215 results on '"Margot Prior"'
Search Results
2. Problems with mathematics
- Author
-
Margot Prior
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
3. Diagnosis and assessment: Methods and measures
- Author
-
Margot Prior
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
4. Remedial techniques and resources
- Author
-
Margot Prior
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
5. Understanding Specific Learning Difficulties
- Author
-
Margot Prior
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
6. The relationship between behaviour problems and learning difficulties
- Author
-
Margot Prior
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
7. Recapitulation
- Author
-
Margot Prior
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
8. Neuropsychology and its uses in understanding learning difficulties
- Author
-
Margot Prior
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
9. Intervention
- Author
-
Margot Prior
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
10. Reading and spelling problems
- Author
-
Margot Prior
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
11. Social mediators of relationships between childhood reading difficulties, behaviour problems, and secondary school noncompletion
- Author
-
Diana Smart, Ann Sanson, Craig A. Olsson, Margot Prior, George J. Youssef, and John W. Toumbourou
- Subjects
050103 clinical psychology ,Mediation (statistics) ,Longitudinal study ,media_common.quotation_subject ,education ,05 social sciences ,050109 social psychology ,Student engagement ,Peer attachment ,Developmental psychology ,Reading (process) ,Intervention (counseling) ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Temperament ,School engagement ,Psychology ,General Psychology ,media_common - Abstract
Background: Childhood reading difficulties (RDs) and behaviour problems (BPs) are well-established predictors of secondary school noncompletion. However, few studies have examined the risk of secondary school noncompletion for children with differing combinations of the two childhood problems. Even less is known about potential mediators of relationships between the two childhood problems and secondary school noncompletion. Aims: This study examined whether key indicators of social connectedness—adolescent school engagement, and family and peer attachment—mediated relationships between childhood RDs and BPs and secondary school noncompletion. Sample: Data were drawn from the Australian Temperament Project, a longitudinal study that has followed a large community sample across 16 waves from infancy to adulthood. Methods: Using standard reading and BP assessments, four childhood exposure groups were identified at 7–8 years: RDs-only, BPs-only, RDs + BPs, and Neither Problem. Results: Both groups with prior BPs had significantly poorer early-adolescent school engagement, and family and peer attachment than the RDs-only and Neither groups. School engagement mediated the relationship between childhood BPs and secondary school noncompletion but there was no evidence of similar mediation by family or peer attachment. Adolescent social connections did not mediate the effects of childhood RDs. Conclusions: The adverse long-term consequences of childhood BPs on secondary school noncompletion may be alleviated by targeting adolescent school engagement. Intervention implications are discussed.
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
12. Language and social-emotional and behavioural wellbeing from 4 to 7 years: a community-based study
- Author
-
Cristina McKean, Emma Sciberras, Penny Levickis, Laura Conway, Sheena Reilly, Angela Pezic, Fiona Mensah, Lesley Bretherton, Edith L. Bavin, Margot Prior, and Patricia Eadie
- Subjects
Male ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Longitudinal study ,Emotions ,Community based study ,Developmental psychology ,Social-emotional problems ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,030225 pediatrics ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,Child and adolescent psychiatry ,medicine ,Social emotional learning ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Language disorder ,Longitudinal Studies ,Behavioural problems ,Child ,Language ,Mental Disorders ,05 social sciences ,Confounding ,General Medicine ,Original Contribution ,medicine.disease ,Mental health ,Childhood ,Psychiatry and Mental health ,Language development ,Mental Health ,Child, Preschool ,Pediatrics, Perinatology and Child Health ,Female ,Psychology ,050104 developmental & child psychology - Abstract
Language disorder (LD) and social-emotional and behavioural (SEB) difficulties are common childhood problems that often co-occur. While there is clear evidence of these associations from clinical samples, less is known about community samples. This paper examines these associations in children aged 4-7 years from a community-based longitudinal study. 771 families provided questionnaire and assessment data at 4, 5 and 7 years. Parent-reported SEB difficulties were measured at each point (SDQ). Child language was directly assessed at 4 (CELF-P2), 5 and 7 years (CELF-4). Linear regression analysis was used to compare cross-sectional differences in mean SDQ scores between children with and without LD at each time point. Linear regression was then used to examine how patterns of language development (language disordered at three time points; never disordered; disordered at one or two time points, i.e. 'unstable' group) related to SEB difficulties at each age, adjusted for potential confounders, as in the previous analyses. Higher hyperactivity/inattention scores were associated with LD at each age. In fully adjusted models, there was little difference in mean emotional symptoms scores between children with and without LD. The 'never' LD group had lower mean SDQ scores at each time point than the 'unstable' group. Findings highlight that children with persistent LD from preschool to early primary school may be more likely to have concomitant SEB difficulties, particularly behavioural difficulties. Those with unstable LD may also have co-occurring SEB difficulties, showing a need for education and health professionals to monitor early language and SEB development.
