50 results on '"Victor de Lafuente"'
Search Results
2. Different subtypes of motor cortex pyramidal tract neurons projects to red and pontine nuclei
- Author
-
Veronica Lopez-Virgen, Rafael Olivares-Moreno, Victor de Lafuente, Luis Concha, and Gerardo Rojas-Piloni
- Subjects
Cellular and Molecular Neuroscience - Abstract
IntroductionPyramidal tract neurons (PTNs) are fundamental elements for motor control. However, it is largely unknown if PTNs are segregated into different subtypes with distinct characteristics.MethodsUsing anatomical and electrophysiological tools, we analyzed in mice motor cortex PTNs projecting to red and pontine midbrain nuclei, which are important hubs connecting cerebral cortex and cerebellum playing a critical role in the regulation of movement.ResultsWe reveal that the vast majority of M1 neurons projecting to the red and pontine nuclei constitutes different populations. Corticopontine neurons have higher conduction velocities and morphologically, a most homogeneous dendritic and spine distributions along cortical layers.DiscussionThe results indicate that cortical neurons projecting to the red and pontine nuclei constitute distinct anatomical and functional pathways which may contribute differently to sensorimotor integration.
- Published
- 2022
3. An abstract categorical decision code in dorsal premotor cortex
- Author
-
Gabriel Diaz-deLeon, Manuel Alvarez, Lucas Bayones, Antonio Zainos, Jerónimo Zizumbo, Sergio Parra, Sebastián Pujalte, Ranulfo Romo, Román Rossi-Pool, and Victor de Lafuente
- Subjects
Neurons ,Multidisciplinary ,Movement ,Motor Cortex - Abstract
The dorsal premotor cortex (DPC) has classically been associated with a role in preparing and executing the physical motor variables during cognitive tasks. While recent work has provided nuanced insights into this role, here we propose that DPC also participates more actively in decision-making. We recorded neuronal activity in DPC while two trained monkeys performed a vibrotactile categorization task, utilizing two distinct ranges of stimuli values that varied on two physical attributes: vibrotactile frequency and amplitude. We observed a broad heterogeneity across DPC neurons, the majority of which maintained the same response patterns across attributes and ranges, coding in the same periods, mixing temporal and categorical dynamics. The predominant categorical signal was maintained throughout the delay, movement periods and notably during the inter-trial period. Putting the entire population’s data through two dimensionality reduction techniques, we found that imposing the sensory structure yielded pure categorical and temporal representations. Furthermore, projecting the activity of one population over the population axes of the other yielded identical categorical and temporal responses. Finally, we sought to identify functional subpopulations based on the combined activity of all stimuli, neurons, and time points, however we found a continuum of single-unit responses mixing temporal and categorical dynamics. All this points to DPC playing a more decision-related role than previously anticipated.SIGNIFICANCE STATEMENTThe DPC’s role in the somatosensory processing network has been generally limited to movement, but our current results suggest a more abstract function. We recorded DPC’s activity in two monkeys trained in a vibrotactile categorization task of two distinct physical attributes, and found a strong decision signal throughout the population, underpinned by purely temporal signals. Importantly, this abstract decision signal remains during the inter-trial period suggesting a consolidation role. Neurons maintained consistent and significant responses for both attributes, and the entire population activity converged to identical categorical representations, even when cross-projected between two contexts. These results suggest that DPC plays a larger role during decision-making and consolidation, regardless of the stimulus attributes that triggered the decision report.
- Published
- 2022
4. Motor Cortex Projections To Red Nucleus And Pons Have Distinct Functional Roles In The Mouse
- Author
-
Veronica Lopez-Virgen, Martin Macias, Paola Rodriguez-Moreno, Rafael Olivares-Moreno, Victor de Lafuente, Luis Concha, and Gerardo Rojas-Piloni
- Abstract
Pyramidal tract neurons (PTNs) are fundamental elements for motor control. However, it is largely unknown if PTNs are segregated into different subtypes with distinct roles in movement performance. Using anatomical, electrophysiological and optogenetics tools, we analyzed in both sexes’ mice motor cortex, PTNs projecting to red and pontine midbrain nuclei, which are important hubs connecting cerebral cortex and cerebellum playing a critical role in the regulation of movement. We reveal that vast majority of M1 neurons projecting to the red and pontine nuclei constitutes different populations. Corticopontine neurons have higher conduction velocities and morphologically, a most homogeneous dendritic and spine distributions along cortical layers. Optogenetically inhibiting either kind projection, differentially affects forelimb movement onset and execution in a lever press task, but only the activity of corticopontine neurons is significantly correlated with trial-by-trial variations in reaction time. The results indicate that cortical neurons projecting to the red and pontine nuclei constitute distinct functional and anatomical pathways and they contribute differently to sensorimotor integration, suggesting that layer 5 output neurons are functionally compartmentalized generating, in parallel, different downstream coding.
- Published
- 2022
5. Motor cortex projections to red and pontine nuclei have distinct roles during movement in the mouse
- Author
-
Veronica Lopez-Virgen, Martín Macías, Paola Rodriguez-Moreno, Rafael Olivares-Moreno, Victor de Lafuente, and Gerardo Rojas-Piloni
- Subjects
General Neuroscience - Published
- 2023
6. Discrimination of Regular and Irregular Rhythms Explained by a Time Difference Accumulation Model
- Author
-
Marisol Espinoza-Monroy and Victor de Lafuente
- Subjects
0301 basic medicine ,business.industry ,General Neuroscience ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Sensory system ,Pattern recognition ,Pulse (music) ,03 medical and health sciences ,030104 developmental biology ,0302 clinical medicine ,Rhythm ,Categorization ,Touch ,Perception ,Bounded function ,Time difference ,Auditory Perception ,Psychophysics ,Humans ,Artificial intelligence ,business ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,media_common ,Mathematics - Abstract
Perceiving the temporal regularity in a sequence of repetitive sensory events facilitates the preparation and execution of relevant behaviors with tight temporal constraints. How we estimate temporal regularity from repeating patterns of sensory stimuli is not completely understood. We developed a decision-making task in which participants had to decide whether a train of visual, auditory, or tactile pulses, had a regular or an irregular temporal pattern. We tested the hypothesis that subjects categorize stimuli as irregular by accumulating the time differences between the predicted and observed times of sensory pulses defining a temporal rhythm. Results suggest that instead of waiting for a single large temporal deviation, participants accumulate timing-error signals and judge a pattern as irregular when the amount of evidence reaches a decision threshold. Model fits of bounded integration showed that this accumulation occurs with negligible leak of evidence. Consistent with previous findings, we show that participants perform better when evaluating the regularity of auditory pulses, as compared with visual or tactile stimuli. Our results suggest that temporal regularity is estimated by comparing expected and measured pulse onset times, and that each prediction error is accumulated towards a threshold to generate a behavioral choice.
- Published
- 2021
7. Look Who is Talking. Identities and Expressions in the Prefrontal Cortex
- Author
-
Luis, Lemus and Victor, de Lafuente
- Subjects
Facial Expression ,General Neuroscience ,Emotions ,Prefrontal Cortex - Published
- 2022
8. Purkinje Cells are Key for Rhythmic Predictions in the Cerebellum
- Author
-
Mildred Salgado-Ménez and Victor de Lafuente
- Subjects
Purkinje Cells ,General Neuroscience ,Cerebellum ,Action Potentials - Published
- 2021
9. Prefrontal and Premotor Cortices Encode Inference Judgements
- Author
-
Karla, Mercado and Victor, de Lafuente
- Subjects
Judgment ,General Neuroscience ,Motor Cortex ,Prefrontal Cortex - Published
- 2022
10. Discrimination of regular and irregular rhythms by accumulation of time differences
- Author
-
Marisol Espinoza-Monroy and Victor de Lafuente
- Subjects
Rhythm ,Categorization ,business.industry ,Mean squared prediction error ,Pattern recognition ,Sensory system ,Behavioral choice ,Artificial intelligence ,Pulse (music) ,business ,Mathematics ,Task (project management) ,Tactile stimuli - Abstract
Perceiving the temporal regularity in a sequence of repetitive sensory events facilitates the preparation and execution of relevant behaviors with tight temporal constraints. How we estimate temporal regularity from repeating patterns of sensory stimuli is not completely understood. We developed a decision-making task in which participants had to decide whether a train of visual, auditory, or tactile pulses, had a regular or an irregular temporal pattern. We tested the hypothesis that subjects categorize stimuli as irregular by accumulating the time differences between the predicted and observed times of sensory pulses defining a temporal rhythm. Results show that instead of waiting for a single large temporal deviation, participants accumulate timing-error signals and judge a pattern as irregular when the amount of evidence reaches a decision threshold. Model fits of bounded integration showed that this accumulation occurs with negligible leak of evidence. Consistent with previous findings, we show that participants perform better when evaluating the regularity of auditory pulses, as compared with visual or tactile stimuli. Our results suggest that temporal regularity is estimated by comparing expected and measured pulse onset times, and that each prediction error is accumulated towards a threshold to generate a behavioral choice.
