214 results on '"Borsboom D"'
Search Results
2. Structural Equation Modeling in Genetics
- Author
-
Bruins, Susanne, Franic, S., Borsboom, D., Dolan, Conor, Boomsma, Dorret, Hoyle, Rick H., Biological Psychology, APH - Mental Health, APH - Methodology, and Amsterdam Reproduction & Development
- Abstract
Our aim in this chapter is to discuss structural equation modeling (SEM) as it is applied in human quantitative genetics. Just as SEM is often referred to as covariance structure modeling, the application of SEM in genetics is often referred to as genetic covariance structure modeling (GCSM). Taking the seminal paper by Martin and Eaves (1977) as a starting point, the genetic analysis of covariance structures now spans a period of nearly 50 years (see Hottenga & Boomsma, 2008, for a brief history). Martin and Eaves (1977) were the first to publish an account of GCSM of twin data using Maximum Likelihood (ML) estimation based on the work of Jöreskog (1973). While Martin and Eaves developed their own program, several authors working in the field of behavior genetics and twin research realized that the user-friendly LISREL program, developed by Jöreskog and Sörbom from the 1970s onwards, could be used for genetic model fitting (Boomsma & Molenaar, 1986; Fulker, Baker, & Bock, 1983). The adoption of the LISREL program cemented the view of quantitative genetic modeling as an instance of SEM of data observed in relatives. The general aim was to obtain estimates of genetic and environmental variance components making up the phenotypic variance. The adoption of the LISREL program also encouraged applications of multivariate SEM, e.g., the common factor model, autoregressive (simplex), cross-lagged and growth curve models in genetically informative designs. It inspired approaches specific to genetics, such as Direction of Causation models, common vs. independent pathway models, and moderation models. The advent of measured genetic variables has inspired SEM for genotype-environment correlation and interaction, and intergenerational transmission). Here we introduce GCSM as it is applied in the classical twin design, and its extensions to other designs comprising larger pedigrees. We first present the basic method of exploiting familial relationships to infer the effects of unmeasured genetic and environmental factors. We explain that any SEM can be incorporated in GCSM of family data to estimate and model genetic and environmental covariance matrices. Next, we discuss several extensions that include the analysis of multi-generation data, causality, moderation of genetic influences to assess genotype-environment interaction, and the analysis of genotype-environment covariance. Finally, we consider some developments that focus on measured genetic variables in GCSM analyses and genomic SEM, which focuses on the SEM of genetic covariance matrices based on results from genetic association studies (Grotzinger et al., 2019).
- Published
- 2023
3. Promoting an open research culture: Author guidelines for journals could help to promote transparency, openness, and reproducibility
- Author
-
Nosek, B. A., Alter, G., Banks, G. C., Borsboom, D., Bowman, S. D., Breckler, S. J., Buck, S., Chambers, C. D., Chin, G., Christensen, G., Contestabile, M., Dafoe, A., Eich, E., Freese, J., Glennerster, R., Goroff, D., Green, D. P., Hesse, B., Humphreys, M., Ishiyama, J., Karlan, D., Kraut, A., Lupia, A., Mabry, P., Madon, T. A., Malhotra, N., Mayo-Wilson, E., McNutt, M., Miguel, E., Paluck, E. Levy, Simonsohn, U., Soderberg, C., Spellman, B. A., Turitto, J., VandenBos, G., Vazire, S., Wagenmakers, E. J., Wilson, R., and Yarkoni, T.
- Published
- 2015
4. Effects of stimulant medication on symptom interrelations in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder
- Author
-
Van Der Pal, Z., Geurts, H.M., Haslbeck, J., Van Keeken, A., Bruijn, A.M., Borsboom, D., Douw, L., Van Rooij, D., Franke, B., Buitelaar, J., Lambregt-Rommelse, N., Hartman, C., Oosterlaan, J., Marjolein, L., Reneman, L., Hoekstra, P.J., Blanken, T.F., and Schrantee, A.
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
5. Idealized Modeling of Psychological Dynamics
- Author
-
Dalege, J., Haslbeck, J.M.B., Marsman, M., Isvoranu, A.-M., Epskamp, S., Waldrop, L., Borsboom, D., Psychology Other Research (FMG), and Psychologische Methodenleer (Psychologie, FMG)
- Abstract
This chapter introduces the reader to studying psychological dynamics operating on network models. The chapter will illustrate such dynamics using the Ising model, an undirected network model for binary variables in which nodes mutually influence one-another. First, the chapter discusses the basics of the Ising model. Second, the chapter introduces the reader to dynamics emerging from the Ising model, such as polarization and hysteresis. These dynamics will be illustrated using the example of attitude networks. Third, the chapter will illustrate how the Ising model can be used to model cross-sectional phenomena as an alternative to latent trait theories. General intelligence will be used as an example for this illustration to show how the positive manifold and block structures in correlation matrices can arise from a network implemented in the Ising model.
- Published
- 2022
6. Pairwise Markov Random Fields
- Author
-
Epskamp, S., Haslbeck, J.M.B., Isvoranu, A.-M., van Borkulo, C.D., Waldrop, L.J., Borsboom, D., Urban Mental Health, Psychology Other Research (FMG), and FMG
- Abstract
This chapter introduces pairwise Markov random fields (PMRFs), a class of models of which the parameters can be represented as an undirected network. In this undirected network nodes represent variables and edges represent the strength of association between two variables after conditioning on all other variables included in the model. The chapter focuses on specific classes of PMRFs often used in network psychometrics: Gaussian graphical models (GGM; a network of partial correlations) for continuous data, Ising models for binary data, and mixed graphical models (MGM) for data with different types of variables. PMRFs can be interpreted in multiple ways: the models can be used as a general statistical modeling framework, as an exploratory tool to investigate predictive relationships between variables, as a tool to generate causal hypotheses, as a causal model itself, and as an exploratory tool to uncover latent variables. The chapter concludes with an introduction to estimating PMRFs from data using the bootnet and psychonetrics packages.
- Published
- 2022
7. Exploring the underlying structure of mental disorders: cross-diagnostic differences and similarities from a network perspective using both a top-down and a bottom-up approach
- Author
-
Wigman, J. T. W., van Os, J., Borsboom, D., Wardenaar, K. J., Epskamp, S., Klippel, A., Viechtbauer, W., Myin-Germeys, I., and Wichers, M.
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
8. SCIENTIFIC STANDARDS: Promoting an open research culture: Author guidelines for journals could help to promote transparency, openness, and reproducibility
- Author
-
Nosek, B. A., Alter, G., Banks, G. C., Borsboom, D., Bowman, S. D., Breckler, S. J., Buck, S., Chambers, C. D., Chin, G., Christensen, G., Contestabile, M., Dafoe, A., Eich, E., Freese, J., Glennerster, R., Goroff, D., Green, D. P., Hesse, B., Humphreys, M., Ishiyama, J., Karlan, D., Kraut, A., Lupia, A., Mabry, P., Madon, T. A., Malhotra, N., Mayo-Wilson, E., McNutt, M., Miguel, E., Paluck, E. Levy, Simonsohn, U., Soderberg, C., Spellman, B. A., Turitto, J., VandenBos, G., Vazire, S., Wagenmakers, E. J., Wilson, R., and Yarkoni, T.
- Published
- 2015
9. Revealing the dynamic network structure of the Beck Depression Inventory-II
- Author
-
Bringmann, L. F., Lemmens, L. H. J. M., Huibers, M. J. H., Borsboom, D., and Tuerlinckx, F.
