their initially scattered mastery of the pragmatics of belief reports, teaches them to track belief reports across contexts, and accordingly shapes their understanding of belief as a representational mental state.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
25. Knowing minds: Linking early perspective taking and later metacognitive insight
- Author
-
Sunae Kim, Markus Paulus, Daniela Kloo, Susanne Kristen-Antonow, and Beate Sodian
- Subjects
Longitudinal study ,False belief ,Intelligence ,05 social sciences ,Outcome measures ,Metacognition ,Verbal reasoning ,050105 experimental psychology ,Task (project management) ,Knowledge ,Developmental Neuroscience ,Perspective-taking ,Theory of mind ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,Humans ,Female ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Longitudinal Studies ,Child ,Psychology ,050104 developmental & child psychology ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Recent metacognitive research using a partial knowledge task indicates that a firm understanding of 'knowing about knowing' develops surprisingly late, at around 6 years of age. To reveal the mechanisms subserving this development, the partial knowledge task was used in a longitudinal study with 67 children (33 girls) as an outcome measure at 5;9 (years;months). In addition, first- and second-order false belief was assessed at 4;2, 5;0, and 5;9. At 2;6, perspective taking and executive abilities were evaluated. Metacognition at 5;9 was correlated with earlier theory of mind and perspective taking - even when verbal intelligence and executive abilities were partialled out. This highlights the importance of perspective taking for the development of an understanding of one's own mind.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
26. Machine Learning Pathway for Harnessing Knowledge and Data in Material Processing
- Author
-
Ning Sun, Diran Apelian, Rasika Karkare, Randy Paffenroth, and Adam Kopper
- Subjects
Materials science ,Materials processing ,Emerging technologies ,business.industry ,False belief ,Metals and Alloys ,Context (language use) ,Machine learning ,computer.software_genre ,Industrial and Manufacturing Engineering ,Field (computer science) ,Mechanics of Materials ,Metallic materials ,Materials Chemistry ,Artificial intelligence ,business ,computer - Abstract
Artificial intelligence (AI) is integral to Industry 4.0 and the evolution of smart factories. To realize this future, material processing industries are embarking on adopting AI technologies into their enterprise and plants; however, like all new technologies, there is always the potential for misuse or the false belief that the outcomes are reliable. The goal of this paper is to provide context for the application of machine learning to materials processing. The general landscapes of data science and materials processing are presented, using the foundry and the metal casting industry as an exemplar. The challenges that exist with typical foundry data are that the data are unbalanced, semi-supervised, heterogeneous, and limited in sample size. Data science methods to address these issues are presented and discussed. The elements of a data science project are outlined and illustrated by a case study using sand cast foundry data. Finally, a prospective view of the application of data science to materials processing and the impact this will have in the field are given.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
27. Bilinguals’ inhibitory control and attentional processes in a visual perceptual task
- Author
-
Christina Marx, Steven Stirk, Peter J. B. Hancock, and Marina C. Wimmer
- Subjects
Visual perception ,False belief ,Psychological research ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,General Medicine ,bilingualism ,inhibition ,Reversal frequency ,attention ,Task (project management) ,executive control ,Arts and Humanities (miscellaneous) ,Inhibitory control ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,reversal ,ambiguous figures ,Psychology ,Neuroscience of multilingualism ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
The aim was to examine theories of bilingual inhibitory control superiority in the visual domain. In an ambiguous figure task, the ability to reverse (switch) interpretations (e.g., duck-rabbit) was examined in 3–5-year-olds bilinguals and monolinguals (N = 67). Bilingualism was no performance predictor in conceptual tasks (Droodle task, false belief task, ambiguous figures production task) that did not pose inhibitory demands. Bilinguals outperformed monolinguals in the ability to reverse, suggesting superior inhibitory capacity per se. Once reversal was experienced there was no difference in the time it took to reverse or reversal frequency between bilinguals and monolinguals. Bayesian analyses confirmed statistical result patterns. Findings support the established view of bilinguals’ superior domain-general inhibitory control. This might be brought to bear by attending the environment differently.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
28. Investor's Pessimistic and False Belief About Trustworthiness and Stake Size in Trust Decision 1
- Author
-
Yoshio Kamijo, Kiri Kuroda, and Tatsuya Kameda
- Subjects
Trustworthiness ,Dictator game ,False belief ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Pessimism ,Psychology ,Social psychology ,Reciprocity (evolution) ,General Psychology ,media_common - Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