- Published
- 2020
11. Memory Consolidation during Sleep Might be Regulated by the Locus Coeruleus
- Author
-
Victor de Lafuente
- Subjects
General Neuroscience ,Prefrontal Cortex ,Sleep, REM ,Locus coeruleus ,Locus Coeruleus ,Memory consolidation ,Sleep ,Psychology ,Neuroscience ,Sleep in non-human animals ,Memory Consolidation - Published
- 2021
12. Integrating Somatosensory Information Over Time
- Author
-
Victor de Lafuente, Raúl Hernández-Pérez, and Eduardo Rojas-Hortelano
- Subjects
0301 basic medicine ,Computer science ,General Neuroscience ,Process (computing) ,Sensory system ,Somatosensory Cortex ,Stimulus (physiology) ,Horizontal plane ,Object (computer science) ,Somatosensory system ,Object (philosophy) ,Task (project management) ,03 medical and health sciences ,Independent Sampling ,030104 developmental biology ,0302 clinical medicine ,Touch Perception ,Touch ,Bounded function ,Humans ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Our choices are often informed by temporally integrating streams of sensory information. This has been well demonstrated in the visual and auditory domains, but the integration of tactile information over time has been less studied. We designed an active touch task in which subjects explored a spheroid-shaped object to determine its inclination with respect to the horizontal plane (inclined to the left or to the right). In agreement with previous findings, our results show that more errors, and longer decision times, accompany difficult decisions (small inclination angles). To gain insight into the decision-making process, we used a task in which the time available for tactile exploration was varied by the experimenter, in a trial-by-trial basis. The behavioral results were fit with a model of bounded accumulation, and also with an independent-sampling model which assumes no sensory accumulation. The results of model fits favor an accumulation-to-bound mechanism, and suggest that participants integrate the first 600 ms of 1800 ms-long stimuli. This means that the somatosensory system benefits from longer streams of information although it does not make use of all available evidence.HighlightsThe somatosensory system integrates information streams through time.Somatosensory discrimination thresholds decrease with longer stimuli.A bounded accumulation model is favored over independent sampling.Humans accumulate up to 600 ms, out of 1800 ms-long stimuli.
- Published
- 2019
13. Author response: Sucrose intensity coding and decision-making in rat gustatory cortices
- Author
-
Esmeralda Fonseca, Sidney A. Simon, Ranier Gutierrez, and Victor de Lafuente
- Subjects
chemistry.chemical_compound ,Sucrose ,chemistry ,Biology ,Neuroscience ,Coding (social sciences) - Published
- 2018
14. Sucrose intensity coding and decision-making in rat gustatory cortices
- Author
-
Esmeralda Fonseca, Victor de Lafuente, Sidney A. Simon, and Ranier Gutierrez
- Subjects
0301 basic medicine ,Taste ,obesity ,Sucrose ,decision-variables ,QH301-705.5 ,Science ,Decision Making ,Action Potentials ,Prefrontal Cortex ,Biology ,behavioral disciplines and activities ,General Biochemistry, Genetics and Molecular Biology ,taste coding ,Intensity discrimination ,03 medical and health sciences ,chemistry.chemical_compound ,0302 clinical medicine ,Animals ,Gustatory system ,Biology (General) ,reward ,Cerebral Cortex ,Neurons ,General Immunology and Microbiology ,General Neuroscience ,General Medicine ,Sweetness ,Intensity (physics) ,Rats ,030104 developmental biology ,Taste intensity ,chemistry ,nervous system ,Medicine ,Rat ,Orbitofrontal cortex ,Neuroscience ,Insula ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,psychological phenomena and processes ,Coding (social sciences) ,Research Article ,taste Intensity - Abstract
Sucrose’s sweet intensity is one attribute contributing to the overconsumption of high-energy palatable foods. However, it is not known how sucrose intensity is encoded and used to make perceptual decisions by neurons in taste-sensitive cortices. We trained rats in a sucrose intensity discrimination task and found that sucrose evoked a widespread response in neurons recorded in posterior-Insula (pIC), anterior-Insula (aIC), and Orbitofrontal cortex (OFC). Remarkably, only a few Intensity-selective neurons conveyed the most information about sucrose’s intensity, indicating that for sweetness the gustatory system uses a compact and distributed code. Sucrose intensity was encoded in both firing-rates and spike-timing. The pIC, aIC, and OFC neurons tracked movement direction, with OFC neurons yielding the most robust response. aIC and OFC neurons encoded the subject’s choices, whereas all three regions tracked reward omission. Overall, these multimodal areas provide a neural representation of perceived sucrose intensity, and of task-related information underlying perceptual decision-making., eLife digest Imagine you wake up in the morning, and you pour yourself and your loved one coffee. They like it with two sugars but you only with one. Our ability to distinguish different sweet intensities allows us to detect how much sugar is in the coffee. It also helps us to predict the amount of energy present in foods and if it is safe to ingest. We can experience the sweet quality because our tongue contains sweet taste receptor cells that are switched on by sugar. This activates neurons across our taste system in the brain. However, we do not completely understand how these areas represent the intensity of sugar. Previous studies have only ‘passively’ measured different sugar concentrations, either using anesthetized animals or behavioral tasks that do not involve decision-making other than licking. But to accurately evaluate how animals perceive the intensity, active decision-making is required, such us ‘reporting’ the perceived concentration of sugar. Fonseca et al. set out to answer this question by training rats in a new sweet intensity discrimination task, in which the rats had to move to the left or right to obtain water as a reward. This way, the animals could ‘indicate’ how sweet they perceived the sugar water to be. At the same time, recordings from the three brain areas involved in taste responses were taken (called the anterior and posterior insular cortices, and the orbitofrontal cortex) to measure how the sugar intensity is processed in the brain. The results showed that a small group of neurons within all three areas contained more information about the sugar intensity than other neurons, suggesting the taste system uses a compact and distributed code to represent its intensity. The information about sugar intensity was contained in both the number of nerve impulses and in the precise timing with which these neurons fired. Many drinks and high-energy foods often contain large quantities of sugar, and their overconsumption contributes to the worldwide problems of obesity and its associated diseases. Therefore, a better understanding of the neurons that code information about the intensity of sugar could be a starting point for other studies to pinpoint the connections and areas in the brain involved in our irremediable attraction for sugar.