- Published
- 2015
10. [Review of: P. Mair (2018) Modern Psychometrics with R]
- Author
-
Kruis, J., Borsboom, D., and Psychologische Methodenleer (Psychologie, FMG)
- Published
- 2020
11. The pathoplasticity of dysphoric episodes: differential impact of stressful life events on the pattern of depressive symptom inter-correlations
- Author
-
Cramer, A. O. J., Borsboom, D., Aggen, S. H., and Kendler, K. S.
- Published
- 2012
12. Treating insomnia and depression: using network analysis to explore working mechanisms
- Author
-
Blanken, T. F., van der Zweerde, T., van Straten, A., van Someren, E. J. W., Borsboom, D., Lancee, J., Integrative Neurophysiology, Clinical Psychology, APH - Mental Health, Amsterdam Neuroscience - Mood, Anxiety, Psychosis, Stress & Sleep, and Amsterdam Neuroscience - Brain Imaging
- Published
- 2018
13. Pathways from speech illusions to psychotic symptoms in subjects at Ultra-High Risk for psychosis: combining an experimental measure of aberrant experiences with network analysis
- Author
-
Isvoranu, A.-M., Boyette, L., Schirmbeck, Frederike, Velthorst, E., Simons, C., Borsboom, D., De Haan, L., Psychologische Methodenleer (Psychologie, FMG), Klinische Psychologie (Psychologie, FMG), and FMG
- Abstract
Background One of the oldest and most influential theories of psychosis formation states that delusions arise in an attempt to explain unusual experiences, including perceptual aberrations. The White Noise Task by Galdos et al (2011) was developed as an experimental task to assess the tendency to attribute meaning to random perceptual stimuli: speech illusions in white noise. Studies to date have demonstrated that speech illusions as assessed with the White Noise Task are associated with a composite measure of positive symptoms in patients with psychotic disorders (Galdos et al, 2011; Catalan et al, 2014). However, findings in non-clinical samples have been inconsistent: one study found an association with a composite measure of subclinical positive symptoms, including support for a relation with familial psychosis liability (Galdos et al, 2011), whereas other studies did not find any association in non-clinical samples or only partly (Catalan et al, 2014; Rimvall et al, 2016; Pries et al, 2017). The current study aims to further examine whether speech illusions as assessed with the White Noise Task are indicative of psychosis liability and to explore specific symptomatic pathways. Methods We conducted symptom-based network analyses in Ultra-High Risk (UHR) subjects participating in the European network of national networks studying gene-environment interactions in schizophrenia project (EU-GEI, 2014; www.eu-gei.eu). Psychotic symptoms were assessed with the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS). Transition to clinical psychosis was assessed with the Comprehensive Assessment of At Risk Mental State (CAARMS). We used a conservative measure of speech illusions, as described in Catalan et al (2014). Results The current sample consisted of 339 UHR subjects, of which 9.1% (N=31) experienced speech illusions. Preliminary network analyses in cross-sectional baseline data showed potential pathways from speech illusions to delusional ideation, through hallucinatory experiences. We also found evidence of prospective relations between speech illusions at baseline and transition to clinical psychosis. Pathways ran via baseline psychotic symptoms and affective symptoms, as well as a ‘direct’ pathway. Discussion As far as we are aware, this is the first study combining an experimental measure of aberrant experiences with symptom-based network analysis. Although the current reported findings are preliminary and exploratory, they tentatively support a relation between speech illusions as assessed with the White Noise Task and psychosis liability. This relation may be dependent on sample composition, and not generalizable to the general population as a whole. Future studies may benefit from focusing on more detailed trajectories of both susceptibility to speech illusions and course of (sub)clinical psychotic symptom severity in subjects with increased risk for psychosis, with use of more frequent, short assessment periods and inclusion of environmental risk factors for transition to clinical disorder.
- Published
- 2018
14. Network models for clinical psychology
- Author
-
van Bork, R., van Borkulo, C.D., Waldorp, L.J., Cramer, A.O.J., Borsboom, D., Wixted, J.T., Department of Methodology and Statistics, and Tilburg Experience Sampling Center (TESC)
- Subjects
050103 clinical psychology ,Dynamical systems theory ,Conceptualization ,05 social sciences ,Face (sociological concept) ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,Common cause and special cause ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Set (psychology) ,Construct (philosophy) ,Psychology ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Clinical psychology ,Network model ,Network analysis - Abstract
The network approach to clinical psychology is a relatively new approach and diverges on various aspects from existing models and theories. The hallmark of the theory is that there is no common cause that underlies a set of symptoms. Instead, the network approach starts out by assuming that symptoms causally interact with each other. In this chapter, we first explain the conceptualization of psychological phenomena as a network in the introduction. Second, we provide an overview of the methods that are used to construct network models from data; both Gaussian and binary data, as well as cross‐sectional and longitudinal data are covered. Third, we describe how a given network can be analyzed to uncover important symptoms in the network, to predict behavior of the network, and to compare network structures. Fourth, we discuss current state‐of‐the‐art research in clinical psychology and psychiatry, to see what these networks taught us about psychopathology. Finally, we discuss the promising prospects for clinical psychology research that the network approach has to offer and some of the challenges a researcher might face in applying this approach to clinical psychology data.
- Published
- 2018
15. Critical Slowing Down as a Personalized Early Warning Signal for Depression
- Author
-
Wichers, Marieke, Groot, PC, Borsboom, D, Cramer, AOJ, Epskamp, S, Kendler, KS, van der Maas, HLJ, Tuerlinckx, Francis, Wigman, JTW, Delespaul, P, Peeters, F, Simons, CJP, Snippe, E, van de Leemput, IA, Scheffer, M, RS: MHeNs - R2 - Mental Health, Psychiatrie & Neuropsychologie, Interdisciplinary Centre Psychopathology and Emotion regulation (ICPE), and Psychologische Methodenleer (Psychologie, FMG)
- Subjects
050103 clinical psychology ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Warning system ,05 social sciences ,General Medicine ,Variance (accounting) ,Tipping point (climatology) ,Correlation ,03 medical and health sciences ,Psychiatry and Mental health ,Clinical Psychology ,0302 clinical medicine ,Mood ,Healthy individuals ,medicine ,ComputingMethodologies_DOCUMENTANDTEXTPROCESSING ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Falling (sensation) ,Psychiatry ,Psychology ,GeneralLiterature_REFERENCE(e.g.,dictionaries,encyclopedias,glossaries) ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Applied Psychology ,Depression (differential diagnoses) ,Clinical psychology - Abstract
About 17% of humanity goes through an episode of major depression at some point in their lifetime. Despite the enormous societal costs of this incapacitating disorder, it is largely unknown how the likelihood of falling into a depressive episode can be assessed. Here, we show for a large group of healthy individuals and patients that the probability of an upcoming shift between a depressed and a normal state is related to elevated temporal autocorrelation, variance, and correlation between emotions in fluctuations of autorecorded emotions. These are indicators of the general phenomenon of critical slowing down, which is expected to occur when a system approaches a tipping point. Our results support the hypothesis that mood may have alternative stable states separated by tipping points, and suggest an approach for assessing the likelihood of transitions into and out of depression.
- Published
- 2016
16. Representation and explanation in psychometric modeling
- Author
-
Borsboom, D., Kendler, K.S., Parnas, J., and Psychologische Methodenleer (Psychologie, FMG)
- Abstract
This chapter presents a commentary on representation and explanation in psychometric modelling, as discussed in the previous chapter. It explores Turkheimer’s arguments that standard psychometric techniques involve assumptions, conventions, and definitions that introduce an arbitrary component into the models used.
