21 results
Search Results
2. Exploring and mitigating potential bias when genetic instrumental variables are associated with multiple non-exposure traits in Mendelian randomization
- Author
-
Yang, Qian, Sanderson, Eleanor, Tilling, Kate, Borges, Maria Carolina, and Lawlor, Deborah A.
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
3. The design of empirical studies: towards a unified view.
- Author
-
Cox, David
- Subjects
EPIDEMIOLOGY ,DRUGS ,DESIGN science ,DEBATE ,DISCUSSION - Abstract
A broad review is given of the general principles underlying study design with emphasis on applications in medical and epidemiological contexts. The main theme of the paper is that, while the distinction between interventionist studies, that is experiments, and purely observational ones is important, there are many common threads. A wide range of specific applications are used in outline to illustrate the discussion. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
4. A descriptive review of variable selection methods in four epidemiologic journals: there is still room for improvement.
- Author
-
Talbot, Denis and Massamba, Victoria Kubuta
- Subjects
CAUSAL models ,UNIVARIATE analysis ,EPIDEMIOLOGISTS ,ROOMS ,DATA analysis ,PRIOR learning - Abstract
A review of epidemiological papers conducted in 2009 concluded that several studies employed variable selection methods susceptible to introduce bias and yield inadequate inferences. Many new confounder selection methods have been developed since then. The goal of the study was to provide an updated descriptive portrait of which variable selection methods are used by epidemiologists for analyzing observational data. Studies published in four major epidemiological journals in 2015 were reviewed. Only articles concerned with a predictive or explicative objective and reporting on the analysis of individual data were included. Method(s) employed for selecting variables were extracted from retained articles. A total of 975 articles were retrieved and 299 met eligibility criteria, 292 of which pursued an explicative objective. Among those, 146 studies (50%) reported using prior knowledge or causal graphs for selecting variables, 34 (12%) used change in effect estimate methods, 26 (9%) used stepwise approaches, 16 (5%) employed univariate analyses, 5 (2%) used various other methods and 107 (37%) did not provide sufficient details to allow classification (more than one method could be employed in a single article). Despite being less frequent than in the previous review, stepwise and univariable analyses, which are susceptible to introduce bias and produce inadequate inferences, were still prevalent. Moreover, 37% studies did not provide sufficient details to assess how variables were selected. We thus believe there is still room for improvement in variable selection methods used by epidemiologists and in their reporting. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
5. Case–control matching on confounders revisited.
- Author
-
Mansournia, Mohammad Ali and Poole, Charles
- Subjects
MARGINAL distributions ,CASE-control method ,CONTROL groups - Abstract
Matching by a confounder in a case–control study nearly always produces a control-selection bias that mixes with the confounding to produce a net bias. Previous theoretical work has assumed that control for a single confounder, the matching factor, is sufficient to remove all the confounding and that the confounder-exposure, confounder-outcome and exposure-outcome associations are monotonic. Under these conditions: (a) The net bias is toward the null if the exposure affects the outcome and nil if it does not. (b) If the confounding is away from the null, the selection bias is toward the null. (c) If the confounding is toward the null, the selection bias can be in any direction or even nil. If more than one confounder needs to be controlled to remove all the confounding, the net bias from matching by one of them can be away from the null, whether the exposure affects the outcome or not. An influential heuristic, that matching controls to cases by a variable associated with exposure always brings the marginal exposure distributions of the case and control groups closer together, turns out to be faulty. The implications of matching by confounders in case–control studies are less straightforward than previously thought. Suggestions are offered for advancing the methodologic literature on this topic. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
6. Theory and methodology: essential tools that can become dangerous belief systems
- Author
-
Greenland, Sander, Jewell, Nicholas Patrick, and Mansournia, Mohammad Ali
- Subjects
Epidemiology ,Health Sciences ,Case-Control Studies ,Confounding Factors ,Epidemiologic ,Humans ,Religion and Psychology ,Case-control studies ,Causal inference ,Confounding ,Epidemiological research ,Case–control studies ,Public Health and Health Services - Abstract
We thank Dr. Karp for his interest [1] in our paper [2]. We agree on some points, but our theoretical description differs from his in ways leading to important divergences for teaching and practice. We also see a danger of overextending abstract theory (with its inevitable and extensive simplifications) into practice [3], especially when the practical questions are causal but the theory applied lacks an explicit, sound longitudinal causal model to address these questions. As we will explain, a defect in the “study base” theory Dr. Karp adopts as a foundational belief system is that it takes as a foundation a parameter affected by baseline risk factors—including exposure when that has effects on follow-up or disease. It consequently leads to biases and misconceptions of the sort documented elsewhere [4, 5] and below, which require a coherent theory of longitudinal causality to address. Our divergence from Dr. Karp thus raises the issue of the role of theory and methods in research, although matching serves to illustrate our points in a familiar epidemiologic context.