- Published
- 2018
15. Author response: Entrainment and maintenance of an internal metronome in supplementary motor area
- Author
-
Hugo Merchant, Jaime Cadena-Valencia, Victor de Lafuente, Mehrdad Jazayeri, and Otto García-Garibay
- Subjects
medicine.medical_specialty ,Physical medicine and rehabilitation ,medicine.anatomical_structure ,Supplementary motor area ,law ,medicine ,Environmental science ,Metronome ,Entrainment (chronobiology) ,law.invention - Published
- 2018
16. Entrainment and maintenance of an internal metronome in supplementary motor area
- Author
-
Mehrdad Jazayeri, Otto García-Garibay, Victor de Lafuente, Hugo Merchant, and Jaime Cadena-Valencia
- Subjects
0301 basic medicine ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Periodicity ,QH301-705.5 ,Science ,Local field potential ,Metronome ,Audiology ,primate ,General Biochemistry, Genetics and Molecular Biology ,law.invention ,03 medical and health sciences ,Neural activity ,0302 clinical medicine ,Rhythm ,supplementary motor area ,law ,Rhesus macaque ,medicine ,timing ,Animals ,Gamma Rhythm ,Biology (General) ,Stopwatch ,Neurons ,local field potential ,General Immunology and Microbiology ,Supplementary motor area ,General Neuroscience ,Motor Cortex ,General Medicine ,Haplorhini ,SMA ,rhythm perception ,030104 developmental biology ,medicine.anatomical_structure ,Medicine ,Psychology ,Gamma band ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Research Article ,Neuroscience - Abstract
To prepare timely motor actions, we constantly predict future events. Regularly repeating events are often perceived as a rhythm to which we can readily synchronize our movements, just as in dancing to music. However, the neuronal mechanisms underlying the capacity to encode and maintain rhythms are not understood. We trained nonhuman primates to maintain the rhythm of a visual metronome of diverse tempos and recorded neural activity in the supplementary motor area (SMA). SMA exhibited rhythmic bursts of gamma band (30–40 Hz) reflecting an internal tempo that matched the extinguished visual metronome. Moreover, gamma amplitude increased throughout the trial, providing an estimate of total elapsed time. Notably, the timing of gamma bursts and firing rate modulations allowed predicting whether monkeys were ahead or behind the correct tempo. Our results indicate that SMA uses dynamic motor plans to encode a metronome for rhythms and a stopwatch for total elapsed time., eLife digest A catchy tune on the radio, and suddenly we are tapping our foot and moving our bodies to the rhythm of the music. We can follow a beat because our motor neurons, the nerve cells that control movements, work together in circuits. During actions that require precise timing – such as dancing to a rhythm – the motor neurons within these circuits increase and decrease their activity in complex patterns. But recent evidence shows that these motor neuron circuits also ‘switch on’ simply when we perceive a rhythm, even if we do not move to it. In fact, just imagining a rhythm triggers the same symphony of electrical activity in the brain. How do motor neurons generate coordinated patterns of activity without movement or even an external stimulus? Cadena-Valencia et al. set out to answer this question by training monkeys to follow a rhythm. The animals learned to track a dot that appeared alternately on the left and right sides of a touchscreen with a regular tempo. After a few repeats, the dot disappeared. The monkeys then had to continue mentally tracking where the dot would have been. A group of neurons in a brain region called the supplementary motor area synchronized their activity with the dot. Whenever the dot was due to appear, the neurons in the area showed a burst of rapid firing. These spikes of activity, called gamma bursts, helped the motor neurons to communicate with one another within their circuits. The gamma bursts thus acted as an internal metronome, making it easier for the monkeys to follow the rhythm. These results should be a starting point for other studies to pinpoint exactly where and how this rhythmic activity arises, and how the brain uses gamma bursts to synchronize our movements to a tempo.
- Published
- 2018
17. Regaining the senses of touch and movement
- Author
-
Victor de Lafuente
- Subjects
somatosensation ,0301 basic medicine ,somatosensory cortex ,QH301-705.5 ,proprioception ,Science ,Somatosensory system ,General Biochemistry, Genetics and Molecular Biology ,03 medical and health sciences ,paraplegia ,0302 clinical medicine ,Cortex (anatomy) ,medicine ,Biology (General) ,Brain–computer interface ,integumentary system ,General Immunology and Microbiology ,Proprioception ,Movement (music) ,General Neuroscience ,fungi ,intracortical microstimulation ,food and beverages ,General Medicine ,030104 developmental biology ,Intracortical microstimulation ,medicine.anatomical_structure ,Medicine ,Psychology ,brain-machine interface ,Neuroscience ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery - Abstract
Artificially activating neurons in the cortex can make a tetraplegic patient feel naturalistic sensations of skin pressure and arm movement.
- Published
- 2018
18. Dopamine reward prediction error signal codes the temporal evaluation of a perceptual decision report
- Author
-
Stefania Sarno, Victor de Lafuente, Néstor Parga, and Ranulfo Romo
- Subjects
0301 basic medicine ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Dopamine ,Decision Making ,Models, Neurological ,Sensory system ,Stimulus (physiology) ,Choice Behavior ,Membrane Potentials ,03 medical and health sciences ,Bayes' theorem ,0302 clinical medicine ,Reward ,Mesencephalon ,Perception ,medicine ,Reinforcement learning ,Animals ,Sensory cue ,media_common ,Multidisciplinary ,business.industry ,Dopaminergic Neurons ,Bayes Theorem ,Haplorhini ,030104 developmental biology ,Perceptual decision ,PNAS Plus ,Touch ,Artificial intelligence ,Cues ,Psychology ,business ,Neuroscience ,Microelectrodes ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,medicine.drug - Abstract
Learning to associate unambiguous sensory cues with rewarded choices is known to be mediated by dopamine (DA) neurons. However, little is known about how these neurons behave when choices rely on uncertain reward-predicting stimuli. To study this issue we reanalyzed DA recordings from monkeys engaged in the detection of weak tactile stimuli delivered at random times and formulated a reinforcement learning model based on belief states. Specifically, we investigated how the firing activity of DA neurons should behave if they were coding the error in the prediction of the total future reward when animals made decisions relying on uncertain sensory and temporal information. Our results show that the same signal that codes for reward prediction errors also codes the animal’s certainty about the presence of the stimulus and the temporal expectation of sensory cues.
- Published
- 2017
19. Monkeys Share the Human Ability to Internally Maintain a Temporal Rhythm
- Author
-
Victor de Lafuente, Jaime Cadena-Valencia, Hugo Merchant, and Otto García-Garibay
- Subjects
rhesus ,Stimulus (physiology) ,rhythm ,050105 experimental psychology ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,Rhythm ,timing ,Rhythm perception ,Psychology ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Sensory cue ,General Psychology ,Prior information ,Original Research ,Communication ,business.industry ,Weber fraction ,05 social sciences ,Motor commands ,model of time perception ,Cognition ,Time perception ,business ,Neuroscience ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery - Abstract
Timing is a fundamental variable for behavior. However, the mechanisms allowing human and non-human primates to synchronize their actions with periodic events are not yet completely understood. Here we characterize the ability of rhesus monkeys and humans to perceive and maintain rhythms of different paces in the absence of sensory cues or motor actions. In our rhythm task subjects had to observe and then internally follow a visual stimulus that periodically changed its location along a circular perimeter. Crucially, they had to maintain this visuospatial tempo in the absence of movements. Our results show that the probability of remaining in synchrony with the rhythm decreased, and the variability in the timing estimates increased, as a function of elapsed time, and these trends were well described by the generalized law of Weber. Additionally, the pattern of errors shows that human subjects tended to lag behind fast rhythms and to get ahead of slow ones, suggesting that a mean tempo might be incorporated as prior information. Overall, our results demonstrate that rhythm perception and maintenance are cognitive abilities that we share with rhesus monkeys, and these abilities do not depend on overt motor commands.
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
20. Tactile object categories can be decoded from the parietal and lateral-occipital cortices
- Author
-
Raúl Hernández-Pérez, Victor de Lafuente, Azalea Reyes-Aguilar, Laura V. Cuaya, Luis Concha, and Eduardo Rojas-Hortelano
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,genetic structures ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Object (grammar) ,Somatosensory system ,050105 experimental psychology ,Functional Laterality ,Temporal lobe ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,Perception ,Parietal Lobe ,Image Processing, Computer-Assisted ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,media_common ,Afferent Pathways ,Brain Mapping ,General Neuroscience ,05 social sciences ,Representation (systemics) ,Cognitive neuroscience of visual object recognition ,Cognition ,SMA ,Hand ,Magnetic Resonance Imaging ,Oxygen ,Touch Perception ,Touch ,Female ,Occipital Lobe ,Psychology ,Neuroscience ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
The visual system classifies objects into categories, and distinct populations of neurons within the temporal lobe respond preferentially to objects of a given perceptual category. We can also classify the objects we recognize with the sense of touch, but less is known about the neuronal correlates underlying this cognitive function. To address this question, we performed a multivariate pattern analysis (MVPA) of functional magnetic resonance imagining (fMRI) activity to identify the cortical areas that can be used to decode the category of objects explored with the hand. We observed that tactile object category can be decoded from the activity patterns of somatosensory and parietal areas. Importantly, we found that categories can also be decoded from the lateral occipital complex (LOC), which is a multimodal region known to be related to the representation of object shape. Furthermore, a hyperalignment analysis showed that activity patterns are similar across subjects. Our results thus indicate that tactile object recognition generates category-specific patterns of activity in a multisensory area known to encode objects, and that these patterns have a similar functional organization across individuals.