- Published
- 2017
13. Temperament in individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder: A systematic review
- Author
-
Margot Prior, Mirko Uljarević, Darren Hedley, Samuel P. Putnam, Lacey Chetcuti, Kristelle Hudry, Kate Ellis-Davies, Antonio Y. Hardan, and Andrew J. O. Whitehouse
- Subjects
050103 clinical psychology ,Surgency ,Adolescent ,Autism Spectrum Disorder ,media_common.quotation_subject ,MEDLINE ,Context (language use) ,behavioral disciplines and activities ,Negative affectivity ,03 medical and health sciences ,Typically developing ,0302 clinical medicine ,mental disorders ,medicine ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Child ,Temperament ,media_common ,Problem Behavior ,05 social sciences ,Infant ,medicine.disease ,030227 psychiatry ,Psychiatry and Mental health ,Clinical Psychology ,Autism spectrum disorder ,Autism ,Psychology ,Clinical psychology - Abstract
The study of temperament in Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) has the potential to provide insight regarding variability in the onset, nature, and course of both core and co-morbid symptoms. The aim of this systematic review was to integrate existing findings concerning temperament in the context of ASD. Searches of Medline, PsychInfo and Scopus databases identified 64 relevant studies. As a group, children and adolescents with ASD appear to be temperamentally different from both typically developing and other clinical non-ASD groups, characterized by higher negative affectivity, lower surgency, and lower effortful control at a higher-order level. Consistent with research on typically developing children, correlational findings and emerging longitudinal evidence suggests that lower effortful control and higher negative affect are associated with increased internalizing and externalizing problems in ASD samples. Longitudinal studies suggest there may be temperamental differences between high familial risk infants who do and do not develop ASD from as early as 6-months of age. Limitations of existing research are highlighted, and possible directions for future research to capitalize on the potential afforded through the study of temperament in relation to ASD are discussed.
- Published
- 2019
14. Parent-reported patterns of loss and gain in communication in 1- to 2-year-old children are not unique to autism spectrum disorder
- Author
-
Patricia Eadie, Amanda Brignell, Angela T Morgan, Edith L. Bavin, Katrina Williams, Susan Donath, Margot Prior, and Sheena Reilly
- Subjects
Male ,Parents ,medicine.medical_specialty ,autism spectrum disorder ,skill loss ,Audiology ,Language Development ,Vocabulary ,Developmental psychology ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,medicine ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Early language ,language ,Intelligence quotient ,communication ,05 social sciences ,Infant ,Language impairment ,Language acquisition ,medicine.disease ,Language development ,Autism spectrum disorder ,Child, Preschool ,trajectory ,Autism ,Female ,regression ,Psychology ,030217 neurology & neurosurgery ,050104 developmental & child psychology ,Gesture - Abstract
We compared loss and gain in communication from 1 to 2 years in children later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder (n = 41), language impairment (n = 110) and in children with typical language development at 7 years (n = 831). Participants were selected from a prospective population cohort study of child language (the Early Language in Victoria Study). Parent-completed communication tools were used. As a group, children with autism spectrum disorder demonstrated slower median skill gain, with an increasing gap between trajectories compared to children with typical development and language impairment. A proportion from all groups lost skills in at least one domain (autism spectrum disorder (41%), language impairment (30%), typical development (26%)), with more children with autism spectrum disorder losing skills in more than one domain (autism spectrum disorder (47%), language impairment (15%, p = 0.0003), typical development (16%, p
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
15. Patterns and Predictors of Language Development from 4 to 7 Years in Verbal Children With and Without Autism Spectrum Disorder
- Author
-
Katrina Williams, Amanda Brignell, Sheena Reilly, Margot Prior, Kim Jachno, and Angela T Morgan
- Subjects
Male ,Parents ,longitudinal ,Autism Spectrum Disorder ,growth ,Interpersonal communication ,outcomes ,behavioral disciplines and activities ,Language Development ,Social Skills ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,Social skills ,Predictive Value of Tests ,030225 pediatrics ,mental disorders ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,medicine ,follow-up ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Language Development Disorders ,Longitudinal Studies ,Prospective Studies ,Child ,development ,language ,Language Tests ,Intelligence quotient ,communication ,05 social sciences ,typical development ,Wechsler Scales ,Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale ,language impairment ,medicine.disease ,Language acquisition ,Language development ,predictors ,Autism spectrum disorder ,trajectory ,Child, Preschool ,Autism ,Female ,Psychology ,050104 developmental & child psychology ,Clinical psychology - Abstract
This study used a prospective community-based sample to describe patterns and predictors of language development from 4 to 7 years in verbal children (IQ ≥ 70) with autism spectrum disorder (ASD; n = 26-27). Children with typical language (TD; n = 858-861) and language impairment (LI; n = 119) were used for comparison. Children with ASD and LI had similar mean language scores that were lower on average than children with TD. Similar proportions across all groups had declining, increasing and stable patterns. Language progressed at a similar rate for all groups, with progress influenced by IQ and language ability at 4 years rather than social communication skills or diagnosis of ASD. These findings inform advice for parents about language prognosis in ASD.