- Published
- 2018
7. The Authors’ Reply: Statins and post-stroke dementia
- Author
-
Yang, Zhirong, Toh, Sengwee, and Mant, Jonathan
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
8. Case–control matching: effects, misconceptions, and recommendations.
- Author
-
Mansournia, Mohammad Ali, Jewell, Nicholas Patrick, and Greenland, Sander
- Subjects
CASE-control method ,SELECTION bias (Statistics) ,CONFOUNDING variables ,STATISTICAL matching ,ODDS ratio - Abstract
Misconceptions about the impact of case–control matching remain common. We discuss several subtle problems associated with matched case–control studies that do not arise or are minor in matched cohort studies: (1) matching, even for non-confounders, can create selection bias; (2) matching distorts dose–response relations between matching variables and the outcome; (3) unbiased estimation requires accounting for the actual matching protocol as well as for any residual confounding effects; (4) for efficiency, identically matched groups should be collapsed; (5) matching may harm precision and power; (6) matched analyses may suffer from sparse-data bias, even when using basic sparse-data methods. These problems support advice to limit case–control matching to a few strong well-measured confounders, which would devolve to no matching if no such confounders are measured. On the positive side, odds ratio modification by matched variables can be assessed in matched case–control studies without further data, and when one knows either the distribution of the matching factors or their relation to the outcome in the source population, one can estimate and study patterns in absolute rates. Throughout, we emphasize distinctions from the more intuitive impacts of cohort matching. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
9. Association of light-to-moderate alcohol drinking in pregnancy with preterm birth and birth weight: elucidating bias by pooling data from nine European cohorts.
- Author
-
Strandberg-Larsen, Katrine, Poulsen, Gry, Bech, Bodil, Chatzi, Leda, Cordier, Sylvaine, Dale, Maria, Fernandez, Marieta, Henriksen, Tine, Jaddoe, Vincent, Kogevinas, Manolis, Kruithof, Claudia, Lindhard, Morten, Magnus, Per, Nohr, Ellen, Richiardi, Lorenzo, Rodriguez-Bernal, Clara, Rouget, Florence, Rusconi, Franca, Vrijheid, Martine, and Andersen, Anne-Marie
- Subjects
PHYSIOLOGICAL effects of alcohol ,PREMATURE labor ,BIRTH weight ,PREGNANCY ,GESTATIONAL age ,CONFOUNDING variables - Abstract
Women who drink light-to-moderately during pregnancy have been observed to have lower risk of unfavourable pregnancy outcomes than abstainers. This has been suggested to be a result of bias. In a pooled sample, including 193 747 live-born singletons from nine European cohorts, we examined the associations between light-to-moderate drinking and preterm birth, birth weight, and small-for-gestational age in term born children (term SGA). To address potential sources of bias, we compared the associations from the total sample with a sub-sample restricted to first-time pregnant women who conceived within six months of trying, and examined whether the associations varied across calendar time. In the total sample, drinking up to around six drinks per week as compared to abstaining was associated with lower risk of preterm birth, whereas no significant associations were found for birth weight or term SGA. Drinking six or more drinks per week was associated with lower birth weight and higher risk of term SGA, but no increased risk of preterm birth. The analyses restricted to women without reproductive experience revealed similar results. Before 2000 approximately half of pregnant women drank alcohol. This decreased to 39% in 2000-2004, and 14% in 2005-2011. Before 2000, every additional drink was associated with reduced mean birth weight, whereas in 2005-2011, the mean birth weight increased with increasing intake. The period-specific associations between low-to-moderate drinking and birth weight, which also were observed for term SGA, are indicative of bias. It is impossible to distinguish if the bias is attributable to unmeasured confounding, which change over time or cohort heterogeneity. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2017
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
10. Principles of confounder selection
- Author
-
Tyler J. VanderWeele
- Subjects
Epidemiology ,business.industry ,Confounding ,Instrumental variable ,Confounding Factors, Epidemiologic ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,Research Design ,Causal inference ,Collider (epidemiology) ,Covariate ,Econometrics ,Humans ,Medicine ,030212 general & internal medicine ,business ,Proxy (statistics) ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Selection (genetic algorithm) ,Causal model - Abstract