- Published
- 2016
21. Perceptual detection as a dynamical bistability phenomenon: A neurocomputational correlate of sensation
- Author
-
Gustavo Deco, Victor de Lafuente, Ranulfo Romo, Mar Pérez-Sanagustín, and Universitat Pompeu Fabra [Barcelona] (UPF)
- Subjects
Bistability ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Models, Neurological ,Sensation ,Prefrontal Cortex ,Somatosensory system ,Premotor cortex ,[SCCO]Cognitive science ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,Perception ,medicine ,Animals ,Contrast (vision) ,ComputingMilieux_MISCELLANEOUS ,Multistability ,030304 developmental biology ,media_common ,Neurons ,Physics ,0303 health sciences ,Multidisciplinary ,Computational Biology ,Biological Sciences ,Neurophysiology ,Macaca mulatta ,Electrophysiology ,medicine.anatomical_structure ,Neuroscience ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery - Abstract
Recent studies that combined psychophysical/neurophysiological experiments [de Lafuente V, Romo R (2005) Nat Neurosci 8:1698–1703] analyzed the responses from single neurons, recorded in several cortical areas of parietal and frontal lobes, while trained monkeys reported the presence or absence of a mechanical vibration of varying amplitude applied to skin of one fingertip. The analysis showed that the activity of primary somatosensory cortex neurons covaried with the stimulus strength but did not covary with the animal's perceptual reports. In contrast, the activity of medial premotor cortex (MPC) neurons did not covary with the stimulus strength but did covary with the animal's perceptual reports. Here, we address the question of how perceptual detection is computed in MPC. In particular, we regard perceptual detection as a bistable neurodynamical phenomenon reflected in the activity of MPC. We show that the activity of MPC is consistent with a decision-making-like scenario of fluctuation-driven computation that causes a probabilistic transition between two bistable states, one corresponding to the case in which the monkey detects the sensory input, the other corresponding to the case in which the monkey does not. Moreover, the high variability activity of MPC neurons both within and between trials reflects stochastic fluctuations that may play a crucial role in the monkey's probabilistic perceptual reports.
- Published
- 2007
22. Comment on 'Single-trial spike trains in parietal cortex reveal discrete steps during decision-making'
- Author
-
Tianming Yang, Jochen Ditterich, Daniel M. Wolpert, Victor de Lafuente, Joshua I. Gold, Jamie D. Roitman, Michael N. Shadlen, William T. Newsome, Ariel Zylberberg, and Roozbeh Kiani
- Subjects
0301 basic medicine ,Male ,Multidisciplinary ,Computer science ,Decision Making ,Posterior parietal cortex ,Choice Behavior ,Article ,03 medical and health sciences ,030104 developmental biology ,0302 clinical medicine ,Perceptual decision ,Time windows ,Parietal Lobe ,Animals ,Spike (software development) ,Single trial ,Neuroscience ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery - Abstract
Shadlen et al.’s Comment focuses on extrapolations of our results that were not implied or asserted in our Report. They discuss alternate analyses of average firing rates in other tasks, the relationship between neural activity and behavior, and possible extensions of the standard models we examined. Although interesting to contemplate, these points are not germane to the findings of our Report: that stepping dynamics provided a better statistical description of lateral intraparietal area spike trains than diffusion-to-bound dynamics for a majority of neurons.
- Published
- 2015
23. Neural correlate of subjective sensory experience gradually builds up across cortical areas
- Author
-
Ranulfo Romo and Victor de Lafuente
- Subjects
Multidisciplinary ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Parietal lobe ,Sensory system ,Somatosensory system ,behavioral disciplines and activities ,medicine.anatomical_structure ,nervous system ,Frontal lobe ,Somatosensory evoked potential ,Cerebral cortex ,Perception ,medicine ,Premovement neuronal activity ,Psychology ,Neuroscience ,media_common - Abstract
When a sensory stimulus is presented, many cortical areas are activated, but how does the representation of a sensory stimulus evolve in time and across cortical areas during a perceptual judgment? We investigated this question by analyzing the responses from single neurons, recorded in several cortical areas of parietal and frontal lobes, while trained monkeys reported the presence or absence of a mechanical vibration of varying amplitude applied to the skin of one fingertip. Here we show that the strength of the covariations between neuronal activity and perceptual judgments progressively increases across cortical areas as the activity is transmitted from the primary somatosensory cortex to the premotor areas of the frontal lobe. This finding suggests that the neuronal correlates of subjective sensory experience gradually build up across somatosensory areas of the parietal lobe and premotor cortices of the frontal lobe.
- Published
- 2006
24. Neuronal correlates of subjective sensory experience
- Author
-
Ranulfo Romo and Victor de Lafuente
- Subjects
Surround suppression ,Action Potentials ,Sensory system ,Stimulus (physiology) ,Vibration ,behavioral disciplines and activities ,Sensory neuroscience ,Fingers ,Physical Stimulation ,Neural Pathways ,medicine ,Animals ,Microstimulation ,Sensory cortex ,Neurons ,Afferent Pathways ,General Neuroscience ,Motor Cortex ,Somatosensory Cortex ,Adequate stimulus ,Macaca mulatta ,Electrophysiology ,medicine.anatomical_structure ,Frontal lobe ,Touch ,Sensory Thresholds ,Psychology ,Mechanoreceptors ,Neuroscience - Abstract
When a near-threshold stimulus is presented, a sensory percept may or may not be produced. The unpredictable outcome of such perceptual judgment is believed to be determined by the activity of neurons in early sensory cortex. We analyzed the responses of neurons in primary somatosensory cortex, recorded while monkeys judged the presence or absence of threshold stimuli. We found that these responses did not covary with the monkeys' perceptual reports. In contrast, the activity of frontal lobe neurons did covary with trial-by-trial judgments. Further control and microstimulation experiments indicated that frontal lobe neurons are closely related to the monkeys' subjective experiences during sensory detection.
- Published
- 2005
25. From sensation to action
- Author
-
Rogelio Luna, Carlos D. Brody, Emilio Salinas, Adrián Hernández, Luis Lemus, Ranulfo Romo, Antonio Zainos, and Victor de Lafuente
- Subjects
Behavior ,Memoria ,Sensation ,Brain ,Cognition ,Haplorhini ,Somatosensory Cortex ,Stimulus (physiology) ,Somatosensory system ,Vibration ,Behavioral Neuroscience ,Discrimination, Psychological ,nervous system ,Physical Stimulation ,Psychophysics ,Animals ,Humans ,Premovement neuronal activity ,Neurons, Afferent ,Psychology ,Neural coding ,Neuroscience - Abstract
Key to understanding somatosensation is the form of how the mechanical stimuli are represented in the evoked neuronal activity of the brain. Here, we focus on studies that address the question of which components of the evoked neuronal activity in the somatosensory system represent the stimulus features. We review experiments that probe whether these neuronal representations are essential to somatosensation. We also discuss recent results that suggest how the somatosensory stimuli are represented in the brain during short-term memory. Finally, we review data that show the neuronal correlates of a decision during somatosensory perception.
- Published
- 2002
26. Introduction to the neurobiology of interval timing
- Author
-
Hugo, Merchant and Victor, de Lafuente
- Subjects
Neurobiology ,Time Perception ,Animals ,Humans ,Sensorimotor Cortex ,Motor Activity ,Circadian Rhythm - Abstract
Time is a fundamental variable that organisms must quantify in order to survive. In humans, for example, the gradual development of the sense of duration and rhythm is an essential skill in many facets of social behavior such as speaking, dancing to-, listening to- or playing music, performing a wide variety of sports, and driving a car (Merchant H, Harrington DL, Meck WH. Annu Rev Neurosci. 36:313-36, 2013). During the last 10 years there has been a rapid growth of research on the neural underpinnings of timing in the subsecond and suprasecond scales, using a variety of methodological approaches in the human being, as well as in varied animal and theoretical models. In this introductory chapter we attempt to give a conceptual framework that defines time processing as a family of different phenomena. The brain circuits and neural underpinnings of temporal quantification seem to largely depend on its time scale and the sensorimotor nature of specific behaviors. Therefore, we describe the main time scales and their associated behaviors and show how the perception and execution of timing events in the subsecond and second scales may depend on similar or different neural mechanisms.
- Published
- 2014
27. Dynamic Control of Response Criterion in Premotor Cortex during Perceptual Detection under Temporal Uncertainty
- Author
-
Victor de Lafuente, Ranulfo Romo, Néstor Parga, Federico Carnevale, and Omri Barak
- Subjects
Neuroscience(all) ,Speech recognition ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Population ,Motion Perception ,Sensory system ,Choice Behavior ,Task (project management) ,Premotor cortex ,Perception ,medicine ,Reaction Time ,Animals ,Attention ,education ,media_common ,Network model ,Communication ,education.field_of_study ,Neural correlates of consciousness ,Behavior, Animal ,business.industry ,Mechanism (biology) ,General Neuroscience ,Motor Cortex ,Uncertainty ,Haplorhini ,medicine.anatomical_structure ,Visual Perception ,business ,Psychology ,Photic Stimulation - Abstract
SummaryUnder uncertainty, the brain uses previous knowledge to transform sensory inputs into the percepts on which decisions are based. When the uncertainty lies in the timing of sensory evidence, however, the mechanism underlying the use of previously acquired temporal information remains unknown. We study this issue in monkeys performing a detection task with variable stimulation times. We use the neural correlates of false alarms to infer the subject’s response criterion and find that it modulates over the course of a trial. Analysis of premotor cortex activity shows that this modulation is represented by the dynamics of population responses. A trained recurrent network model reproduces the experimental findings and demonstrates a neural mechanism to benefit from temporal expectations in perceptual detection. Previous knowledge about the probability of stimulation over time can be intrinsically encoded in the neural population dynamics, allowing a flexible control of the response criterion over time.