- Published
- 2017
17. Mapping the manuals of madness: Comparing the ICD-10 and DSM-IV-TR using a network approach
- Author
-
Tio, P., Epskamp, S., Noordhof, A., Borsboom, D., Psychologische Methodenleer (Psychologie, FMG), Klinische Psychologie (Psychologie, FMG), and Department of Methodology and Statistics
- Abstract
The International Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems (ICD) and the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM) represent dominant approaches to diagnosis of mental disorders. However, it is unclear how these alternative systems relate to each other when taking into account the symptoms that make up the disorders. This study uses a network approach to investigate the overlap in structure between diagnostic networks pertaining to ICD-10 and DSM-IV-TR. Networks are constructed by representing individual symptoms as nodes, and connecting nodes whenever the corresponding symptoms feature as diagnostic criteria for the same mental disorder. Results indicate that, relative to the DSM-IV-TR network, the ICD-10 network contains (a) more nodes, (b) lower level of clustering, and (c) a higher level of connectivity. Both networks show features of a small world, and have similar (of “the same”) high centrality nodes. Comparison to empirical data indicates that the DSM-IV-TR network structure follows comorbidity rates more closely than the ICD-10 network structure. We conclude that, despite their apparent likeness, ICD-10 and DSM-IV-TR harbour important structural differences, and that both may be improved by matching diagnostic categories more closely to empirically determined network structures.
- Published
- 2016
18. Robustness and replicability of psychopathology networks
- Author
-
Borsboom, D., Robinaugh, D.J., Fried, E.I., Rhemtulla, M., Cramer, A.O.J., Psychosystems Group, Department of Methodology and Statistics, Tilburg Experience Sampling Center (TESC), and Psychologische Methodenleer (Psychologie, FMG)
- Subjects
050103 clinical psychology ,medicine.medical_specialty ,business.industry ,05 social sciences ,03 medical and health sciences ,Psychiatry and Mental health ,0302 clinical medicine ,Robustness (computer science) ,Research based ,medicine ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Model quality ,Pshychiatric Mental Health ,Psychiatry ,business ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Cognitive psychology ,Psychopathology ,Perspectives - Abstract
Network approaches to psychopathology hold that mental disorders arise from the interplay between symptoms in a network structure1, 2. In the past few years, statistical techniques that estimate networks were developed and applied to many disorders3. As empirical findings start to accumulate, the question arising is which of these findings are robust and replicable. Here we evaluate the state of psychopathological network research based on three methodological criteria: model quality, precision, and replicability.
- Published
- 2018
19. Network Psychometrics
- Author
-
Epskamp, S., Maris, G., Waldorp, L.J., Borsboom, D., Irwing, P., Booth, T., Hughes, D.J., Psychologische Methodenleer (Psychologie, FMG), and FMG
- Subjects
Methodology (stat.ME) ,FOS: Computer and information sciences ,010104 statistics & probability ,Statistics::Applications ,05 social sciences ,050109 social psychology ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Computer Science::Human-Computer Interaction ,0101 mathematics ,01 natural sciences ,Statistics - Methodology - Abstract
This chapter provides a general introduction of network modeling in psychometrics. The chapter starts with an introduction to the statistical model formulation of pairwise Markov random fields (PMRF), followed by an introduction of the PMRF suitable for binary data: the Ising model. The Ising model is a model used in ferromagnetism to explain phase transitions in a field of particles. Following the description of the Ising model in statistical physics, the chapter continues to show that the Ising model is closely related to models used in psychometrics. The Ising model can be shown to be equivalent to certain kinds of logistic regression models, loglinear models and multi-dimensional item response theory (MIRT) models. The equivalence between the Ising model and the MIRT model puts standard psychometrics in a new light and leads to a strikingly different interpretation of well-known latent variable models. The chapter gives an overview of methods that can be used to estimate the Ising model, and concludes with a discussion on the interpretation of latent variables given the equivalence between the Ising model and MIRT., In Irwing, P., Hughes, D., and Booth, T. (2018). The Wiley Handbook of Psychometric Testing, 2 Volume Set: A Multidisciplinary Reference on Survey, Scale and Test Development. New York: Wiley
- Published
- 2016
20. Open Peer Commentary and Author's Response
- Author
-
Asendorpf, JB, Baumert, A, Schmitt, M, Blum, G, van Bork, R, Rhemtulla, M, Borsboom, D, Chapman, BP, Clark, DA, Durbin, CE, Hicks, BM, Condon, DM, Mroczek, DK, Costantini, G, Perugini, M, Freese, J, Goldberg, LR, McCrae, RR, Nave, CS, Funder, DC, Ones, DS, Wiernik, BM, Wilmot, MP, Kostal, JW, Ozer, DJ, Poropat, A, Revelle, W, Elleman, LG, Sher, KJ, Weston, SJ, Jackson, JJ, Wood, D, Harms, PD, Ziegler, M, Ziegler, J, and Mõttus, R
- Subjects
Social Psychology ,Psychology - Abstract
Among the topics discussed in the comments, one idea appeared to be supported by most commenters: when personality trait scores are related to possible outcome variables (or possible causal factors, for that matter), scale-level analyses should be supplemented by item-level analyses. This could help to corroborate causal inferences, refine interpretations, rule out measurement/construct overlaps and/or lead to new discoveries. This suggestion is consistent with recent evidence regarding single items often reflecting unique personality characteristics (“nuances”) with trait-like properties. Future work could focus on improving item properties and delineating a useful set of nuances.
- Published
- 2016
21. Composites can be casual too
- Author
-
van Bork, R., Rhemtulla, M., Borsboom, D., Psychologische Methodenleer (Psychologie, FMG), FMG, and Brain and Cognition
- Abstract
Mõttus gives the impression that composites, as well as other models in which traits are a result rather than a cause of their indicators, require “emergent properties” to have causal power. We argue that this is not necessary; composites can be considered causally relevant by themselves when they mediate the relation between their constituents and the outcome variable.
- Published
- 2016
22. Psychometrics
- Author
-
Borsboom, D., Molenaar, D., Wright, J.D., and Psychologische Methodenleer (Psychologie, FMG)
- Abstract
Psychometrics is a scientific discipline concerned with the construction of measurement models for psychological data. In these models, a theoretical construct (e.g., intelligence) is systematically coordinated with observables (e.g., IQ scores). This is often done through latent variable models, which represent the construct of interest as a latent variable that acts as the common determinant of a set of test scores. Important psychometric questions include (1) how much information about the latent variable is contained in the data (measurement precision), (2) whether the test scores indeed measure the intended construct (validity), and (3) to what extent the test scores function in the same way in different groups (measurement invariance). Recent developments have focused on extending the basic latent variable model for more complex research designs and on implementing psychometric models in freely available software.
- Published
- 2015
23. Problems attract problems: a network perspective on mental disorders
- Author
-
Cramer, A.O.J., Borsboom, D., Scott, R.A., Kosslyn, S.M., and Psychologische Methodenleer (Psychologie, FMG)
- Subjects
medicine.medical_specialty ,Psychotherapist ,Panic disorder ,Chinese Classification of Mental Disorders ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Perspective (graphical) ,Psychological intervention ,medicine.disease ,Comorbidity ,Feeling ,mental disorders ,medicine ,Psychiatry ,Psychology ,Depression (differential diagnoses) ,media_common - Abstract
What is the nature of mental disorders such as major depression and panic disorder? Are mental disorders analogous to tumors, in that they exist as separate entities somewhere in people's minds? Do mental disorders cause symptoms such as insomnia and fatigue? Until very recently, it was exactly this sort of thinking that (implicitly) permeated many, if not all, research paradigms in clinical psychology and psychiatry. However, in recent years, a novel approach has been advocated (i.e., the network perspective), in which mental disorders are not conceived of as entities that have a separate existence from their respective symptoms. Instead, mental disorders are hypothesized to be networks of symptoms that directly influence one another. So, for example, from a network perspective, insomnia and fatigue are not caused by the same underlying disorder (i.e., major depression) but causally influence one another (i.e., insomnia → fatigue). A disorder, then, develops because of such direct relations between symptoms in which positive feedback mechanisms (i.e., vicious circles) are present: for example, insomnia → fatigue → feelings of guilt → insomnia. These feedback mechanisms may propel the aggravation of one's condition and make a person end up in, for example, a full-fledged depressive episode. In this contribution, we elaborate on network perspectives on the nature of mental disorders as well as their implications for our outlook on diagnosis and comorbidity.