Selecting an appropriate set of confounders for which to control is critical for reliable causal inference. Recent theoretical and methodological developments have helped clarify a number of principles of confounder selection. When complete knowledge of a causal diagram relating all covariates to each other is available, graphical rules can be used to make decisions about covariate control. Unfortunately, such complete knowledge is often unavailable. This paper puts forward a practical approach to confounder selection decisions when the somewhat less stringent assumption is made that knowledge is available for each covariate whether it is a cause of the exposure, and whether it is a cause of the outcome. Based on recent theoretically justified developments in the causal inference literature, the following proposal is made for covariate control decisions: control for each covariate that is a cause of the exposure, or of the outcome, or of both; exclude from this set any variable known to be an instrumental variable; and include as a covariate any proxy for an unmeasured variable that is a common cause of both the exposure and the outcome. Various principles of confounder selection are then further related to statistical covariate selection methods.
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
11. The disjunctive cause criterion by VanderWeele: An easy solution to a complex problem?
- Author
-
Ikram, Mohammad Arfan
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
12. Summary of relationships between exchangeability, biasing paths and bias.
- Author
-
Flanders, William and Eldridge, Ronald
- Subjects
COMPREHENSION ,SOCIALIZATION agents ,HYPOTHESIS ,PICTURES ,COGNITION - Abstract
Definitions and conceptualizations of confounding and selection bias have evolved over the past several decades. An important advance occurred with development of the concept of exchangeability. For example, if exchangeability holds, risks of disease in an unexposed group can be compared with risks in an exposed group to estimate causal effects. Another advance occurred with the use of causal graphs to summarize causal relationships and facilitate identification of causal patterns that likely indicate bias, including confounding and selection bias. While closely related, exchangeability is defined in the counterfactual-model framework and confounding paths in the causal-graph framework. Moreover, the precise relationships between these concepts have not been fully described. Here, we summarize definitions and current views of these concepts. We show how bias, exchangeability and biasing paths interrelate and provide justification for key results. For example, we show that absence of a biasing path implies exchangeability but that the reverse implication need not hold without an additional assumption, such as faithfulness. The close links shown are expected. However confounding, selection bias and exchangeability are basic concepts, so comprehensive summarization and definitive demonstration of links between them is important. Thus, this work facilitates and adds to our understanding of these important biases. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
13. Theory and methodology: essential tools that can become dangerous belief systems
- Author
-
Mohammad Ali Mansournia, Nicholas P. Jewell, and Sander Greenland
- Subjects
Religion and Psychology ,Matching (statistics) ,Epidemiology ,Context (language use) ,01 natural sciences ,010104 statistics & probability ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,Humans ,Medicine ,sort ,Confounding ,030212 general & internal medicine ,0101 mathematics ,Causal model ,Epidemiologic ,Divergence (linguistics) ,business.industry ,Foundation (evidence) ,Causality ,Confounding Factors ,Case–control studies ,Epistemology ,Epidemiological research ,Case-Control Studies ,Causal inference ,Public Health and Health Services ,business - Abstract
We thank Dr. Karp for his interest [1] in our paper [2]. We agree on some points, but our theoretical description differs from his in ways leading to important divergences for teaching and practice. We also see a danger of overextending abstract theory (with its inevitable and extensive simplifications) into practice [3], especially when the practical questions are causal but the theory applied lacks an explicit, sound longitudinal causal model to address these questions. As we will explain, a defect in the “study base” theory Dr. Karp adopts as a foundational belief system is that it takes as a foundation a parameter affected by baseline risk factors—including exposure when that has effects on follow-up or disease. It consequently leads to biases and misconceptions of the sort documented elsewhere [4, 5] and below, which require a coherent theory of longitudinal causality to address. Our divergence from Dr. Karp thus raises the issue of the role of theory and methods in research, although matching serves to illustrate our points in a familiar epidemiologic context.