- Published
- 2014
28. The parietal cortices participate in encoding, short-term memory, and decision-making related to tactile shape
- Author
-
Luis Concha, Eduardo Rojas-Hortelano, and Victor de Lafuente
- Subjects
Cognitive science ,Adult ,Male ,Communication ,Brain Mapping ,Physiology ,business.industry ,General Neuroscience ,Decision Making ,Cognitive neuroscience of visual object recognition ,Short-term memory ,Somatosensory system ,Magnetic Resonance Imaging ,Form Perception ,Young Adult ,Discrimination, Psychological ,Memory, Short-Term ,Touch Perception ,Encoding (memory) ,Parietal Lobe ,Physical Stimulation ,Humans ,Female ,Psychology ,business - Abstract
We routinely identify objects with our hands, and the physical attributes of touched objects are often held in short-term memory to aid future decisions. However, the brain structures that selectively process tactile information to encode object shape are not fully identified. In this article we describe the areas within the human cerebral cortex that specialize in encoding, short-term memory, and decision-making related to the shape of objects explored with the hand. We performed event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging in subjects performing a shape discrimination task in which two sequentially presented objects had to be explored to determine whether they had the same shape or not. To control for low-level and nonspecific brain activations, subjects performed a temperature discrimination task in which they compared the temperature of two spheres. Our results show that although a large network of brain structures is engaged in somatosensory processing, it is the areas lining the intraparietal sulcus that selectively participate in encoding, maintaining, and deciding on tactile information related to the shape of objects.
- Published
- 2014
29. Neural dynamics of perceptual detection under temporal uncertainty
- Author
-
Federico Carnevale, Ranulfo Romo, Omri Barak, Victor de Lafuente, and Néstor Parga
- Subjects
Cellular and Molecular Neuroscience ,General Neuroscience ,Philosophy ,Poster Presentation ,Humanities - Abstract
El paradigma experimental en el que se centro el trabajo de esta tesis corresponde a la tarea de deteccion de estimulos vibrotactiles. En la primera etapa de la investigacion, se procedio a estudiar las propiedades estadisticas de la actividad electrica de neuronas en varias areas corticales (cortezas somatosensoriales primaria y secundaria, premotora y motora). Especial enfasis se dio a la estadistica de segundo orden, es decir, las correlaciones en la actividad de partes de neuronas. Esta etapa del estudio condujo a interesantes resultados sobre la dinamica de los circuitos implicados y motivo las siguientes etapas de la investigacion. (sacado del extracto del informe del Dr. D. Nestor Parga Carballeda)
- Published
- 2014
30. Neurobiology of Interval Timing
- Author
-
Victor de Lafuente and Hugo Merchant
- Subjects
genetic structures ,medicine.diagnostic_test ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Polyrhythm ,Neurophysiology ,Time perception ,Electroencephalography ,behavioral disciplines and activities ,medicine.anatomical_structure ,Functional neuroimaging ,Perception ,medicine ,Psychology ,Prefrontal cortex ,Neuroscience ,psychological phenomena and processes ,media_common ,Motor cortex - Abstract
Introduction to the Neurobiology of Interval Timing.- About the (non)scalar property for time perception.- Elucidating the internal structure of psychophysical timing performance in the sub-second and second range by utilizing confirmatory factor analysis.- Neurocomputational models of time perception.- Dedicated Clock/Timing-Circuit Theories of Time Perception and Timed Performance.- Neural Dynamics Based Timing in the Subsecond to Seconds Range.- Signs of timing in motor cortex during movement preparation and cue anticipation.- Neurophysiology of timing in the hundreds of milliseconds: multiple layers of neuronal clocks in the medial premotor areas.- The Olivo-Cerebellar System as a Neural Clock.- From duration and distance comparisons to goal encoding in prefrontal cortex.- Probing Interval Timing with Scalp-recorded Electroencephalography (EEG).- Searching for the Holy Grail: Temporally Informative Firing Patterns in the Rat.- Getting the timing right: experimental protocols for investigating time with functional neuroimaging and psychopharmacology.- Motor and Perceptual timing in Parkinson's disease.- Music Perception: Information Flow within the Human Auditory Cortices.- Perceiving temporal regularity in music: The role of auditory event-related potentials (ERPs) in probing beat perception.- Neural Mechanisms of Rhythm Perception: Present Findings and Future Directions.- Neural underpinnings of music: The polyrhythmic brain.
- Published
- 2014
31. Introduction to the Neurobiology of Interval Timing
- Author
-
Victor de Lafuente and Hugo Merchant
- Subjects
Interval (music) ,Rhythm ,Conceptual framework ,Duration (music) ,Perception ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Active listening ,Time perception ,Psychology ,Variety (cybernetics) ,media_common ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
Time is a fundamental variable that organisms must quantify in order to survive. In humans, for example, the gradual development of the sense of duration and rhythm is an essential skill in many facets of social behavior such as speaking, dancing to-, listening to- or playing music, performing a wide variety of sports, and driving a car (Merchant H, Harrington DL, Meck WH. Annu Rev Neurosci. 36:313–36, 2013). During the last 10 years there has been a rapid growth of research on the neural underpinnings of timing in the subsecond and suprasecond scales, using a variety of methodological approaches in the human being, as well as in varied animal and theoretical models. In this introductory chapter we attempt to give a conceptual framework that defines time processing as a family of different phenomena. The brain circuits and neural underpinnings of temporal quantification seem to largely depend on its time scale and the sensorimotor nature of specific behaviors. Therefore, we describe the main time scales and their associated behaviors and show how the perception and execution of timing events in the subsecond and second scales may depend on similar or different neural mechanisms.
- Published
- 2014
32. Internal signal correlates neural populations and biases perceptual decision reports
- Author
-
Néstor Parga, Ranulfo Romo, Victor de Lafuente, and Federico Carnevale
- Subjects
Neurons ,Multidisciplinary ,Behavior, Animal ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Decision Making ,Sensory system ,Stimulus (physiology) ,Biological Sciences ,Somatosensory system ,Macaca mulatta ,Developmental psychology ,Frontal Lobe ,Perceptual decision ,Frontal lobe ,Perception ,Animals ,Second-order stimulus ,Psychology ,Stimulus control ,Neuroscience ,media_common ,Signal Transduction - Abstract
In perceptual decision-making tasks the activity of neurons in frontal and posterior parietal cortices covaries more with perceptual reports than with the physical properties of stimuli. This relationship is revealed when subjects have to make behavioral choices about weak or uncertain stimuli. If knowledge about stimulus onset time is available, decision making can be based on accumulation of sensory evidence. However, the time of stimulus onset or even its very presence is often ambiguous. By analyzing firing rates and correlated variability of frontal lobe neurons while monkeys perform a vibrotactile detection task, we show that behavioral outcomes are crucially affected by the state of cortical networks before stimulus onset times. The results suggest that sensory detection is partly due to a purely internal signal whereas the stimulus, if finally applied, adds a contribution to this initial processing later on. The probability to detect or miss the stimulus can thus be explained as the combined effect of this variable internal signal and the sensory evidence.