29. Two systems for thinking about others’ thoughts in the developing brain
- Author
-
Nikolaus Steinbeis, Charlotte Grosse Wiesmann, Angela D. Friederici, and Tania Singer
- Subjects
Male ,Dissociation (neuropsychology) ,False belief ,Temporoparietal junction ,Theory of Mind ,Precuneus ,brain development ,behavioral disciplines and activities ,050105 experimental psychology ,Cortical thickness ,Thinking ,false belief ,03 medical and health sciences ,Nonverbal communication ,0302 clinical medicine ,Supramarginal gyrus ,Theory of mind ,Image Processing, Computer-Assisted ,medicine ,Humans ,Encoding (semiotics) ,Interpersonal Relations ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Gray matter ,Problem Solving ,Brain Mapping ,Multidisciplinary ,05 social sciences ,Brain ,gray matter ,Biological Sciences ,cortical thickness ,Magnetic Resonance Imaging ,Brain development ,Social relation ,medicine.anatomical_structure ,Child, Preschool ,Psychological and Cognitive Sciences ,Female ,Nerve Net ,Psychology ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Significance The ability to reason about other people’s thoughts and beliefs characterizes the complex social interaction among humans. This ability, called Theory of Mind (ToM), has long been argued to develop around 4 y when children start explicitly reasoning about others' beliefs. However, when tested nonverbally, infants already show action expectations congruent with others’ beliefs before the age of 2 y. Do these behaviors reflect different systems for understanding others’ minds—an early and a later developing one—or when does ToM develop? We show that these abilities are supported by the maturation of independent brain networks, suggesting different systems for explicit verbal ToM and early nonverbal action expectations., Human social interaction crucially relies on the ability to infer what other people think. Referred to as Theory of Mind (ToM), this ability has long been argued to emerge around 4 y of age when children start passing traditional verbal ToM tasks. This developmental dogma has recently been questioned by nonverbal ToM tasks passed by infants younger than 2 y of age. How do young children solve these tests, and what is their relation to the later-developing verbal ToM reasoning? Are there two different systems for nonverbal and verbal ToM, and when is the developmental onset of mature adult ToM? To address these questions, we related markers of cortical brain structure (i.e., cortical thickness and surface area) of 3- and 4-y-old children to their performance in novel nonverbal and traditional verbal TM tasks. We showed that verbal ToM reasoning was supported by cortical surface area and thickness of the precuneus and temporoparietal junction, classically involved in ToM in adults. Nonverbal ToM reasoning, in contrast, was supported by the cortical structure of a distinct and independent neural network including the supramarginal gyrus also involved in emotional and visual perspective taking, action observation, and social attention or encoding biases. This neural dissociation suggests two systems for reasoning about others’ minds—mature verbal ToM that emerges around 4 y of age, whereas nonverbal ToM tasks rely on different earlier-developing possibly social-cognitive processes.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
30. Childhood Abuse and Adult Sociocognitive Skills: Distinguishing Between Self and Other Following Early Trauma
- Author
-
Anna R. Hudson, Marcel Brass, Sylvia Verbeke, Sven C. Mueller, Kaat Van der Jeught, Lize De Coster, and Hanne Spoormans
- Subjects
Adult ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Theory of Mind ,Empathy ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Clinical Psychology ,Developmental psychology ,03 medical and health sciences ,bepress|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Psychology|Clinical Psychology ,0302 clinical medicine ,Theory of mind ,medicine ,Humans ,Child ,Childhood abuse ,Applied Psychology ,Depression (differential diagnoses) ,Social functioning ,media_common ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences|Clinical Psychology|Trauma and Stress ,False belief ,medicine.disease ,030227 psychiatry ,PsyArXiv|Social and Behavioral Sciences ,Clinical Psychology ,bepress|Social and Behavioral Sciences ,Facilitation ,Female ,Psychology ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Psychological trauma - Abstract
Experience of childhood abuse impairs complex social functioning in children; however, much less is known about its effects on basic socio-cognitive processes and even fewer studies have investigated these in adult survivors. Using two behavioral tasks, this study investigated spontaneous theory of mind (ToM) and imitative behavior in 41 women with childhood abuse (CA) and 26 unaffected comparison women (UC). In the spontaneous ToM task, UCs showed a larger ToM index than CAs, indicating more facilitation by knowledge of another’s false belief. In the imitation-inhibition task, CAs experienced less interference than UCs when observing another’s incongruent movements. After controlling for depression, differences in ToM became marginally significant, yet remained highly significant for inhibiting imitative behavior. The findings suggest childhood abuse survivors have altered perspective-taking, and are less influenced by others’ perspectives, potentially due to changes in self-other distinction. Clinical implications regarding therapeutic practice with survivors of childhood abuse are discussed.