- Published
- 2015
24. A skeptical eye on psi
- Author
-
Wagenmakers, E.-J., Wetzels, R., Borsboom, D., Kievit, R.A., van der Maas, H.L.J., May, E.C., Marwaha, S.B., and Psychologische Methodenleer (Psychologie, FMG)
- Abstract
Research on extrasensory perception (ESP) or psi is contentious and highly polarized. On the one hand, its proponents believe that evidence for psi is overwhelming, and they support their case with a seemingly impressive series of experiments and meta-analyses. On the other hand, psi skeptics believe that the phenomenon does not exist, and the claimed statistical support is entirely spurious. We are firmly in the camp of the skeptics. However, the main goal of this chapter is not to single out and critique individual experiments on psi. Instead, we wish to highlight the many positive consequences that psi research has had on traditional empirical research—an influence that we hope and expect will continue in the future
- Published
- 2015
25. The meaning of 'significance' for different types of research
- Author
-
de Groot, A.D., Wagenmakers, E.-J., Borsboom, D., Verhagen, J., Kievit, R., Bakker, M., Cramer, A., Matzke, D., Mellenbergh, D., van der Maas, H.L.J., and Psychologische Methodenleer (Psychologie, FMG)
- Abstract
Adrianus Dingeman de Groot (1914-2006) was one of the most influential Dutch psychologists. He became famous for his work "Thought and Choice in Chess", but his main contribution was methodological — De Groot co-founded the Department of Psychological Methods at the University of Amsterdam (together with R. F. van Naerssen), founded one of the leading testing and assessment companies (CITO), and wrote the monograph "Methodology" that centers on the empirical-scientific cycle: observation-induction-deduction-testing-evaluation. Here we translate one of De Groot's early articles, published in 1956 in the Dutch journal Nederlands Tijdschrift voor de Psychologie en Haar Grensgebieden. This article is more topical now than it was almost 60 years ago. De Groot stresses the difference between exploratory and confirmatory ("hypothesis testing") research and argues that statistical inference is only sensible for the latter: "One ‘is allowed’ to apply statistical tests in exploratory research, just as long as one realizes that they do not have evidential impact". De Groot may have also been one of the first psychologists to argue explicitly for preregistration of experiments and the associated plan of statistical analysis. The appendix provides annotations that connect De Groot's arguments to the current-day debate on transparency and reproducibility in psychological science.
- Published
- 2014
26. Dwelling on the past
- Author
-
Bakker, M., Cramer, A.O.J., Matzke, D., Kievit, R.A., van der Maas, H.L.J., Wagenmakers, E.-J., Borsboom, D., and Psychologische Methodenleer (Psychologie, FMG)
- Abstract
We welcome the recommendations suggested by Asendorpf et al. Their proposed changes will undoubtedly improve psychology as an academic discipline. However, our current knowledge is based on past research. We therefore have an obligation to ‘dwell on the past’; that is, to investigate the veracity of previously published findings—particularly those featured in course materials and popular science books. We discuss some examples of staple ‘facts’ in psychology that are actually no more than hypotheses with rather weak empirical support and suggest various ways to remedy this situation.
- Published
- 2013
27. Derailed: the rise and fall of Diederik Stapel [Review of: D. Stapel (2012) Ontsporing]
- Author
-
Borsboom, D., Wagenmakers, E.-J., and Psychologische Methodenleer (Psychologie, FMG)
- Published
- 2013
28. Frontiers of test validity theory: measurement, causation, and meaning
- Author
-
Markus, K.A., Borsboom, D., and Psychologische Methodenleer (Psychologie, FMG)
- Published
- 2013
29. Relating ASD symptoms to well-being: moving across different construct levels.
- Author
-
Deserno, M. K., Borsboom, D., Begeer, S., and Geurts, H. M.
- Subjects
- *
AUTISM , *QUESTIONNAIRES , *WELL-being , *SYMPTOMS - Abstract
BackgroundLittle is known about the specific factors that contribute to the well-being (WB) of individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). A plausible hypothesis is that ASD symptomatology has a direct negative effect on WB. In the current study, the emerging tools of network analysis allow to explore the functional interdependencies between specific symptoms of ASD and domains of WB in a multivariate framework. We illustrate how studying both higher-order (total score) and lower-order (subscale) representations of ASD symptomatology can clarify the interrelations of factors relevant for domains of WB.MethodsWe estimated network structures on three different construct levels for ASD symptomatology, as assessed with the Adult Social Behavior Questionnaire (item, subscale, total score), relating them to daily functioning (DF) and subjective WB in 323 adult individuals with clinically identified ASD (aged 17–70 years). For these networks, we assessed the importance of specific factors in the network structure.ResultsWhen focusing on the highest representation level of ASD symptomatology (i.e. a total score), we found a negative connection between ASD symptom severity and domains of WB. However, zooming in on lower representation levels of ASD symptomatology revealed that this connection was mainly funnelled by ASD symptoms related to insistence on sameness and experiencing reduced contact and that those symptom scales, in turn, impact different domains of WB.ConclusionsZooming in across construct levels of ASD symptom severity into subscales of ASD symptoms can provide us with important insights into how specific domains of ASD symptoms relate to specific domains of DF and WB. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
30. An Introduction to Network Psychometrics: Relating Ising Network Models to Item Response Theory Models.
- Author
-
Marsman, M., Borsboom, D., Kruis, J., Epskamp, S., van Bork, R., Waldorp, L. J., Maas, H. L. J. van der, and Maris, G.
- Subjects
- *
PSYCHOMETRICS , *ITEM response theory , *RASCH models - Abstract
In recent years, network models have been proposed as an alternative representation of psychometric constructs such as depression. In such models, the covariance between observables (e.g., symptoms like depressed mood, feelings of worthlessness, and guilt) is explained in terms of a pattern of causal interactions between these observables, which contrasts with classical interpretations in which the observables are conceptualized as the effects of a reflective latent variable. However, few investigations have been directed at the question how these different models relate to each other. To shed light on this issue, the current paper explores the relation between one of the most important network models—the Ising model from physics—and one of the most important latent variable models—the Item Response Theory (IRT) model from psychometrics. The Ising model describes the interaction between states of particles that are connected in a network, whereas the IRT model describes the probability distribution associated with item responses in a psychometric test as a function of a latent variable. Despite the divergent backgrounds of the models, we show a broad equivalence between them and also illustrate several opportunities that arise from this connection. [ABSTRACT FROM PUBLISHER]
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
31. What kind of causal modelling approach does personality research need?
- Author
-
Borsboom, D., van der Sluis, S., Noordhof, A., Wichers, M., Geschwind, N., Aggen, S.H., Kendler, K.S., Cramer, A.O.J., and Psychologische Methodenleer (Psychologie, FMG)
- Abstract
Lee (2012) proposes that personality research should utilise recent theories of causality. Although we agree that such theories are important, we also note that their empirical application has not been very successful to date. The reason may be that psychological systems are frequently characterised by feedback, nonlinearity and individual differences in causal structure. Such features do not preclude the application of causal modelling but do limit the usefulness of the approach for the analysis of typical personality data. To adequately investigate personality, intensive time series of repeated measurements are needed.