- Published
- 2012
33. Functional impact of interneuronal inhibition in the cerebral cortex of behaving animals
- Author
-
Fernando Peña-Ortega, Hugo Merchant, Victor de Lafuente, and Jorge Larriva-Sahd
- Subjects
Cerebral Cortex ,Behavior, Animal ,General Neuroscience ,Neural Inhibition ,Action Potentials ,Sensory system ,Gating ,Stimulus (physiology) ,Inhibitory postsynaptic potential ,Electrophysiology ,medicine.anatomical_structure ,Neurochemical ,Cerebral cortex ,Interneurons ,medicine ,Animals ,Psychology ,Neuroscience - Abstract
This paper reviews recent progress in understanding the functional roles of inhibitory interneurons in behaving animals and how they affect information processing in cortical microcircuits. Multiple studies have shown that the morphological subtypes of inhibitory cells show distinct electrophysiological properties, as well as different molecular and neurochemical identities, providing a large mosaic of inhibitory mechanisms for the dynamic processing of information in the cortex. However, it is only recently that some specific functions of different interneuronal subtypes have been described in behaving animals. In this regard, influential results have been obtained using the known differences of interneurons and pyramidal cells recorded extracellularly to dissociate the functional roles that these two classes of neurons may play in the cortical microcircuits during various behaviors. Neurons can be segregated into fast-spiking (FS) cells that show short action potentials, high discharge rates, and correspond to putative interneurons; and regular-spiking (RS) cells that show larger action potentials and correspond to pyramidal neurons. Using this classification strategy, it has been found that cortical inhibition is involved in sculpting the tuning to different stimulus or behavioral features across a wide variety of sensory, association, and motor areas. Recent studies have suggested that the increase in high-frequency synchronization during information processing and spatial attention may be mediated by FS activation. Finally, FS are active during motor planning and movement execution in different motor areas, supporting the notion that inhibitory interneurons are involved in shaping the motor command but not in gating the cortical output.
- Published
- 2012
34. Sense, memory, and decision-making in the somatosensory cortical network
- Author
-
Victor de Lafuente, Ranulfo Romo, and Luis Lemus
- Subjects
Categorical perception ,Visual perception ,General Neuroscience ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Decision Making ,Models, Neurological ,Sensory system ,Somatosensory Cortex ,Somatosensory system ,Stimulus modality ,Cognition ,Categorization ,Memory ,Mental process ,Perception ,Animals ,Humans ,Psychology ,Neuroscience ,media_common ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
The brain constructs representations of objects and concepts based in sensory information combined with experience. This mental process, that we call perception, is the result of a chain of events consisting of phenomena such as detection, memory, discrimination, categorization and decision-making. Although the phenomenon of perception is not necessarily dependent on a given sensory modality (e.g. visual perception, auditory, tactile), single sensory models are indispensable for studying the neural mechanisms that generate it. The somatosensory system is a suitable model for studying the manner in which presentation of a single physical variable (e.g. vibration) triggers a perceptual process. Here, we discuss some recent studies in the somatosensory system that in our view, constitute a breakthrough to understanding decision making.
- Published
- 2012
35. Conversion of sensory signals into perceptual decisions
- Author
-
Victor de Lafuente and Ranulfo Romo
- Subjects
genetic structures ,Working memory ,General Neuroscience ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Sensory memory ,Decision Making ,Parietal lobe ,Sensory maps and brain development ,Sensory system ,Somatosensory Cortex ,Sensory neuroscience ,Sensory substitution ,Touch Perception ,Memory ,Perception ,Animals ,Humans ,Psychology ,Neuroscience ,media_common ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
A fundamental problem in neurobiology is to understand how brain circuits represent sensory information and how such representations give rise to perception, memory and decision-making. We demonstrate that a sensory stimulus engages multiple areas of the cerebral cortex, including primary sensory, prefrontal, premotor and motor cortices. As information transverses the cortical circuits it shows progressively more relation to perception, memory and decision reports. In particular, we show how somatosensory areas on the parietal lobe generate a parameterized representation of a tactile stimulus. This representation is maintained in working memory by prefrontal and premotor areas of the frontal lobe. The presentation of a second stimulus, that monkeys are trained to compare with the first, generates decision-related activity reflecting which stimulus had the higher frequency. Importantly, decision-related activity is observed across several cortical circuits including prefrontal, premotor and parietal cortices. Sensory information is encoded by neuronal populations with opposite tuning, and suggests that a simple subtraction operation could be the underlying mechanism by which past and present sensory information is compared to generate perceptual decisions.
- Published
- 2011
36. Dopamine neurons code subjective sensory experience and uncertainty of perceptual decisions
- Author
-
Victor de Lafuente and Ranulfo Romo
- Subjects
media_common.quotation_subject ,Action Potentials ,Sensory system ,Stimulus (physiology) ,Somatosensory system ,Midbrain ,Reward ,Dopamine ,Mesencephalon ,Perception ,medicine ,Animals ,Sensory cue ,media_common ,Communication ,Multidisciplinary ,Behavior, Animal ,business.industry ,Dopaminergic Neurons ,Uncertainty ,Vibration amplitude ,Biological Sciences ,Macaca mulatta ,Electric Stimulation ,Cues ,Psychology ,business ,Neuroscience ,medicine.drug - Abstract
Midbrain dopamine (DA) neurons respond to sensory stimuli associated with future rewards. When reward is delivered probabilistically, DA neurons reflect this uncertainty by increasing their firing rates in a period between the sensory cue and reward delivery time. Probability of reward, however, has been externally conveyed by visual cues, and it is not known whether DA neurons would signal uncertainty arising internally. Here we show that DA neurons code the uncertainty associated with a perceptual judgment about the presence or absence of a vibrotactile stimulus. We observed that uncertainty modulates the activity elicited by a go cue instructing monkey subjects to communicate their decisions. That is, the same go cue generates different DA responses depending on the uncertainty level of a judgment made a few seconds before the go instruction. Easily detected suprathreshold stimuli elicit small DA responses, indicating that future reward will not be a surprising event. In contrast, the absence of a sensory stimulus generates large DA responses associated with uncertainty: was the stimulus truly absent, or did a low-amplitude vibration go undetected? In addition, the responses of DA neurons to the stimulus itself increase with vibration amplitude, but only when monkeys correctly detect its presence. This finding suggests that DA activity is not related to actual intensity but rather to perceived intensity. Therefore, in addition to their well-known role in reward prediction, DA neurons code subjective sensory experience and uncertainty arising internally from perceptual decisions.
- Published
- 2011
37. Flexible decisions and chess expertise
- Author
-
Victor de Lafuente
- Subjects
Speed-accuracy tradeoff ,business.industry ,Computer science ,General Commentary ,General Neuroscience ,Decision Making ,Data science ,lcsh:RC321-571 ,Text mining ,Decision model ,Chess ,business ,lcsh:Neurosciences. Biological psychiatry. Neuropsychiatry ,Neuroscience - Published
- 2010
38. Biomimetic brain machine interfaces for the control of movement
- Author
-
Andrew H. Fagg, James M. Rebesco, Jake Reimer, Ranulfo Romo, Victor de Lafuente, Dennis Tkach, Karen A. Moxon, Nicholas G. Hatsopoulos, Sara A. Solla, Eric A. Pohlmeyer, Lee E. Miller, and Shamim Nemati
- Subjects
Focus (computing) ,Computer science ,General Neuroscience ,Movement ,Work (physics) ,Models, Neurological ,Representation (systemics) ,Brain ,Somatosensory system ,Signal ,Article ,User-Computer Interface ,Nonlinear Dynamics ,Artificial Intelligence ,Biomimetics ,Sensation ,Animals ,Humans ,Control (linguistics) ,Neuroscience ,Man-Machine Systems ,Brain–computer interface - Abstract
Quite recently, it has become possible to use signals recorded simultaneously from large numbers of cortical neurons for real-time control. Such brain machine interfaces (BMIs) have allowed animal subjects and human patients to control the position of a computer cursor or robotic limb under the guidance of visual feedback. Although impressive, such approaches essentially ignore the dynamics of the musculoskeletal system, and they lack potentially critical somatosensory feedback. In this mini-symposium, we will initiate a discussion of systems that more nearly mimic the control of natural limb movement. The work that we will describe is based on fundamental observations of sensorimotor physiology that have inspired novel BMI approaches. We will focus on what we consider to be three of the most important new directions for BMI development related to the control of movement. (1) We will present alternative methods for building decoders, including structured, nonlinear models, the explicit incorporation of limb state information, and novel approaches to the development of decoders for paralyzed subjects unable to generate an output signal. (2) We will describe the real-time prediction of dynamical signals, including joint torque, force, and EMG, and the real-time control of physical plants with dynamics like that of the real limb. (3) We will discuss critical factors that must be considered to incorporate somatosensory feedback to the BMI user, including its potential benefits, the differing representations of sensation and perception across cortical areas, and the changes in the cortical representation of tactile events after spinal injury.