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
31. Conditionals: Truth, safety, and success
- Author
-
Richard Bradley, Hugh Mellor, and Apollo - University of Cambridge Repository
- Subjects
Linguistics and Language ,5004 Religious Studies ,SUBMITTED ARTICLES ,Antecedent (logic) ,SUBMITTED ARTICLE ,actions ,5003 Philosophy ,Inference ,0603 philosophy, ethics and religion ,050105 experimental psychology ,Language and Linguistics ,Truth value ,conditionals ,BC Logic ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,suppositions ,truth values ,False belief ,05 social sciences ,06 humanities and the arts ,inferential dispositions ,Philosophy ,Action (philosophy) ,50 Philosophy and Religious Studies ,If and only if ,060302 philosophy ,Psychology ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Whether I take some action that aims at desired consequence C depends on whether or not I take it to be true that if I so act, I will bring C about and that if I do not, I will fail to. And the action will succeed if and only if my beliefs are true. We argue that two theses follow: (I) To believe a conditional is to be disposed to infer its consequent from the truth of its antecedent, and (II) The conditional is true iff the inference would not make a true belief in the antecedent cause a false belief in the consequent.
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
32. Asociación entre las funciones ejecutivas y la teoría de la mente en niños: Evidencia empírica e implicaciones teóricas
- Author
-
Gómez-Tabares, Anyerson S. and Departamentos de la UMH::Psicología de la Salud
- Subjects
159.9 - Psicología ,false belief ,executive control ,lectura de mentes ,neurodevelopmental disorders ,memoria de trabajo ,falsa creencia ,trastornos del neurodesarrollo ,control ejecutivo ,working memory ,mind reading - Abstract
Estudios previos han encontrado que el funcionamiento ejecutivo (FE) se relaciona con la teoría de la mente (TdM). Sin embargo, la direccionalidad y fuerza de este vínculo sigue siendo un tema de debate en la literatura actual. El objetivo de este trabajo fue analizar las perspectivas de estudio y evidencia empírica sobre la direccionalidad y fuerza del co-desarrollo de la FE y la TdM en niños. La búsqueda bibliográfica se efectuó en Web of Science. Para el análisis se utilizaron Sci2 Tool y Gephi. El análisis de clúster mostró tres perspectivas de estudio enfocadas a la relación entre el rendimiento de las FEs y la TdM en niños con trastorno de déficit de atención e hiperactividad (TDAH) (1), trastorno del espectro autista (TEA) (2) y desarrollo típico (DT) (3). Se encontró un patrón consistente de asociación entre FE y TdM en niños con TEA, TDAH y DT. Los hallazgos longitudinales mostraron que la asociación entre FE temprana y TdM tardía, incluida la comprensión de falsas creencias, es más fuerte que la asociación inversa y tiende a consolidarse con la edad, lo cual indica una direccionalidad FE→TdM, mas no TdM→FE, y no se explica mejor por el efecto de las demandas ejecutivas planteadas en las tareas de TdM. En conjunto, la evidencia apoya los relatos teóricos de la “emergencia” y el “enriquecimiento” al considerar que las FEs en el neurodesarrollo temprano del niño están implicadas ontogénicamente en la adquisición, consolidación y cambio de las capacidades de comprensión de estados psicológicos en los demás Association between executive functions and theory of mind in children: Empirical evidence and theoretical implications. Previous studies have found that executive functioning (EF) is related to theory of mind (ToM). However, the directionality and strength of this link remain a topic of debate in the current literature. The aim of this paper was to analyze the study perspectives and empirical evidence on the directionality and strength of the co-development of EF and ToM in children. The literature search was performed in Web of Science. Sci2 Tool and Gephi were used for the analysis. Cluster analysis showed three study perspectives focused on the relationship between EF performance and ToM in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) (1), autism spectrum disorder (ASD) (2) and typical development (TD) (3). We found a consistent pattern of association between EF and ToM in children with ASD, ADHD, and TD. Longitudinal findings showed that the association between early EF and late ToM, including false belief comprehension, is stronger than the inverse association and tends to consolidate with age, indicating an EF→ToM, but not ToM→EF directionality, and is not better explained by the effect of executive demands posed in ToM tasks. Taken together, the evidence supports the “emergence” and “enrichment” theoretical accounts in considering that EFs in early child neurodevelopment are ontogenetically implicated in the acquisition, consolidation, and change of psychological state comprehension abilities in others.