- Published
- 2018
16. Assessing early communication skills at 12 months: a retrospective study of Autism Spectrum Disorder
- Author
-
Margot Prior, Patricia Eadie, Sheena Reilly, and Nathaniel Swain
- Subjects
Linguistics and Language ,Rehabilitation ,medicine.medical_treatment ,Retrospective cohort study ,Peer group ,medicine.disease ,behavioral disciplines and activities ,Language and Linguistics ,Developmental psychology ,Speech and Hearing ,Autism spectrum disorder ,mental disorders ,medicine ,Autism ,Observational study ,Prospective cohort study ,Psychology ,Symbolic behavior ,Clinical psychology - Abstract
Background Early identification of Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) is currently limited by the absence of reliable biological markers for the disorder, as well as the reliability of screening and assessment tools for children aged between 6 and 18 months. Ongoing research has demonstrated the importance of early social communication skills in differentiating children later diagnosed with ASD from their typically developing (TD) peers, but researchers have not yet investigated whether these differences can be detected using community-ascertained systematic observation data as early as 12 months. Aims To investigate whether differences in early social communication skills can be detected at 12 months of age, comparing children later diagnosed with ASD, and TD peers; and to determine whether differences remain when groupings are based on age of subsequent ASD diagnosis. Methods & Procedures From a prospective community-ascertained sample, we collected data on children in early life, then conducted retrospective analyses for those children who were later diagnosed with ASD by the age of 7 years, compared with matched TD peers. We analysed standardized observational data of early communication skills, collected using the Communication and Symbolic Behavior Scales—Developmental Profile (CSBS-DP) Behavior Sample, when participants were 12 months of age. Outcomes & Results Children in the ASD group exhibited significantly lower social communication skills than the TD group, including on the Total score and Social and Symbolic Composite scores of the CSBS-DP Behavior Sample. Differences on the Total score and Social Composite were also detected for both early and late ASD diagnosis groups when compared with the TD group. Conclusions & Implications These findings give further support for the importance of social communication in assessing children at risk of ASD as early as 12 months of age. Future research could evaluate the sensitivity and specificity of direct observation of these early communication skills as diagnostic indicators for ASD at 12 months, and investigate whether it is possible to distinguish between ASD and other high-risk groups (e.g. developmental delay) at this age.
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
17. Developing a comprehensive model of risk and protective factors that can predict spelling at age seven: findings from a community sample of Victorian children
- Author
-
Angela Pezic, Lesley Bretherton, Sheena Reilly, Edith L. Bavin, Fiona Mensah, Tanya Serry, Margot Prior, Anne Castles, and Patricia Eadie
- Subjects
Vocabulary ,Social Psychology ,Intelligence quotient ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Regression analysis ,Sample (statistics) ,Spelling ,Education ,Developmental psychology ,Test (assessment) ,Phonological awareness ,Reading (process) ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,Psychology ,media_common - Abstract
The paper reports on a study designed to develop a risk model that can best predict single-word spelling in seven-year-old children when they were aged 4 and 5. Test measures, personal characteristics and environmental influences were all considered as variables from a community sample of 971 children. Strong concurrent correlations were found between single-word spelling and single-word reading while expressive language and, to a lesser extent, receptive language were less strongly correlated. Predictors of single-word spelling at ages 4 and 5 were dominated by child-related factors such as letter knowledge, a history of speech impairment and expressive language along with maternal word reading ability. Based on the strength of the predictive factors identified at both age 4 and 5, our results provide initial support for using a targeted model for screening pre-schoolers at risk of not mastering spelling in a timely manner.
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
18. Regression in autism spectrum disorders
- Author
-
Amanda Brignell, Margot Prior, Lawrence Bartak, Katrina Williams, and Jacqueline Roberts
- Subjects
medicine.medical_specialty ,genetic structures ,business.industry ,Disease progression ,medicine.disease ,behavioral disciplines and activities ,Child development ,Regression ,Developmental psychology ,mental disorders ,Pediatrics, Perinatology and Child Health ,Medicine ,Autism ,business ,Psychiatry ,Developmental regression - Abstract
Since the Journal of Paediatrics and Child Health was first published, there has been substantial change in the field of autism spectrum disorders (ASDs) with an exponential increase in the amount of funded and published research. In this paper, we focus on regression in children with ASD, a phenomenon that remains poorly understood. We discuss the implications of what we know about regression in ASD for the way we think about ASD more broadly and for paediatric practice.