- Published
- 2007
39. Decoding the temporal evolution of a simple perceptual act
- Author
-
Ranulfo, Romo, Adrián, Hernández, Antonio, Zainos, Luis, Lemus, Victor, de Lafuente, Rogelio, Luna, and Verónica, Nacher
- Subjects
Cerebral Cortex ,Neurons ,Discrimination, Psychological ,Memory ,Decision Making ,Animals ,Perception ,Motor Activity ,Biological Evolution - Abstract
Most perceptual tasks require sequential steps to be carried out. This must be the case, for example, when subjects discriminate the difference in frequency between two mechanical vibrations applied sequentially to their fingertips. This perceptual task can be understood as a chain of neural operations: encoding the two consecutive stimulus frequencies, maintaining the first stimulus in working memory, comparing the second stimulus to the memory trace left by the first stimulus, and communicating the result of the comparison to the motor apparatus. Where and how in the brain are these cognitive operations executed? We addressed this problem by recording single neurons from several cortical areas while trained monkeys executed the vibrotactile discrimination task. We found that primary somatosensory cortex (S1) drives higher cortical areas where past and current sensory information are combined, such that a comparison of the two evolves into a decision. Consistent with this result, direct activation of the S1 can trigger quantifiable percepts in this task. These findings provide a fairly complete panorama of the neural dynamics that underlies the transformation of sensory information into an action and emphasize the importance of studying multiple cortical areas during the same behavioural task.
- Published
- 2006
40. Behavioral Basis of Focal Hand Dystonia
- Author
-
Jon H. Kaas, Nancy Byl, Raymond Hermer-Vazquez, Ford F. Ebner, Linda Hermer-Vazquez, Sathian, Adri√°n Hern√°ndez, Ranulfo Romo, Victor de Lafuente, Christopher I. Moore, Miriam Schneider, Michael Brecht, Elena Zoubina, C. Gerloff, Mathew E. Diamond, Friedhelm C. Hummel, Rogelio Luna, Mark L. Andermann, Leonardo G. Cohen, Randolph J. Nudo, Ian D. Manns, Luis Lemus, Michael Armstrong-James, Antonio Zainos, John K. Chapin, and Scott Barbay
- Subjects
Somatosensory evoked potential ,Focal Hand Dystonia ,Somatosensory system ,Psychology ,Neuroscience - Published
- 2005
41. The orientation dependence of the Hermann grid illusion
- Author
-
Victor de Lafuente and Octavio Ruiz
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,Adolescent ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Models, Neurological ,Illusion ,Grid illusion ,Luminance ,Contrast Sensitivity ,Optics ,Orientation (geometry) ,Orientation ,Humans ,Anisotropy ,Lighting ,media_common ,Observer Variation ,business.industry ,Optical illusion ,General Neuroscience ,Oblique case ,Illusions ,Pattern Recognition, Visual ,Space Perception ,Oblique effect ,Female ,Cues ,Visual Fields ,Psychology ,business ,Photic Stimulation - Abstract
The Hermann grid illusion (HGI), elicited by a grid displayed as either horizontal-vertical (HV) or oblique (45 degrees ) configuration, was measured as the luminance necessary to cancel the illusory spots at the grid intersections. Overall, the HGI produced by the oblique grid was about one-third of that produced by the HV grid. The observers exhibited different sensitivities to the HGI orientation, and seemed to perceive the illusion in two manners: with moderate anisotropy (reduction of about 20%, three subjects) or large anisotropy (90% reduction, four subjects). The quantitative reduction of the HGI elicited by the oblique pattern tested and its reduction to almost zero in some subjects, constitute a benchmark for any model aimed at explaining the HGI on psychophysical grounds.
- Published
- 2003
42. Decisions arising from opposing views
- Author
-
Ranulfo Romo and Victor de Lafuente
- Subjects
General Neuroscience ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Eye movement ,Sensory system ,Stimulus (physiology) ,Gaze ,medicine.anatomical_structure ,Perception ,Visual Objects ,medicine ,Microstimulation ,Neuron ,Psychology ,Neuroscience ,computer ,media_common ,computer.programming_language - Abstract
Imagine sitting next to a window in a restaurant atop a high building. You see people moving in the plaza below, and you instinctively direct your gaze in the direc- tion that most people are moving. This effortless action—identifying the net direc- tion of motion of a more-or-less randomly moving group of objects—involves cortical neurons from the middle temporal area (MT) of the brain. These neurons activate in response to visual objects moving in a par- ticular direction and inform the brain that an object is in motion 1 ,b ut understanding how this neuronal representation is used to causal relationships between neuronal fir- ing patterns and perception. Because MT neurons are very selective about the physi- cal attributes of the stimuli that activate them, the researchers first determined the appropriate combination of size, location, velocity and direction of the moving dots that optimally activated the neurons in the vicinity of the recording electrode. Once the best activating stimulus was found, the monkeys' task was designed such that the dots moved in the direction preferred by the neurons near the recording electrode in half of the trials, and in the opposite direc- tion in the other half. For example, if the recording electrode was near a group of neurons with upward direction preference, the stimulus set was designed such that monkeys had to decide whether the net motion of the dots was upward or down- ward. Thanks to this design, the authors could artificially activate neurons while monkeys performed the direction discrimi- nation task and add motion information that was either congruent or opposite to the direction of the moving dots. They could then evaluate how the addition of either type of information affected the monkeys' choice behavior. How this artificial addition of information affected the monkeys' behavior depended on the direction of the dots being presented with respect to the microstimulated neuron popu- lations. If the dots were moving in the direc- tion preferred by the microstimulated neurons, the authors found that the monkeys were substantially more likely to choose to make an eye movement in that direction. Additionally, the stimulus-viewing intervals (reaction times) were faster. This suggested that microstimulation was adding informa- tion to the stimulus, biasing the monkeys' behavioral responses in the direction pre- ferred by the stimulated neurons and causing the monkeys to reach faster decisions, pre- sumably due to a quicker accumulation of sensory evidence. These results elegantly confirmed the causal relationship between the activity of MT neurons and the percep- tion of motion direction 4 .
- Published
- 2003
43. Códigos neurales para la percepción
- Author
-
Victor de Lafuente Flores, Emilio Salinas Fernández, Luis Lemus Sandoval, Rogelio Luna Almeida, Adrián Hernández Alva, Ranulfo Romo Trujillo, and Antonio Zainos Rosales
- Subjects
Neural activity ,Motor area ,Frontal lobe ,Perception ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Microstimulation ,Neurology (clinical) ,General Medicine ,Tactile perception ,Psychology ,Somatosensory system ,Cognitive psychology ,media_common - Abstract
Objective. This article describes experiments designed to show the neural codes associated with the perception and processing of tactile information. Development The results of these experiments have shown the neural activity correlated with tactile perception. The neurones of the primary somatosensory cortex (S1) represent the physical attributes of tactile perception. We found that these representations correlated with tactile perception. By means ofintracortical microstimulation we demonstrated the causal relationship between S1 activity and tactile perception. In the motor areas of the frontal lobe is to be found the connection between sensorial and motor representation whilst decisions are being taken. Conclusions. S1 generates neural representations of thesomatosensorystimuli which seen to be sufficient for tactile perception. These neural representations aresubsequently processed by central areas to S1 and seem useful in perception, memory and decision-making.
- Published
- 2002
44. An Optimal Decision Population Code that Accounts for Correlated Variability Unambiguously Predicts a Subject’s Choice
- Author
-
Victor de Lafuente, Néstor Parga, Federico Carnevale, and Ranulfo Romo
- Subjects
Neuroscience(all) ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Decision Making ,Models, Neurological ,Population ,Action Potentials ,Stimulus (physiology) ,Somatosensory system ,Perception ,Animals ,education ,media_common ,Neurons ,Communication ,education.field_of_study ,business.industry ,General Neuroscience ,Motor Cortex ,Somatosensory Cortex ,Cortical neurons ,Macaca mulatta ,Population code ,Decision variables ,Touch Perception ,nervous system ,Psychology ,business ,Neuroscience ,Psychomotor Performance ,Optimal decision - Abstract
Summary Decisions emerge from the concerted activity of neuronal populations distributed across brain circuits. However, the analytical tools best suited to decode decision signals from neuronal populations remain unknown. Here we show that knowledge of correlated variability between pairs of cortical neurons allows perfect decoding of decisions from population firing rates. We recorded pairs of neurons from secondary somatosensory (S2) and premotor (PM) cortices while monkeys reported the presence or absence of a tactile stimulus. We found that while populations of S2 and sensory-like PM neurons are only partially correlated with behavior, those PM neurons active during a delay period preceding the motor report predict unequivocally the animal's decision report. Thus, a population rate code that optimally reveals a subject's perceptual decisions can be implemented just by knowing the correlations of PM neurons representing decision variables.