- Published
- 2022
33. Live Sandbox task
- Author
-
Samuel, Steven
- Subjects
FOS: Psychology ,False Belief ,Social Psychology ,Cognitive Psychology ,Theory of Mind ,Psychology ,Social and Behavioral Sciences - Abstract
Testing the hypothesis that adults simulate others' beliefs.
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
34. Infants differentiate between successful and failed communication in a false-belief context
- Author
-
Cornelia Schulze and David Buttelmann
- Subjects
False belief ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,Infant ,Humans ,Female ,Intention ,Awareness ,Psychology ,150 Psychology ,Social relation ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Communication is based on social interaction, that is, interlocutors sharing attention to the intentions that they communicate about. In this study, we asked whether infants are aware of the fact that for information to be transferred, both interlocutors need to be present and share attention. Using a violation-of-expectation paradigm created to test infants’ understanding of others’ false beliefs, we asked whether 18-month-olds (n=84) understood that correcting an agent’s false belief via communication requires that the agent discerns the verbal statement. Participants saw how an agent put a toy into a box and left. An assistant then moved the toy into a cup. The intervention phase varied between three conditions: The agent and the assistant communicated about the actual location of the toy (full-communication), the agent was absent during the assistant’s statement (incomplete-communication) or no communication took place (no-communication). At test, the agent reached into either the box or the cup. When no communication took place, infants expected the agent to search the toy at the original location. Full communication resulted in infants’ expectations that the recipient’s mental states were altered, that is, the infants expected her to search the toy at the actual location. In contrast, incomplete communication did not yield clear expectations. Eighteen-month-olds thus seem to understand that for information to be transferred, it is a precondition that the recipient of the communicative act must be present and share attention during the communicator’s statement. Only then communication can change a recipient’s mental state.
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
35. The Paradox of False Belief Understanding
- Author
-
Julia Wolf
- Subjects
Social cognition ,False belief ,Cognition ,Situational ethics ,Psychology ,Cognitive psychology - Published
- 2021
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
36. Why Do Children Who Solve False Belief Tasks Begin to Find True Belief Control Tasks Difficult? A Test of Pragmatic Performance Factors in Theory of Mind Tasks
- Author
-
Lydia P, Schidelko, Michael, Huemer, Lara M, Schröder, Anna S, Lueb, Josef, Perner, and Hannes, Rakoczy
- Subjects
knowledge ,false belief ,Theory of Mind ,true belief ,Psychology ,false sign task ,Brief Research Report ,pragmatics - Abstract
The litmus test for the development of a metarepresentational Theory of Mind is the false belief (FB) task in which children have to represent how another agent misrepresents the world. Children typically start mastering this task around age four. Recently, however, a puzzling finding has emerged: Once children master the FB task, they begin to fail true belief (TB) control tasks. Pragmatic accounts assume that the TB task is pragmatically confusing because it poses a trivial academic test question about a rational agent’s perspective; and we do not normally engage in such discourse about subjective mental perspectives unless there is at least the possibility of error or deviance. The lack of such an obvious possibility in the TB task implicates that there might be some hidden perspective difference and thus makes the task confusing. In the present study, we test the pragmatic account by administering to 3- to 6-year-olds (N = 88) TB and FB tasks and structurally analogous true and false sign (TS/FS) tasks. The belief and sign tasks are matched in terms of representational and metarepresentational complexity; the crucial difference is that TS tasks do not implicate an alternative non-mental perspective and should thus be less pragmatically confusing than TB tasks. The results show parallel and correlated development in FB and FS tasks, replicate the puzzling performance pattern in TB tasks, but show no trace of this in TS tasks. Taken together, these results speak in favor of the pragmatic performance account.