- Published
- 2012
32. Why psychologists must change the way they analyze their data: the case of psi
- Author
-
Wagenmakers, E.-J., Wetzels, R., Borsboom, D., van der Maas, H.L.J., and Psychologische Methodenleer (Psychologie, FMG)
- Abstract
Does psi exist? D. J. Bem (2011) conducted 9 studies with over 1,000 participants in an attempt to demonstrate that future events retroactively affect people's responses. Here we discuss several limitations of Bem's experiments on psi; in particular, we show that the data analysis was partly exploratory and that one-sided p values may overstate the statistical evidence against the null hypothesis. We reanalyze Bem's data with a default Bayesian t test and show that the evidence for psi is weak to nonexistent. We argue that in order to convince a skeptical audience of a controversial claim, one needs to conduct strictly confirmatory studies and analyze the results with statistical tests that are conservative rather than liberal. We conclude that Bem's p values do not indicate evidence in favor of precognition; instead, they indicate that experimental psychologists need to change the way they conduct their experiments and analyze their data.
- Published
- 2011
33. Where are the genes? The implications of a network perspective on gene hunting in psychopathology. [A commentary on Johnson et al.]
- Author
-
Cramer, A.O.J., Kendler, K.S., Borsboom, D., and Psychologische Methodenleer (Psychologie, FMG)
- Abstract
The missing heritability problem is pervasive and Johnson, Penke and Spinath (2011) present a number of compelling reasons for its existence. In this comment, we present another reason for the apparent discrepancy between heritability estimates and gene‐hunting results in psychopathological research: if syndromes are networks of causally related symptoms in which both symptoms and relations between them are driven by different sets of genetic polymorphisms, then gene hunting based on a phenotypic sumscore might be ill‐advised because it will only capture genetic variance shared among those symptoms and their relations.
- Published
- 2011
34. Metaphors in psychological conceptualization and explanation
- Author
-
Dooremalen, H., Borsboom, D., Toomela, A., Valsiner, J., Theoretical Philosophy, and Psychologische Methodenleer (Psychologie, FMG)
- Published
- 2010
35. Multivariate genetic analysis of longitudinally measured cognitive abilities
- Author
-
Franic, S., Dolan, C.V., Borsboom, D., van Beijsterveldt, C.E.M., Boomsma, D.I., and Biological Psychology
- Subjects
Netherlands Twin Register (NTR) - Published
- 2010
36. Genetic analysis of longitudinally measured IQ, educational attainment and educational level in Dutch twin-sib samples
- Author
-
Boomsma, D.I., van Beijsterveldt, C.E.M., van Soelen, I.L.C., Franic, S., Dolan, C.V., Borsboom, D., Bartels, M., and Biological Psychology
- Subjects
Netherlands Twin Register (NTR) - Published
- 2010
37. How genetics can help psychometrics: Determining the dimensionality of the Internalizing grouping of the Dutch Version of the Child Behavioral Checklist
- Author
-
Franic, S., Dolan, C.V., Borsboom, D., van Beijsterveldt, C.E.M., Hudziak, J., Boomsma, D.I., and Biological Psychology
- Subjects
Netherlands Twin Register (NTR) - Published
- 2009
38. Dat is geen kunst! Dat had ik zelf ook wel kunnen bedenken!
- Author
-
Israels, J.T., Borsboom, D., Speekenbrink, M., Criminal Law and Criminology, and RS: N.P.IV.C Rechtspsychologie
- Published
- 2003
39. Conceptual issues in psychological measurement
- Author
-
Borsboom, D., Mellenbergh, Don, van Heerden, J.H., and Psychologische Methodenleer (Psychologie, FMG)
- Published
- 2003
40. Kinds versus continua: a review of psychometric approaches to uncover the structure of psychiatric constructs.
- Author
-
Borsboom, D., Rhemtulla, M., Cramer, A. O. J., van der Maas, H. L. J., Scheffer, M., and Dolan, C. V.
- Subjects
- *
HYPOTHESIS , *MENTAL illness , *PSYCHIATRY , *PSYCHOLOGY , *MATHEMATICAL models of psychology , *PATHOLOGICAL psychology , *PSYCHOMETRICS , *TERMS & phrases , *MATHEMATICAL variables , *THEORY - Abstract
The question of whether psychopathology constructs are discrete kinds or continuous dimensions represents an important issue in clinical psychology and psychiatry. The present paper reviews psychometric modelling approaches that can be used to investigate this question through the application of statistical models. The relation between constructs and indicator variables in models with categorical and continuous latent variables is discussed, as are techniques specifically designed to address the distinction between latent categories as opposed to continua (taxometrics). In addition, we examine latent variable models that allow latent structures to have both continuous and categorical characteristics, such as factor mixture models and grade-of-membership models. Finally, we discuss recent alternative approaches based on network analysis and dynamical systems theory, which entail that the structure of constructs may be continuous for some individuals but categorical for others. Our evaluation of the psychometric literature shows that the kinds–continua distinction is considerably more subtle than is often presupposed in research; in particular, the hypotheses of kinds and continua are not mutually exclusive or exhaustive. We discuss opportunities to go beyond current research on the issue by using dynamical systems models, intra-individual time series and experimental manipulations. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
41. Mental Disorders as Causal Systems
- Author
-
McNally, Richard J., Robinaugh, Donald John, Wu, Gwyneth Winnie Y, Wang, Li, Deserno, M. K., and Borsboom, D.
- Subjects
causal systems ,network analysis ,posttraumatic stress disorder ,psychiatric diagnosis ,latent variable - Abstract
Debates about posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) often turn on whether it is a timeless, cross-culturally valid natural phenomenon or a socially constructed idiom of distress. Most clinicians seem to favor the first view, differing only in whether they conceptualize PTSD as a discrete category or the upper end of a dimension of stress responsiveness. Yet both categorical and dimensional construals presuppose that PTSD symptoms are fallible indicators reflective of an underlying, latent variable. This presupposition has governed psychopathology research for decades, but it rests on problematic psychometric premises. In this article, we review an alternative, network perspective for conceptualizing mental disorders as causal systems of interacting symptoms, and we illustrate this perspective via analyses of PTSD symptoms reported by survivors of the Wenchuan earthquake in China. Finally, we foreshadow emerging computational methods that may disclose the causal structure of mental disorders., Psychology
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
42. Reply to ‘Critiques of network analysis of multivariate data in psychological science’
- Author
-
Denny Borsboom, Marie K. Deserno, Mijke Rhemtulla, Sacha Epskamp, Eiko I. Fried, Richard J. McNally, Donald J. Robinaugh, Marco Perugini, Jonas Dalege, Giulio Costantini, Adela-Maria Isvoranu, Anna C. Wysocki, Claudia D. van Borkulo, Riet van Bork, Lourens J. Waldorp, Psychologische Methodenleer (Psychologie, FMG), Klinische Psychologie (Psychologie, FMG), Urban Mental Health, Psychology Other Research (FMG), Borsboom, D, Deserno, M, Rhemtulla, M, Epskamp, S, Fried, E, Mcnally, R, Robinaugh, D, Perugini, M, Dalege, J, Costantini, G, Isvoranu, A, Wysocki, A, van Borkulo, C, van Bork, R, and Waldorp, L
- Subjects
model selection ,reliability ,psychometric ,network analysi ,General Medicine ,General Chemistry - Published
- 2022
43. Changing perspectives on insomnia and depression: From symptoms to system
- Author
-
Blanken, T.F., van Someren, Eus, Borsboom, D., Amsterdam Neuroscience - Mood, Anxiety, Psychosis, Stress & Sleep, Integrative Neurophysiology, and Amsterdam Neuroscience - Brain Imaging
- Subjects
insomnia ,depression ,complex systems - Published
- 2020
44. State of the aRt personality research: A tutorial on network analysis of personality data in R
- Author
-
Denny Borsboom, Sacha Epskamp, René Mõttus, Angélique O. J. Cramer, Giulio Costantini, Lourens J. Waldorp, Marco Perugini, Psychologische Methodenleer (Psychologie, FMG), Costantini, G, Epskamp, S, Borsboom, D, Perugini, M, Mõttus, R, Waldorp, L, and Cramer, A
- Subjects
Psychology (all) ,Social Psychology ,Psychometrics ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Network analysi ,Data science ,Clustering ,Data set ,HEXACO ,Latent variable ,Centrality ,Personality ,Big Five personality traits ,Personality trait ,Construct (philosophy) ,Cluster analysis ,Psychology ,Social psychology ,Psychometric ,General Psychology ,Network analysis ,media_common - Abstract
Network analysis represents a novel theoretical approach to personality. Network approaches motivate alternative ways of analyzing data, and suggest new ways of modeling and simulating personality processes. In the present paper, we provide an overview of network analysis strategies as they apply to personality data. We discuss different ways to construct networks from typical personality data, show how to compute and interpret important measures of centrality and clustering, and illustrate how one can simulate on networks to mimic personality processes. All analyses are illustrated using a data set on the commonly used HEXACO questionnaire using elementary R-code that readers may easily adapt to apply to their own data.