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
45. Language Abilities of Motor Cortex
- Author
-
Ranulfo Romo and Victor de Lafuente
- Subjects
General Neuroscience ,Neuroscience(all) ,Motor Cortex ,Semantics ,Magnetic Resonance Imaging ,medicine.anatomical_structure ,Cortical network ,medicine ,Speech Perception ,Humans ,Neuron ,Nerve Net ,Psychology ,Cognitive psychology ,Meaning (linguistics) ,Motor cortex ,Language - Abstract
A new exploration of the cortical network underlying our language abilities by Hauk et al., in this issue of Neuron, shows that the process of giving meaning to words differentially activates the motor cortex according to the semantic category of the word.
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
46. How Confident Do You Feel?
- Author
-
Victor de Lafuente and Ranulfo Romo
- Subjects
Male ,General Neuroscience ,Neuroscience(all) ,Decision Making ,Motion Perception ,Sensory system ,Article ,medicine.anatomical_structure ,Visual cortex ,nervous system ,medicine ,Motion direction ,Microstimulation ,Animals ,Neuron ,Psychology ,Neuroscience ,Visual Cortex - Abstract
Decisions are often associated with a degree of certainty, or confidence — an estimate of the probability that the chosen option will be correct. Recent neurophysiological results suggest that the central processing of evidence leading to a perceptual decision also establishes a level of confidence. Here we provide a causal test of this hypothesis by electrically stimulating areas of the visual cortex involved in motion perception. Monkeys discriminated the direction of motion in a noisy display, and were sometimes allowed to opt out of the direction choice if their confidence was low. Microstimulation did not reduce overall confidence in the decision but instead altered confidence in a manner that mimicked a change in visual motion, plus a small increase in sensory noise. The results suggest that the same sensory neural signals support choice, reaction time and confidence in a decision, and that artificial manipulation of these signals preserves the quantitative relationship between accumulated evidence and confidence.
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
47. Probing the cortical evidence of somatosensory discrimination
- Author
-
Romo, R., Hernández, A., Zainos, A., Lemus, L., Victor de Lafuente, and Luna, R.
48. A Hidden Sensory Function for Motor Cortex
- Author
-
Ranulfo Romo and Victor de Lafuente
- Subjects
genetic structures ,Movement ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Neuroscience(all) ,Posterior parietal cortex ,Sensory system ,Vibration ,behavioral disciplines and activities ,Magnetics ,Perception ,medicine ,Humans ,Kinesthesis ,media_common ,Neurons ,General Neuroscience ,Motor Cortex ,Evoked Potentials, Motor ,Hand ,Illusions ,Magnetic Resonance Imaging ,Sensory function ,medicine.anatomical_structure ,Neurocomputational speech processing ,Neuron ,sense organs ,Primary motor cortex ,Psychology ,Neuroscience ,psychological phenomena and processes ,Motor cortex - Abstract
Sensory perception has traditionally been attributed to the activation of sensory cortices. However, by inducing an illusory perception of movement, Naito and colleagues show in this issue of Neuron that the illusory perception of movement is related to activation of primary motor cortex.
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
49. The role of neural correlations in a decision-making task
- Author
-
Néstor Parga, Victor de Lafuente, Federico Carnevale, and Ranulfo Romo
- Subjects
education.field_of_study ,General Neuroscience ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Population ,Sensory system ,Stimulus (physiology) ,Biology ,Premotor cortex ,Correlation ,Cellular and Molecular Neuroscience ,medicine.anatomical_structure ,Perception ,medicine ,Oral Presentation ,Geometric mean ,education ,Neuroscience ,Decorrelation ,media_common - Abstract
Simultaneous recordings of the firing activity of pairs of cortical neurons have shown that spike-count correlation coefficients (CCs) cover a wide range of values. According to recent theoretical and experimental work [1,2] recurrent cortical networks decorrelate neural activity producing very low CCs. However little is known about the origin of correlations and data analysis based on recordings of cortical activity of awake, behaving animals performing non-trivial tasks are scarce. In this study, we aim to understand the role of neural correlations in perceptual decision-making tasks and its relationship with the covariation between neural firing activity and behavior. We examine spike-count correlations obtained from pairs of simultaneously recorded premotor cortex (PC) neurons while trained monkeys performed a vibrotactile detection task in which the stimulus was often absent or weak, and the time of its application was variable [3,4]. By analyzing firing rates and correlated variability we show that behavioral outcomes are crucially affected by the state of cortical networks before stimulus onset times. Our results suggest that sensory detection is partly due to a purely internal signal whereas the stimulus, if finally applied, adds a contribution to this initial processing later on [5]. Noise correlations can be weak; their smallest values are attained at the end of the delay period of the task. Importantly, we found that small CCs are compatible with high firing rates. Although the firing rate in hit trials is higher than in correct rejections, the distributions of CCs over the population of pairs are similar, presenting mean values of 0.06. Moreover, the CCs do not covary with the geometrical mean of the firing rate of the pair. The observation that single neurons covary with the subject's response (characterized by the choice probability index, CP) is usually explained by the existence of variability correlations among the cells in a neuronal population [6]. Here we show a simple approximate expression that explicitly relates the population-averaged CP index and the CCs. This expression shows that the CP index is different from 0.5 when CCs evaluated using all trials differ from choice-conditioned correlations. Neurons could covary significantly with behavior even if the latter are very small. Thus, we show that there is no contradiction between the correlated activity required for the non-chance CP index and the small correlations produced by decorrelation in recurrent networks. Although the CP index is useful to study the role of single neurons in decision-making tasks, in reality, the decision is formed through the coordinated action of several pools of neurons. Hence, the relevant quantities to investigate the elaboration of the choice are population variables combining the activity of several pools. By extending the notion of CP from single neurons to neural pools, we defined the CPN index to quantify the amount of covariation with behavior of arbitrary linear combinations of oppositely-tuned neural pools. We found that pools of PC neurons exhibiting persistent activity become fully correlated with the subject's choice soon after stimulus onset and during the entire delay period of the task.
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
50. The dopamine signal in decision-making tasks with stimulus and timing uncertainty
- Author
-
Victor de Lafuente, Néstor Parga, Ranulfo Romo, and Stefania Sarno
- Subjects
Computer science ,General Neuroscience ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Bayesian probability ,Sensory system ,Stimulus (physiology) ,Bayesian inference ,Cellular and Molecular Neuroscience ,Perception ,Poster Presentation ,Reinforcement learning ,Operant conditioning ,False alarm ,Neuroscience ,Cognitive psychology ,media_common - Abstract
It was long suggested that the phasic activity of midbrain dopamine (DA) neurons codes the subject’s error in the prediction of reward [1]. Most of the experimental work in this field was done in the context of classical and instrumental conditioning. Recently, there has been an increasing interest to study the activity of DA neurons while the subject performs a decision-making task with the goal of obtaining reward at the end of the trial [2,3]. In an experiment in which monkeys have to decide about the presence or absence of a somatosensory stimulus, recordings of DA neurons have shown that the activity of these cells is modulated according to the trial type. The averaged activity in hit, miss, false alarm or correct rejection trials each presents a distinct temporal profile [3]. In particular, the neurons’ response to the go cue is correlated with the subject’s uncertainty about his choice. The signal of dopamine neurons in classical and instrumental conditioning has been explained in terms of the temporal-difference (TD) algorithm. However it is not clear whether and, if so, how reinforcement learning can account for the dopamine signals in complex decision-making tasks with noisy sensory information and temporal uncertainty of the relevant task events, as is the case in the detection task mentioned above [4]. We have developed an actor-critic model which deals with both these aspects of the problem. While an internal temporal representation keeps track of past relevant events, partial observability is accounted for by means of a Bayesian approach. The dopamine phasic activity predicted by the model matches the experimental data and the prediction of the psychometric curve is consistent with the animal performance. Furthermore, the model provides an interpretation of the condition-dependent dopamine response to the go cue instruction in terms of reward prediction error. Using Bayesian inference the model constructs an internal belief about the presence of the somatosensory stimulus. This belief reflects the confidence about the sensory perception and thus the value assigned to this perceptual judgment. The large belief in stimulus-present decisions represents a high degree of confidence in the sensory perception and a great expectation for future rewards. On the contrary, stimulus-absent choices reflect a small belief and consequently a larger uncertainty about the decision and the future reward. This computational description of belief agrees with the previous interpretation of the data [3] and describes well other experimental observations such as the dependence of DA neurons' signals on the stimulus amplitude. The model also predicts a decrease in dopamine activity before the go cue instruction, which is also observed in the data. We explain this decreasing tonic activity as an effect of the timing uncertainty. This is partly due to the task structure and partly generated from the limited temporal resolution of the stimulus representation, which creates subjective variability in the timing of events.
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.