- Published
- 2021
37. The posterior cerebellum and temporoparietal junction support explicit learning of social belief sequences
- Author
-
Frank Van Overwalle, Elien Heleven, Kris Baetens, Qianying Ma, Chris Baeken, Natacha Deroost, Naem Haihambo, Min Pu, Psychology, Faculty of Psychology and Educational Sciences, Brain, Body and Cognition, Pharmaceutical and Pharmacological Sciences, Brussels University Consultation Center, Clinical sciences, Neuroprotection & Neuromodulation, and Psychiatry
- Subjects
Serial reaction time ,Social Cognition ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,Temporoparietal junction ,Clinical psychology ,Task (project management) ,Behavioral Neuroscience ,Social actions ,false belief ,Psychiatry and Mental health ,medicine.anatomical_structure ,Mentalization ,Orientation (mental) ,Social cognition ,Cerebellum ,serial reaction time task ,medicine ,Explicit sequence learning ,Reaction Time ,Humans ,Learning ,Psychology ,Control (linguistics) ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
This study tests the hypothesis that the posterior cerebellum is involved in social cognition by identifying and automatizing sequences of social actions. We applied a belief serial reaction time task (Belief SRT task), which requires mentalizing about two protagonists' beliefs about how many flowers they receive. The protagonists' beliefs could either be true or false depending on their orientation (true belief: oriented towards and directly observing the flowers; or false belief: oriented away and knowing only prior information about flowers). A Control SRT task was created by replacing protagonists and their beliefs with shapes and colors. Participants were explicitly told that there was a standard sequence related to the two protagonists' belief orientations (Belief SRT task) or the shapes' colors (Control SRT task). Both tasks included a Training phase where the standard sequence was repeated and a Test phase where this standard sequence was interrupted by random sequences. As hypothesized, compared with the Control SRT task, the Belief SRT task recruited the posterior cerebellar Crus II and the temporoparietal junction (TPJ) more. Faster response times were correlated with less Crus II activation and with more TPJ activation, suggesting that the Crus II supported automatizing the belief sequence while the TPJ supported inferring the protagonists' beliefs. Also as hypothesized, compared with an implicit version of the Belief SRT task (i.e., participants did not know about the existence of sequences; Ma, Pu, et al., 2021b), the cerebellar Crus I &II was engaged less during initial training and automatic application of the sequence, and the cortical TPJ was activated more in processing random sequences.
- Published
- 2021
38. Why Do Young Children Look so Smart and Older Children Look so Dumb on True Belief Control Tasks? An Investigation of Pragmatic Performance Factors
- Author
-
Hannes Rakoczy and Nese Oktay-Gür
- Subjects
Age differences ,False belief ,05 social sciences ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Litmus ,050105 experimental psychology ,Psychiatry and Mental health ,Theory of mind ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,Cognitive development ,Task analysis ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Heuristics ,Psychology ,Competence (human resources) ,050104 developmental & child psychology ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
When do children acquire a meta-representational Theory of Mind? False Belief (FB) tasks have become the litmus test to answer this question. In such tasks, subjects must ascribe a non-veri...