- Published
- 2015
45. Modeling psychological attributes: Merits and drawbacks of taxometrics and latent variable mixture models
- Author
-
Hillen, Robert, Wicherts, Jelte, Emons, Wilco, Borsboom, D., Denissen, Jaap, Timmerman, M.E., Bouwmeester, Samantha, Hickendorff, M., and Department of Methodology and Statistics
- Subjects
ComputingMilieux_LEGALASPECTSOFCOMPUTING - Published
- 2017
46. Development of Indirect Measures of Conscientiousness: Combining a Facets Approach and Network Analysis
- Author
-
Mijke Rhemtulla, Denny Borsboom, Eiko I. Fried, Juliette Richetin, Giulio Costantini, Marco Perugini, Costantini, G, Richetin, J, Borsboom, D, Fried, E, Rhemtulla, M, Perugini, M, and Psychologische Methodenleer (Psychologie, FMG)
- Subjects
Social Psychology ,Working memory ,media_common.quotation_subject ,05 social sciences ,Short-term memory ,Implicit-association test ,Implicit Association Test ,050109 social psychology ,Conscientiousness ,Test validity ,Self-control ,Hierarchical structure of the Big Five ,conscientiousness facet ,050105 experimental psychology ,working memory ,Personality ,network analysi ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,elf-control ,Psychology ,Social psychology ,media_common ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Because indirect measures of personality self–concepts such as the Implicit Association Test (IAT) allow tapping into automatic processes, they can offer advantages over self–report measures. However, prior investigations have led to mixed results regarding the validity of indirect measures of conscientiousness. We suggest that these results might be due to a failure to consider the different facets of conscientiousness. These facets are of crucial importance because they are associated differentially with other psychobiological constructs and they are also characterized by different mechanisms. Therefore, focusing on facets while developing indirect measures of conscientiousness may improve the validity of such measures. In Study 1, we conducted a psycholexical investigation to develop one IAT for each conscientiousness facet. In Study 2, we examined the convergent and discriminant validities of each facet IAT in relation to self–report measures, peer–report measures and self–report behavioural indicators, and we investigated differential associations of the conscientiousness facets with working memory capacity and self–control. We employed network analysis as a novel approach to elucidate differential relationships involving personality facets. The results corroborated the convergent and discriminant validity of the conscientiousness facet IATs with self–reports and showed that the conscientiousness facets were differentially associated with working memory capacity and with self–control. Copyright © 2015 European Association of Personality Psychology
- Published
- 2015
47. Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science
- Author
-
Yoram K. Kunkels, Dylan Selterman, Denise J. Humphries, Kristina G. Brown, David G. Dobolyi, David J. Johnson, Mark A. Roebke, Andy T. Woods, John Hodsoll, Marije van der Hulst, Alexander A. Aarts, Kim Kelso, Erin C. Westgate, James A. Grange, Jesse Chandler, Jenelle Feather, Annick Bosch, Olivia Devitt, Benjamin T. Brown, Megan M. Kyc, Štěpán Bahník, Alissa Melinger, Michael Conn, Rebecca S. Frazier, Marc Jekel, Sara Bowman, Michael J. Wood, Erica Baranski, Sining Wu, Milan Valášek, Anna E. Van't Veer, Jeanine L. M. Skorinko, Joeri Wissink, Sara Steegen, Michael C. Pitts, Douglas Gazarian, Steve N.H. Tsang, Matthew W. Kirkhart, Jennifer S. Beer, Nathali Immelman, Elizabeth Chagnon, Robbie C. M. van Aert, Maya B. Mathur, Magnus Johannesson, Joshua D. Foster, Frank J. Farach, Gandalf Nicolas, Ian S. Penton-Voak, Rebecca M. Goldberg, Sarah L. Thomas, Kathleen Schmidt, Stephanie C. Lin, Linda Cillessen, Belén Fernández-Castilla, Taru Flagan, René Schlegelmilch, Joanneke Weerdmeester, Cyril Pernet, Andreas Cordes, Onur Sahin, Jolanda J. Kossakowski, Samuel Shaki, David Santos, Sabine Scholz, Jeremy R. Gray, Frank Renkewitz, Key Jung Lee, Gillian M. Sandstrom, Marie K. Deserno, Melissa Vazquez, Ed Cremata, Rebecca Saxe, Manuela Thomae, Johannes M. Meixner, Emma Heikensten, Sylvia Eboigbe, Carmel A. Levitan, Natalia Vélez, James G. Field, Riet van Bork, Vivien Estel, Michèle B. Nuijten, Lin Lin, Kate M. Johnson, Bobby Den Bezemer, Jennifer A. Joy-Gaba, Francis Tuerlinckx, Frits Traets, Ilse Luteijn, Christopher R. Chartier, Denise C. Marigold, Denny Borsboom, Elizabeth Gilbert, Jeff Galak, Shannon P. Callahan, E. J. Masicampo, Thomas Talhelm, Chris H.J. Hartgerink, Patrick T. Goodbourn, Stephanie M. Müller, Taylor Nervi, Marcus Möschl, Katherine Moore, Wolf Vanpaemel, Seung K. Kim, Elizabeth Bartmess, Heather N. Mainard, Martin Voracek, Gea Hoogendoorn, Sean P. Mackinnon, Ryan Donohue, Kate A. Ratliff, Jin X. Goh, Anastasia E. Rigney, Andreas Glöckner, Marieke Vermue, Angela S. Attwood, Michelle A. DeGaetano, Nick Spencer, Heather Bentley, Nina Strohminger, Geneva T. Dodson, R. Nathan Pipitone, Hayley M. D. Cleary, Matt Motyl, Amanda L. Forest, Marcus R. Munafò, Marcel Zeelenberg, Susann Fiedler, Ann Calhoun-Sauls, Mallorie Miller, Anondah R. Saide, Ljiljana B. Lazarević, Hilmar Brohmer, Mallory C. Kidwell, Pranjal H. Mehta, Jessie Gorges, Russ Clay, Jeffrey R. Spies, Joanna E. Anderson, Johnny van Doorn, Ashley A. Ricker, Elizabeth W. Dunn, Erin L Braswell, Jamie DeCoster, Larissa Seibel, Matthias Lippold, Lutz Ostkamp, William B. Simpson, Cathy On-Ying Hung, Carina Sonnleitner, Emily M. Wright, Laura Dewitte, Koen Ilja Neijenhuijs, Tim Kuhlmann, Job Krijnen, Leah Beyan, Jesse Graham, Andrew M Rivers, Sacha Epskamp, Aamir Laique, Christopher J. Anderson, Peter Raymond Attridge, Eric-Jan Wagenmakers, Agnieszka Slowik, Michael C. Frank, Bryan Gorges, Alejandro Vásquez Echeverría, Gina Vuu, Giulio Costantini, Eskil Forsell, Michelangelo Vianello, Don van den Bergh, Anna Fedor, Courtney K. Soderberg, M. Brent Donnellan, Kayleigh E Easey, Shauna Gordon-McKeon, Raoul Bell, William J. Johnston, Brian A. Nosek, Ashlee Welsh, Melissa Lewis, Anna Dreber, Simon Columbus, Frank A. Bosco, Pia Tio, Joshua K. Hartshorne, Lars Goellner, Elisa Maria Galliani, Etienne P. Le Bel, Kellylynn Zuni, Olivia Perna, Kristi M. Lemm, Marco Perugini, Anniek M. te Dorsthorst, Hedderik van Rijn, Timothy M. Errington, Bennett Kleinberg, Vanessa C. Irsik, Frank Jäkel, Timothy Hayes, Mark Verschoor, Mark D. Cloud, Bethany Lassetter, Justin Goss, Paul J. Turchan, Gavin Brent Sullivan, Darren Loureiro, Jo