- Published
- 2018
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
14. Selection of confounding variables should not be based on observed associations with exposure.
- Author
-
Groenwold, Rolf, Klungel, Olaf, Grobbee, Diederick, and Hoes, Arno
- Subjects
ADRENERGIC beta agonists ,PHARMACOEPIDEMIOLOGY ,MORTALITY ,ASTHMATICS ,OBSTRUCTIVE lung diseases patients ,CARDIOVASCULAR diseases ,LUNG diseases - Abstract
In observational studies, selection of confounding variables for adjustment is often based on observed baseline incomparability. The aim of this study was to evaluate this selection strategy. We used clinical data on the effects of inhaled long-acting beta-agonist (LABA) use on the risk of mortality among patients with obstructive pulmonary disease to illustrate the impact of selection of confounding variables for adjustment based on baseline comparisons. Among 2,394 asthma and COPD patients included in the analyses, the LABA ever-users were considerably older than never-users, but cardiovascular co-morbidity was equally prevalent (19.9% vs. 19.9%). Adjustment for cardiovascular co-morbidity status did not affect the crude risk ratio (RR) for mortality: crude RR 1.19 (95% CI 0.93-1.51) versus RR 1.19 (95% CI 0.94-1.50) after adjustment for cardiovascular co-morbidity. However, after adjustment for age (RR 0.95, 95% CI 0.76-1.19), additional adjustment for cardiovascular co-morbidity status did affect the association between LABA use and mortality (RR 1.01, 95% CI 0.80-1.26). Confounding variables should not be discarded based on balanced distributions among exposure groups, because residual confounding due to the omission of confounding variables from the adjustment model can be relevant. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2011
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
15. A descriptive review of variable selection methods in four epidemiologic journals: there is still room for improvement
- Author
-
Victoria Kubuta Massamba and Denis Talbot
- Subjects
Causal graph ,Univariate analysis ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Epidemiology ,business.industry ,Confounding ,Feature selection ,Confounding Factors, Epidemiologic ,Epidemiologists ,030204 cardiovascular system & hematology ,03 medical and health sciences ,Epidemiologic Studies ,0302 clinical medicine ,Bias ,Research Design ,Statistics ,Individual data ,Medicine ,Humans ,Observational study ,030212 general & internal medicine ,Selection method ,Periodicals as Topic ,business - Abstract
A review of epidemiological papers conducted in 2009 concluded that several studies employed variable selection methods susceptible to introduce bias and yield inadequate inferences. Many new confounder selection methods have been developed since then. The goal of the study was to provide an updated descriptive portrait of which variable selection methods are used by epidemiologists for analyzing observational data. Studies published in four major epidemiological journals in 2015 were reviewed. Only articles concerned with a predictive or explicative objective and reporting on the analysis of individual data were included. Method(s) employed for selecting variables were extracted from retained articles. A total of 975 articles were retrieved and 299 met eligibility criteria, 292 of which pursued an explicative objective. Among those, 146 studies (50%) reported using prior knowledge or causal graphs for selecting variables, 34 (12%) used change in effect estimate methods, 26 (9%) used stepwise approaches, 16 (5%) employed univariate analyses, 5 (2%) used various other methods and 107 (37%) did not provide sufficient details to allow classification (more than one method could be employed in a single article). Despite being less frequent than in the previous review, stepwise and univariable analyses, which are susceptible to introduce bias and produce inadequate inferences, were still prevalent. Moreover, 37% studies did not provide sufficient details to assess how variables were selected. We thus believe there is still room for improvement in variable selection methods used by epidemiologists and in their reporting.