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
39. False‐Belief Understanding
- Author
-
Christopher N. Maymon, Claudie M. Peloquin, Katheryn Edwards, Schyana Sivanantham, and Jason Low
- Subjects
False belief ,Cognitive development ,Cognition ,Psychology ,Cognitive psychology - Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
40. UNDERSTANDING FALSE BELIEF TASKS BY BILINGUAL MINORITY CHILDREN IN BULGARIA
- Author
-
Hristo Kyuchukov
- Subjects
False belief ,Psychology ,Cognitive psychology - Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
41. False Belief and Harmful Cultural Practices of Chhaupadi System in Nepal
- Author
-
Kalpana Khadka and Tej Bahadur Karki
- Subjects
False belief ,Psychology ,Social psychology - Abstract
Belief system is strongly associated with the socio-cultural practices, family orientation, social environment, schooling of people. Some of the false belief and harmful practices are associated with the Chhaupadi system of Nepal. Chhaupadi is a social tradition related to "menstrual taboo" in the western part of Nepal for Hindu women, which prohibits them from participating in normal family activities during a menstruation period, as they are considered "impure". The main aim of this study is to identify the false belief and harmful cultural practices of Chhaupadi system in Nepal. The study was conducted among the girl students of grade 9 and 10 by using the structured questionnaire survey. The study found that there are many false belief system and harmful cultural practices were associated with Chhaupadi system. The menstrual women were not allowed to enter into the kitchen, not allowed to cook food, not allowed to touch the drinking water, not allowed to enter into the religious places and not allowed to participate in the social functions. The false belief system and harmful cultural practices had negatively affected in the health and hygiene and personal and professional growth and development of women and girls, so such belief system and practices should be removed from the society though the series of orientation and wider level of community discussion.
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
42. Epicurus on the false belief that sense-impressions conflict
- Author
-
James Warren
- Subjects
Philosophy ,False belief ,Classics ,Humanities - Abstract
Selon les epicuriens, toutes les impressions des sens sont vraies et la raison trouve en elles son fondement. Nombreux sont ceux, cependant, qui croient que les impressions des sens ne sont pas toutes vraies. Les epicuriens expliquent cette croyance de la facon suivante : la source de cette erreur est souvent la croyance que les impressions des sens peuvent se contredire. Mais cette derniere croyance resulte souvent de ce que les epicuriens tiennent pour notre tendance naturelle, et frequemment utile, a generaliser et a former des opinions par extrapolation a partir des impressions de nos sens.
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
43. The Relationship Between Nonlinguistic False Belief Tasks, Visual Processing Capacity, and Language Ability in Children With Specific Language Impairment
- Author
-
Su Yeon Lee and Dong Sun Yim
- Subjects
Visual processing ,Language ability ,False belief ,Theory of mind ,medicine ,Specific language impairment ,medicine.disease ,Psychology ,Cognitive psychology - Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
44. An Analysis on the Developmental Aspects of Children’s Theory of Mind
- Author
-
Yu, Sora and Parkchanok
- Subjects
False belief ,Theory of mind ,Psychology ,Cognitive psychology - Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
45. False-belief of college students in computerized picture-book reading
- Author
-
Motoo Mitsuda
- Subjects
Picture books ,False belief ,Reading (process) ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Mathematics education ,Psychology ,media_common - Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
46. A new model of the development of deception: Disentangling the role of false‐belief understanding in deceptive ability
- Author
-
Joanna Jakubowska and Marta Białecka-Pikul
- Subjects
Sociology and Political Science ,False belief ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Deception ,intentionality ,Developmental psychology ,deception ,lying ,Intentionality ,false‐belief understanding ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,Psychology ,Lying ,Social Sciences (miscellaneous) ,media_common ,Cognitive psychology - Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
47. Gaining or losing wisdom: Developmental trends in theory of mind in old age
- Author
-
Zijing Hong, Fei Gao, Shen Liu, Wei Zhou, Lin Zhang, and Zhongchen Mou
- Subjects
False belief ,05 social sciences ,050109 social psychology ,050105 experimental psychology ,Developmental psychology ,Task (project management) ,Social group ,Consistency (negotiation) ,Social cognition ,Theory of mind ,Faux pas ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Psychology ,General Psychology - Abstract
Theory of mind (ToM) is a tool in the field of social cognition that is employed to judge and predict others’ intentions, motives, and psychological states. At present, studies on the development of ToM in older adults have offered some meaningful insights. However, researchers still lack an overall understanding of the differences of ToM abilities based on different age brackets. Accordingly, the present study utilized a variety of tasks to investigate the development of ToM in the elderly at different ages. The results demonstrated that: (1) The ToM abilities showed a declined tendency with the increasing age. (2) While overall, different types of ToM tasks varied in accordance with each age brackets. In the double emotion task, the development of ToM abilities showed no difference among three groups of people aged 65–74, 75–79, 80–84, while in comparison, the people aged 85–89 had a notable decline in ToM abilities. In the double bluff task, the ToM abilities of people aged 65–74 were notably stronger than that of the people aged 85–89, while the other groups did not varied much. In the false belief task, ToM abilities of people aged 65–74 were significantly stronger than that of people aged 65–74 and 85–89, while the other groups did not varied much. In the second-order false belief task, the ToM abilities of people aged 65–74 and 75–79 were greatly stronger than that of other two groups that had similar results. In the faux pas task, the ToM abilities of people aged 65–74 were notably stronger than that of other three groups, people aged 75–79 than that of 85–89, while other groups showed no difference. In general, the ToM abilities of the elderly displayed the inner consistency of development, but manifested particularity in different tasks.