Embley, Robert S. Ryan, Jovita Brüning, Jan Crusius, Joel S. Snyder, Larissa Gabrielle Johnson, Nicolás Delia Penna, Grace Binion, Calvin K. Lai, Gustav Nilsonne, Heather M. Fuchs, Angela Rachael Dorrough, Michelle Dugas, Johanna Cohoon, Minha Lee, Robert Krause, David Reinhard, Goran Knežević, Jason M. Prenoveau, Kristin A. Lane, Stanka A. Fitneva, Rima-Maria Rahal, Mathijs Van De Ven, Anup Gampa, Marcel A.L.M. van Assen, Jordan Axt, Felix Henninger, Misha Pavel, Daniel Lakens, Jeremy K. Miller, Sara García, Leslie Cramblet Alvarez, Colleen Osborne, Kai J. Jonas, Taylor Holubar, Stefan Stieger, Heather Barry Kappes, Felix Cheung, Daan R. van Renswoude, Catherine Olsson, Roel van Dooren, Tylar Martinez, Megan Tapia, Philip A. Gable, Cody D. Christopherson, Franziska Plessow, Roger Giner-Sorolla, Abraham M. Rutchick, Michael Barnett-Cowan, Mark J. Brandt, Rebecca A. Dore, Michael May, H. Colleen Sinclair, Georg Jahn, Daniel P. Martin, Fred Hasselman, Casey Eggleston, Nicole Mechin, Joshua J. Matacotta, Molly Babel, Franziska Maria Kolorz, Social & Organizational Psychology, IBBA, Clinical Psychology, EMGO+ - Mental Health, Social Networks, Solidarity and Inequality, Department of Social Psychology, Department of Methodology and Statistics, Aarts, A, Anderson, J, Anderson, C, Attridge, P, Attwood, A, Axt, J, Babel, M, Bahník, Š, Baranski, E, Barnett Cowan, M, Bartmess, E, Beer, J, Bell, R, Bentley, H, Beyan, L, Binion, G, Borsboom, D, Bosch, A, Bosco, F, Bowman, S, Brandt, M, Braswell, E, Brohmer, H, Brown, B, Brown, K, Brüning, J, Calhoun Sauls, A, Callahan, S, Chagnon, E, Chandler, J, Chartier, C, Cheung, C, Cd, Cillessen, L, Clay, R, Cleary, H, Cloud, M, Cohn, M, Cohoon, J, Columbus, S, Cordes, A, Costantini, G, Cramblet Alvarez, L, Cremata, E, Crusius, J, Decoster, J, Degaetano, M, Della Penna, N, den Bezemer, B, Deserno, M, Devitt, O, Dewitte, L, Dobolyi, D, Dodson, G, Donnellan, M, Donohue, R, Dore, R, Dorrough, A, Dreber, A, Dugas, M, Dunn, E, Easey, K, Eboigbe, S, Eggleston, C, Embley, J, Epskamp, S, Errington, T, Estel, V, Farach, F, Feather, J, Fedor, A, Fernández Castilla, B, Fiedler, S, Field, J, Fitneva, S, Flagan, T, Forest, A, Forsell, E, Foster, J, Frank, M, Frazier, R, Fuchs, H, Gable, P, Galak, J, Galliani, E, Gampa, A, Garcia, S, Gazarian, D, Gilbert, E, Giner Sorolla, R, Glöckner, A, Goellner, L, Goh, J, Goldberg, R, Goodbourn, P, Gordon McKeon, S, Gorges, B, Gorges, J, Goss, J, Graham, J, Grange, J, Gray, J, Hartgerink, C, Hartshorne, J, Hasselman, F, Hayes, T, Heikensten, E, Henninger, F, Hodsoll, J, Holubar, T, Hoogendoorn, G, Humphries, D, Hung, C, Immelman, N, Irsik, V, Jahn, G, Jäkel, F, Jekel, M, Johannesson, M, Johnson, L, Johnson, D, Johnson, K, Johnston, W, Jonas, K, Joy Gaba, J, Kappes, H, Kelso, K, Kidwell, M, Kim, S, Kirkhart, M, Kleinberg, B, Kneževic, G, Kolorz, F, Kossakowski, J, Krause, R, Krijnen, J, Kuhlmann, T, Kunkels, Y, Kyc, M, Lai, C, Laique, A, Lakens, D, Lane, K, Lassetter, B, Lazarevic, L, Lebel, E, Lee, K, Lee, M, Lemm, K, Levitan, C, Lewis, M, Lin, L, Lin, S, Lippold, M, Loureiro, D, Luteijn, I, Mackinnon, S, Mainard, H, Marigold, D, Martin, D, Martinez, T, Masicampo, E, Matacotta, J, Mathur, M, May, M, Mechin, N, Mehta, P, Meixner, J, Melinger, A, Miller, J, Miller, M, Moore, K, Möschl, M, Motyl, M, Müller, S, Munafo, M, Neijenhuijs, K, Nervi, T, Nicolas, G, Nilsonne, G, Nosek, B, Nuijten, M, Olsson, C, Osborne, C, Ostkamp, L, Pavel, M, Penton Voak, I, Perna, O, Pernet, C, Perugini, M, Pipitone, N, Pitts, M, Plessow, F, Prenoveau, J, Rahal, R, Ratliff, K, Reinhard, D, Renkewitz, F, Ricker, A, Rigney, A, Rivers, A, Roebke, M, Rutchick, A, Ryan, R, Sahin, O, Saide, A, Sandstrom, G, Santos, D, Saxe, R, Schlegelmilch, R, Schmidt, K, Scholz, S, Seibel, L, Selterman, D, Shaki, S, Simpson, E, Sinclair, H, Skorinko, J, Slowik, A, Snyder, J, Soderberg, C, Sonnleitner, C, Spencer, N, Spies, J, Steegen, S, Stieger, S, Strohminger, N, Sullivan, G, Talhelm, T, Tapia, M, te Dorsthorst, A, Thomae, M, Thomas, S, Tio, P, Traets, F, Tsang, S, Tuerlinckx, F, Turchan, P, Valášek, M, van 't Veer, A, Van Aert, R, van Assen, M, van Bork, R, van de Ven, M, van den Bergh, D, van der Hulst, M, van Dooren, R, van Doorn, J, van Renswoude, D, van Rijn, H, Vanpaemel, W, Vásquez Echeverría, A, Vazquez, M, Velez, N, Vermue, M, Verschoor, M, Vianello, M, Voracek, M, Vuu, G, Wagenmakers, E, Weerdmeester, J, Welsh, A, Westgate, E, Wissink, J, Wood, M, Woods, A, Wright, E, Wu, S, Zeelenberg, M, Zuni, K, Sociology/ICS, Experimental Psychology, Human Technology Interaction, Sociale Psychologie (Psychologie, FMG), Ontwikkelingspsychologie (Psychologie, FMG), and Brein en Cognitie (Psychologie, FMG)
- Subjects
Research design ,Department Psychologie ,BF Psychology ,media_common.quotation_subject ,POWER ,Learning and Plasticity ,Reproducibility Project ,Q1 ,Experimental Psychopathology and Treatment ,Replication (statistics) ,Statistics ,TRUTH ,Psychology ,General ,Mathematics ,media_common ,Selection bias ,Replication crisis ,Behaviour Change and Well-being ,Multidisciplinary ,PUBLICATION ,Publication bias ,Reproducibility ,Confidence interval ,INCENTIVES ,PREVALENCE ,Meta-analysis ,REPLICABILITY ,REPLICATION ,Developmental Psychopathology ,FALSE - Abstract
IntroductionReproducibility is a defining feature of science, but the extent to which it characterizes current research is unknown. Scientific claims should not gain credence because of the status or authority of their originator but by the replicability of their supporting evidence. Even research of exemplary quality may have irreproducible empirical findings because of random or systematic error.RationaleThere is concern about the rate and predictors of reproducibility, but limited evidence. Potentially problematic practices include selective reporting, selective analysis, and insufficient specification of the conditions necessary or sufficient to obtain the results. Direct replication is the attempt to recreate the conditions believed sufficient for obtaining a previously observed finding and is the means of establishing reproducibility of a finding with new data. We conducted a large-scale, collaborative effort to obtain an initial estimate of the reproducibility of psychological science.ResultsWe conducted replications of 100 experimental and correlational studies published in three psychology journals using high-powered designs and original materials when available. There is no single standard for evaluating replication success. Here, we evaluated reproducibility using significance and P values, effect