- Published
- 2018
16. On the relationship of sufficient component cause models with potential outcome (counterfactual) models
- Author
-
Flanders, W. Dana
- Published
- 2006
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
17. Confounding in publications of observational intervention studies
- Author
-
Arno W. Hoes, Eelko Hak, and Rolf H.H. Groenwold
- Subjects
medicine.medical_specialty ,bias ,MEDLINE ,education ,Alternative medicine ,VACCINE ,Observation ,Epidemiology ,medicine ,Humans ,confounding factors ,business.industry ,Public health ,Confounding ,Confounding Factors, Epidemiologic ,methodology ,Intervention studies ,Treatment Outcome ,Bibliometrics ,Family medicine ,Observational study ,epidemiology ,Periodicals as Topic ,business ,Medical literature - Abstract
We conducted a systematic literature search in Medline to assess the proportion of observational intervention studies appreciating confounding bias in peer-reviewed medical literature from 1985 through 2005. This study shows only 9% of all papers on observational intervention studies published in peer-reviewed medical journals mention any of the terms (confounding, adjustment, or bias) indicating appreciation of confounding.
- Published
- 2007
18. Spontaneous abortion in a hospital population: Are tobacco and coffee intake risk factors?
- Author
-
P. Ortega-Molina, V. Domínguez-Rojas, J. R. de Juanes-Pardo, P. Astasio-Arbiza, and E. Gordillo-Florencio
- Subjects
Adult ,Gerontology ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Alcohol Drinking ,Epidemiology ,Abortion ,Logistic regression ,Coffee ,Miscarriage ,Cohort Studies ,Pregnancy ,Recurrence ,Risk Factors ,Caffeine ,Odds Ratio ,medicine ,Humans ,Reproductive History ,Retrospective Studies ,Menarche ,Dose-Response Relationship, Drug ,Marital Status ,Obstetrics ,business.industry ,Smoking ,Confounding ,Age Factors ,Confounding Factors, Epidemiologic ,Retrospective cohort study ,Odds ratio ,medicine.disease ,Abortion, Spontaneous ,Logistic Models ,Case-Control Studies ,Marital status ,Female ,business - Abstract
The objective of this study was to examine the possible relationships between spontaneous abortion and caffeine, tobacco and alcohol intake in a well-controlled group of hospital workers. A retrospective cohort study design including 711 women, 20 to 41 years old, was used. All data regarding the purpose of this study were extracted from clinical histories registered at the Preventive Medicine Service. The dependent variable was spontaneous abortion and the independent variables were tobacco, coffee, and alcohol intake. Age, previous spontaneous abortion, menarcheal age and marital status were considered as potential confounders. The data were analyzed by multiple logistic regression. The following adjusted odds ratios of spontaneous abortion by caffeine consumption were calculated: 141-280 mg/day, 2.20 (1.22-3.96); 281-420 mg/day, 4.81 (2.28-10.14) and 421 or more, 15.43 (7.38-32.43); p0.05. The adjusted odds ratio for tobacco were 11 or more cigarettes/day, 3.35 (1.65-6.92); p0.05. It appears from this and other papers that tobacco and caffeine intake must be considered as clear risk factors for spontaneous abortion or miscarriage.
- Published
- 1994
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
19. The Janus face of statistical adjustment: confounders versus colliders
- Author
-
Anders Ahlbom, Imre Janszky, and Anna C. Svensson
- Subjects
Epidemiology ,business.industry ,Confounding ,Physical activity ,Data interpretation ,Confounding Factors, Epidemiologic ,Causality ,Bias ,Data Interpretation, Statistical ,Epidemiologic Research Design ,Econometrics ,Medicine ,Humans ,Observational study ,Statistical analysis ,business - Abstract
It has long been established that controlling for confounders is essential to delineate the causal relationship between exposure and disease. For this purpose, statistical adjustment is widely used in observational studies. However, many researchers don't acknowledge the potential pitfalls of statistical adjustment. The aim of the present paper was to demonstrate that statistical adjustment is a double edged sword. By using numerically identical examples, we show that adjustment for a common consequence of the exposure and the outcome can lead to as much bias as absence of necessary adjustment for a confounder.