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
48. False belief reasoning in pronoun resolution during on-line sentence processing
- Author
-
Hui Fang Or
- Subjects
Computer science ,False belief ,business.industry ,Artificial intelligence ,Line (text file) ,computer.software_genre ,business ,computer ,Sentence processing ,Natural language processing ,Pronoun resolution ,Linguistics - Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
49. Progressing from an implicit to an explicit false belief understanding: A matter of executive control?
- Author
-
Beate Sodian, Daniela Kloo, and Susanne Kristen-Antonow
- Subjects
Longitudinal study ,Social Psychology ,False belief ,05 social sciences ,Executive functions ,Child development ,050105 experimental psychology ,Education ,Developmental Neuroscience ,Theory of mind ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,Cognitive development ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Life-span and Life-course Studies ,Relation (history of concept) ,Control (linguistics) ,Psychology ,Social Sciences (miscellaneous) ,050104 developmental & child psychology ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
In a longitudinal study ( N = 54), we investigated the developmental relation between children’s implicit and explicit theory of mind and executive functions. We found that implicit false belief understanding at 18 months was correlated with explicit false belief understanding at 4 to 5 years of age, with the latter being closely related to second-order false belief understanding at 5 years of age. Also, replicating a number of studies, explicit first- and second-order false belief understanding, in contrast to implicit false belief understanding, were related to executive functioning. This indicates that executive functions play a role in standard explicit false belief tasks, but not in implicit false belief understanding. We argue that spontaneous, implicit false belief understanding does not require conscious control, whereas explicit false belief understanding is based on conscious, reflective processing. In sum, we suggest a developmental enrichment account of theory of mind development, with belief processing becoming increasingly reflective and controlled with advancing age.
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
50. Neural correlates of theory-of-mind are associated with variation in children’s everyday social cognition
- Author
-
David Dodell-Feder, Christine I. Hooker, Sarah Hope Lincoln, Cora E. Mukerji, and Charles A. Nelson
- Subjects
Male ,Adolescent ,effective connectivity ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Temporoparietal junction ,Theory of Mind ,Precuneus ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Empathy ,050105 experimental psychology ,false belief ,03 medical and health sciences ,Cognition ,0302 clinical medicine ,Social cognition ,Theory of mind ,medicine ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Child ,Social Behavior ,media_common ,Brain Mapping ,Neural correlates of consciousness ,medicine.diagnostic_test ,perspective-taking ,fMRI ,05 social sciences ,Brain ,General Medicine ,Magnetic Resonance Imaging ,medicine.anatomical_structure ,Social Perception ,Female ,Original Article ,Nerve Net ,Functional magnetic resonance imaging ,Psychology ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Social cognitive theory ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Theory of mind (ToM), the capacity to reason about others’ mental states, is central to healthy social development. Neural mechanisms supporting ToM may contribute to individual differences in children’s social cognitive behavior. Employing a false belief functional magnetic resonance imaging paradigm, we identified patterns of neural activity and connectivity elicited by ToM reasoning in school-age children (N = 32, ages 9–13). Next, we tested relations between these neural ToM correlates and children’s everyday social cognition. Several key nodes of the neural ToM network showed greater activity when reasoning about false beliefs (ToM condition) vs non-mentalistic false content (control condition), including the bilateral temporoparietal junction (RTPJ and LTPJ), precuneus (PC) and right superior temporal sulcus. In addition, children demonstrated task-modulated changes in connectivity among these regions to support ToM relative to the control condition. ToM-related activity in the PC was negatively associated with variation in multiple aspects of children’s social cognitive behavior. Together, these findings elucidate how nodes of the ToM network act and interact to support false belief reasoning in school-age children and suggest that neural ToM mechanisms are linked to variation in everyday social cognition.
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.