sizes, subjective assessments of replication teams, and meta-analysis of effect sizes. The mean effect size (r) of the replication effects (Mr = 0.197, SD = 0.257) was half the magnitude of the mean effect size of the original effects (Mr = 0.403, SD = 0.188), representing a substantial decline. Ninety-seven percent of original studies had significant results (P < .05). Thirty-six percent of replications had significant results; 47% of original effect sizes were in the 95% confidence interval of the replication effect size; 39% of effects were subjectively rated to have replicated the original result; and if no bias in original results is assumed, combining original and replication results left 68% with statistically significant effects. Correlational tests suggest that replication success was better predicted by the strength of original evidence than by characteristics of the original and replication teams.ConclusionNo single indicator sufficiently describes replication success, and the five indicators examined here are not the only ways to evaluate reproducibility. Nonetheless, collectively these results offer a clear conclusion: A large portion of replications produced weaker evidence for the original findings despite using materials provided by the original authors, review in advance for methodological fidelity, and high statistical power to detect the original effect sizes. Moreover, correlational evidence is consistent with the conclusion that variation in the strength of initial evidence (such as original P value) was more predictive of replication success than variation in the characteristics of the teams conducting the research (such as experience and expertise). The latter factors certainly can influence replication success, but they did not appear to do so here. Reproducibility is not well understood because the incentives for individual scientists prioritize novelty over replication. Innovation is the engine of discovery and is vital for a productive, effective scientific enterprise. However, innovative ideas become old news fast. Journal reviewers and editors may dismiss a new test of a published idea as unoriginal. The claim that “we already know this” belies the uncertainty of scientific evidence. Innovation points out paths that are possible; replication points out paths that are likely; progress relies on both. Replication can increase certainty when findings are reproduced and promote innovation when they are not. This project provides accumulating evidence for many findings in psychological research and suggests that there is still more work to do to verify whether we know what we think we know.
- Published
- 2015
48. From Structural Equation Models to Next-Generation Sequencing: The Evolving Landscape of Modern Behavioral Genetics
- Author
-
Franic, S.F., Boomsma, DI, Dolan, CV, Borsboom, D., Biological Psychology, and Neuroscience Campus Amsterdam - Neurobiology of Mental Health
- Published
- 2014
49. Rejoinder to McNeish and Mislevy: What Does Psychological Measurement Require?
- Author
-
Sijtsma K, Ellis JL, and Borsboom D
- Abstract
In this rejoinder to McNeish (2024) and Mislevy (2024), who both responded to our focus article on the merits of the simple sum score (Sijtsma et al., 2024), we address several issues. Psychometrics education and in particular psychometricians' outreach may help researchers to use IRT models as a precursor for the responsible use of the latent variable score and the sum score. Different methods used for test and questionnaire construction often do not produce highly different results, and when they do, this may be due to an unarticulated attribute theory generating noisy data. The sum score and transformations thereof, such as normalized test scores and percentiles, may help test practitioners and their clients to better communicate results. Latent variables prove important in more advanced applications such as equating and adaptive testing where they serve as technical tools rather than communication devices. Decisions based on test results are often binary or use a rather coarse ordering of scale levels, hence, do not require a high level of granularity (but nevertheless need to be precise). A gap exists between psychology and psychometrics which is growing deeper and wider, and that needs to be bridged. Psychology and psychometrics must work together to attain this goal., (© 2024. The Author(s).)
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
50. Bouncing back from life's perturbations: Formalizing psychological resilience from a complex systems perspective.
- Author
-
Lunansky G, Bonanno GA, Blanken TF, van Borkulo CD, Cramer AOJ, and Borsboom D
- Abstract
Experiencing stressful or traumatic events can lead to a range of responses, from mild disruptions to severe and persistent mental health issues. Understanding the various trajectories of response to adversity is crucial for developing effective interventions and support systems. Researchers have identified four commonly observed response trajectories to adversity, from which the resilient is the most common one. Resilience refers to the maintenance of healthy psychological functioning despite facing adversity. However, it remains an open question how to understand and anticipate resilience, due to its dynamic and multifactorial nature. This article presents a novel formalized framework to conceptualize resilience from a complex systems perspective. We use the network theory of psychopathology, which states that mental disorders are self-sustaining endpoints of direct symptom-symptom interactions organized in a network system. The internal structure of the network determines the most likely trajectory of symptom development. We introduce the resilience quadrant, which organizes the state of symptom networks on two domains: (1) healthy versus dysfunctional and (2) stable versus unstable. The quadrant captures the four commonly observed response trajectories to adversity along those dimensions: resilient trajectories in the face of adversity, as well as persistent symptoms despite treatment interventions. Subsequently, an empirical illustration, by means of a proof-of-principle, shows how simulated observations from four different network architectures lead to the four commonly observed responses to adversity. As such, we present a novel outlook on resilience by combining existing statistical symptom network models with simulation techniques. (PsycInfo Database Record (c) 2024 APA, all rights reserved).
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.