- Published
- 2010
20. Predictors of follow-up and assessment of selection bias from dropouts using inverse probability weighting in a cohort of university graduates
- Author
-
Almudena Sánchez-Villegas, Maria Segui-Gomez, Alvaro Alonso, Miguel Ángel Martínez-González, Jokin de Irala, and Juan José Beunza
- Subjects
Gerontology ,Adult ,Male ,Patient Dropouts ,Universities ,Epidemiology ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Overweight ,Rate ratio ,Body Mass Index ,Cohort Studies ,Surveys and Questionnaires ,Medicine ,Humans ,Obesity ,Students ,Selection Bias ,media_common ,Probability ,Selection bias ,business.industry ,Inverse probability weighting ,Confounding ,Middle Aged ,Confidence interval ,Cohort ,Hypertension ,Female ,medicine.symptom ,business ,Demography ,Cohort study ,Follow-Up Studies - Abstract
Dropouts in cohort studies can introduce selection bias. In this paper, we aimed (i) to assess predictors of retention in a cohort study (the SUN Project) where participants are followed-up through biennial mailed questionnaires, and (ii) to evaluate whether differential follow-up introduced selection bias in rate ratio (RR) estimates. The SUN Study recruited 9907 participants from December 1999 to January 2002. Among them, 8647 (87%) participants answered the 2-year follow-up questionnaire. The presence of missing information in key variables at baseline, being younger, smoker, a marital status different of married, being obese/overweight and a history of motor vehicle injury were associated with being lost to follow-up, while a self-reported history of cardiovascular disease predicted a higher retention proportion. To assess whether differential follow-up affected RR estimates, we studied the association between body mass index and the risk of hypertension, using inverse probability weighting (IPW) to adjust for confounding and selection bias. Obese individuals had a higher crude rate of hypertension compared with normoweight participants (RR=6.4, 95% confidence interval (CI): 3.9-10.5). Adjustment for confounding using IPW attenuated the risk of hypertension associated to obesity (RR=2.4, 95% CI: 1.1-5.3). Additional adjustment for selection bias did not modify the estimations. In conclusion, we show that the follow-up through mailed questionnaires of a geographically disperse cohort in Spain is possible. Furthermore, we show that despite existing differences between retained or lost to follow-up participants this may not necessarily have an important impact on the RR estimates of hypertension associated to obesity.
- Published
- 2006
21. Duration of breast feeding and cognitive function: Population based cohort study
- Author
-
Finbar O'Callaghan, Antônio Augusto Moura da Silva, and Ziyah Mehta
- Subjects
Pediatrics ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Time Factors ,Epidemiology ,Birth weight ,Population ,Structural equation modeling ,Cognition ,Linear regression ,medicine ,Humans ,Longitudinal Studies ,education ,Child ,education.field_of_study ,business.industry ,Confounding ,Models, Theoretical ,Breast Feeding ,Child, Preschool ,Standardized coefficient ,Regression Analysis ,Female ,business ,Breast feeding ,Cohort study ,Demography - Abstract
Some evidence suggests that breast feeding is weakly but positively associated with cognitive function. This association has been robust to adjustment for various confounders. The aim of this paper is to determine if duration of breast feeding is associated with cognitive function in late childhood. Data was abstracted from the 1970 British Cohort Study. 11004 liveborn white singletons born during 5-11 April 1970 in the United Kingdom were followed from birth to 10 years. Cognitive function at 10 years is the dependent variable, a latent construct composed of one ability test and three performance measures. Estimates derived from multiple linear regression and structural equation modeling were compared. Effect sizes were estimated using standardized coefficients (SC). Differences in cognitive function according to breast feeding duration were estimated to be small by multiple linear regression (SC = 0.07) and much smaller and non-significant as estimated by structural equation modeling (SC = 0.02) after adjusting for parental socioeconomic status (SES), birth weight, parity, gestational age, maternal age and maternal smoking. Differences in cognitive function according to duration of breast feeding appear to be small and of little clinical importance as estimated by structural equation modeling.
- Published
- 2006
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.