8 results on '"Eric J. Vanman"'
Search Results
2. Empathy moderates the relationship between cognitive load and prosocial behaviour
- Author
-
Roger S. Gamble, Julie D. Henry, and Eric J. Vanman
- Subjects
Medicine ,Science - Abstract
Abstract Cognitive load reduces both empathy and prosocial behaviour. However, studies demonstrating these effects have induced cognitive load in a temporally limited, artificial manner that fails to capture real-world cognitive load. Drawing from cognitive load theory, we investigated whether naturally occurring cognitive load from the ongoing COVID-19 pandemic moderated the relationship between empathy and prosocial behaviour (operationalised as support for public health measures). This large study in an Australian sample (N = 600) identified negative relationships between pandemic fatigue, empathy for people vulnerable to COVID-19, and prosocial behaviour, and a positive relationship between empathy and prosocial behaviour. Additionally, we found that the negative effect of the pandemic on prosocial behaviour depended on empathy for vulnerable others, with pandemic fatigue’s effects lowest for those with the highest empathy. These findings highlight the interrelationships of cognitive load and empathy, and the potential value of eliciting empathy to ease the impact of real-world cognitive load on prosocial behaviour.
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
3. A functionalist approach to online trolling
- Author
-
Lewis Nitschinsk, Stephanie J. Tobin, and Eric J. Vanman
- Subjects
trolling ,sadism ,psychopathy ,anonymity ,online ,motivations ,Psychology ,BF1-990 - Abstract
Online trolling is often linked to sadism and psychopathy. Yet, little research has assessed why people high in these traits seek online environments to achieve their nefarious goals. We employ a functionalist approach to examine whether people high in sadism and psychopathy are motivated to seek the affordances of online environments (e.g., anonymity) to reveal their malevolent self-aspects by engaging in trolling behavior. A sample of 515 university undergraduates (Mage = 20.47) read vignettes depicting trolling incidents and rated the acceptability of the perpetrators’ actions and whether they had ever written similar comments. Participants then completed measures of psychopathy, sadism, and toxic anonymous motivations. We find that toxic anonymous motivations partially mediate the relationship between psychopathy and sadism, and online trolling. Whereas trolling is often understood through its underlying personality traits, toxic motivations to seek anonymity may be a more proximal predictor of who is likely to troll online.
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
4. The Relationship of Gender Roles and Beliefs to Crying in an International Sample
- Author
-
Leah S. Sharman, Genevieve A. Dingle, Marc Baker, Agneta Fischer, Asmir Gračanin, Igor Kardum, Harry Manley, Kunalan Manokara, Sirirada Pattara-angkoon, Ad J. J. M. Vingerhoets, and Eric J. Vanman
- Subjects
crying ,gender roles ,social support ,beliefs about crying ,emotion regulation ,Psychology ,BF1-990 - Abstract
This study aimed to (1) investigate the variation in self ascription to gender roles and attitudes toward gender roles across countries and its associations with crying behaviors, emotion change, and beliefs about crying and (2) understand how the presence of others affects our evaluations of emotion following crying. This was a large international survey design study (N = 893) conducted in Australia, Croatia, the Netherlands, Thailand, and the United Kingdom. Analyses revealed that, across countries, gender, self-ascribed gender roles, and gender role attitudes (GRA) were related to behavioral crying responses, but not related to emotion change following crying. How a person evaluates crying, instead, appeared to be highly related to one’s beliefs about the helpfulness of crying, irrespective of gender. Results regarding crying when others were present showed that people are more likely both to cry and to feel that they received help around a person that they know, compared to a stranger. Furthermore, closeness to persons present during crying did not affect whether help was provided. When a crier reported that they were helped, they also tended to report feeling better following crying than those who cried around others but did not receive help. Few cross-country differences emerged, suggesting that a person’s responses to crying are quite consistent among the countries investigated here, with regard to its relationship with a person’s gender role, crying beliefs, and reactions to the presence of others.
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
5. Probing prejudice with startle eyeblink modification: a marker of attention, emotion, or both?
- Author
-
Eric J. Vanman, John P. Ryan, William C. Pedersen, and Tiffany A. Ito
- Subjects
Startle ,Facial electromyography ,Affect ,Prejudice ,Attention ,Psychology ,BF1-990 - Abstract
In social neuroscience research, startle eyeblink modification can serve as a marker of emotion, but it is less clear whether it can also serve as a marker of prejudice. In Experiment 1, 30 White students viewed photographs of White and Black targets while the startle eyeblink reflex and facial EMG from the brow and cheek regions were recorded. Prejudice was related to facial EMG activity, but not to startle modification, which instead appeared to index attention to race. To test further whether racial categorizations are associated with differential attention, a dual-task paradigm was used in Experiment 2. Fifty-four White and fifty-five Black participants responded more slowly to a tone presented when viewing a racial outgroup member or a negative stimulus, indicating that both draw more attention than ingroup members or positive stimuli. We conclude that startle modification is useful to index differential attention to groups when intergroup threat is low.
- Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
6. Using crying to cope
- Author
-
Leah Sharman, Ad J. J. M. Vingerhoets, Genevieve A. Dingle, Eric J. Vanman, and Medical and Clinical Psychology
- Subjects
Adult ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Coping (psychology) ,endocrine system ,Adolescent ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Emotions ,Crying ,Audiology ,050105 experimental psychology ,Young Adult ,Heart rate ,Respiration ,Sadness ,medicine ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,General Psychology ,media_common ,05 social sciences ,Stressor ,Cold pressor test ,Middle Aged ,Tears ,Female ,medicine.symptom ,Psychology - Abstract
This research tested the hypothesis that emotional crying facilitates coping and recovery, specifically through physiological changes that occur during crying. Female undergraduate students (N = 197) were randomly assigned to either a sad or neutral condition using short videos. Sad videos were selected for their extreme emotion elicitation. We predicted that compared to those who did not cry to the stimuli and those who were exposed to neutral videos, people who cried would (a) be able to withstand a stressful task for longer; (b) show lower levels of cortisol following crying and exposure to the stressor; and (c) have faster recovery (i.e., return to baseline levels of affect). The final groups consisted of the neutral group (n = 65), sad criers (n = 71), and sad noncriers (n = 61). After a 5-min baseline period, participants watched either the sad or neutral videos for 17 min and then completed a physical stressor (cold pressor test). Heart rate and respiration were continuously recorded, whereas salivary samples for cortisol were taken at 4 separate time points during testing. Analyses revealed no differences between the 3 groups in time withstanding the stressor or cortisol changes. Respiration rate, however, increased in the neutral group and noncriers while watching the videos, with criers' respiration remaining stable. Furthermore, heart rate was found to decelerate just before crying, with a return to baseline during the first crying period. These results suggest that crying may assist in generally maintaining biological homeostasis, perhaps consciously through self-soothing via purposeful breathing and unconsciously through regulation of heart rate. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2020 APA, all rights reserved).
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
7. Detection of deception based on fMRI activation patterns underlying the production of a deceptive response and receiving feedback about the success of the deception after a mock murder crime
- Author
-
Qian Cui, Dongtao Wei, Wenjing Yang, Qinglin Zhang, Lei Jia, and Eric J. Vanman
- Subjects
Male ,Deception ,Cognitive Neuroscience ,Middle temporal gyrus ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Feedback, Psychological ,Lie Detection ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Right striatum ,Brain mapping ,Developmental psychology ,Lie detection ,Judgment ,Young Adult ,medicine ,Image Processing, Computer-Assisted ,Humans ,media_common ,Brain Mapping ,medicine.diagnostic_test ,Brain ,Inferior parietal lobule ,General Medicine ,Original Articles ,Magnetic Resonance Imaging ,Oxygen ,Female ,Knowledge test ,Crime ,Psychology ,Functional magnetic resonance imaging - Abstract
The ability of a deceiver to track a victim's ongoing judgments about the truthfulness of the deceit can be critical for successful deception. However, no study has yet investigated the neural circuits underlying receiving a judgment about one's lie. To explore this issue, we used a modified Guilty Knowledge Test in a mock murder situation to simultaneously record the neural responses involved in producing deception and later when judgments of that deception were made. Producing deception recruited the bilateral inferior parietal lobules (IPLs), right ventral lateral prefrontal (VLPF) areas and right striatum, among which the activation of the right VLPF contributed mostly to diagnosing the identities of the participants, correctly diagnosing 81.25% of 'murderers' and 81.25% of 'innocents'. Moreover, the participant's response when their deception was successful uniquely recruited the right middle frontal gyrus, bilateral IPLs, bilateral orbitofrontal cortices, bilateral middle temporal gyrus and left cerebellum, among which the right IPL contributed mostly to diagnosing participants' identities, correctly diagnosing 93.75% of murderers and 87.5% of innocents. This study shows that neural activity associated with being a successful liar (or not) is a feasible indicator for detecting lies and may be more valid than neural activity associated with producing deception.
- Published
- 2013
8. Developing a Comprehensive Social Psychology with Shared Explanations of Primate Social Behavior
- Author
-
Eric J. Vanman
- Subjects
General Psychology - Abstract
Two primate social psychologies have developed in recent decades—one that focuses on the social behaviors of humans and the other on nonhuman primates. Despite the gains in knowledge in each field of social psychology, the two research traditions seem to be largely unaware of the other’s existence. Our common evolutionary ancestry makes this ignorance about the “other” social psychology especially troublesome for both fields. This article explores possible points of mutual interest that might lead to shared explanations of social behavior. In particular, I discuss how the topics of sexual behavior, cooperation and conflict resolution, and culture could benefit both social psychologies with respect to theory and methodology.
- Published
- 2003
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.