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
19. Music therapy with hospitalized infants-the art and science of communicative musicality
- Author
-
Helen Shoemark, Campbell Paul, Stephen Malloch, Denis K Burnham, Margot Prior, Rudi Črnčec, Carol Newnham, and Sean W Coward
- Subjects
Pediatrics ,medicine.medical_specialty ,education.field_of_study ,Neonatal intensive care unit ,Music therapy ,business.industry ,Population ,Social engagement ,Mental health ,Social relation ,Psychiatry and Mental health ,Distress ,Intervention (counseling) ,Pediatrics, Perinatology and Child Health ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,medicine ,Psychiatry ,education ,business - Abstract
Infants seek contingent, companionable interactions with others. Infants in a Neonatal Intensive Care Unit (NICU), while receiving care that optimizes their chances of survival, often do not have the kind of interactions that are optimal for their social development. Live music therapy (MT) with infants is an intervention that aims for contingent, social interaction between therapist and infant. This study, with a limited numbers of infants, examined the effectiveness of an MT intervention in the NICU at The Royal Children's Hospital Melbourne. Two groups of late pre-term and full-term infants were recruited to the study; one was given MT and the other was not. A healthy group of infants not given MT served as an additional control. The effect of MT was indexed using two measures reflecting infant social engagement: the Neurobehavioral Assessment of the Preterm Infant (NAPI) and the Alarm Distress Baby Scale (ADBB). Results suggest that the MT intervention used at The Royal Children's Hospital Melbourne supports infants' neurobehavioral development. In particular, hospitalized infants who received MT were better able to maintain self-regulation during social interaction with an adult, were less irritable and cried less, and were more positive in their response to adult handling, when compared with infants who did not receive the intervention. These are important prerequisites for social interaction and development. Further and larger scale research using MT with this population is indicated.
- Published
- 2017
20. Severity of Autism is Related to Children's Language Processing
- Author
-
Cheryl Dissanayake, Evan Kidd, Emma Baker, Margot Prior, Edith L. Bavin, and Luke A. Prendergast
- Subjects
medicine.medical_specialty ,genetic structures ,General Neuroscience ,Significant group ,Eye movement ,Lexical access ,Audiology ,Fixation (psychology) ,medicine.disease ,behavioral disciplines and activities ,Developmental psychology ,Nonverbal communication ,Autism spectrum disorder ,mental disorders ,medicine ,Autism ,Eye tracking ,Neurology (clinical) ,Psychology ,Genetics (clinical) - Abstract
Problems in language processing have been associated with autism spectrum disorder (ASD), with some research attributing the problems to overall language skills rather than a diagnosis of ASD. Lexical access was assessed in a looking-while-listening task in three groups of 5- to 7-year-old children; two had high-functioning ASD (HFA), an ASD severe (ASD-S) group (n = 16) and an ASD moderate (ASD-M) group (n = 21). The third group were typically developing (TD) (n = 48). Participants heard sentences of the form “Where's the x?” and their eye movements to targets (e.g., train), phonological competitors (e.g., tree), and distractors were recorded. Proportions of looking time at target were analyzed within 200 ms intervals. Significant group differences were found between the ASD-S and TD groups only, at time intervals 1000–1200 and 1200–1400 ms postonset. The TD group was more likely to be fixated on target. These differences were maintained after adjusting for language, verbal and nonverbal IQ, and attention scores. An analysis using parent report of autistic-like behaviors showed higher scores to be associated with lower proportions of looking time at target, regardless of group. Further analysis showed fixation for the TD group to be significantly faster than for the ASD-S. In addition, incremental processing was found for all groups. The study findings suggest that severity of autistic behaviors will impact significantly on children's language processing in real life situations when exposed to syntactically complex material. They also show the value of using online methods for understanding how young children with ASD process language. Autism Res 2014, 7: 687–694. © 2014 International Society for Autism Research, Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
21. Predicting autism diagnosis by 7 years of age using parent report of infant social communication skills
- Author
-
Sheena Reilly, Margot Prior, Patricia Eadie, Edith L. Bavin, and Carly Veness
- Subjects
Longitudinal study ,Joint attention ,business.industry ,medicine.disease ,behavioral disciplines and activities ,Checklist ,Social relation ,Nonverbal communication ,Autism spectrum disorder ,mental disorders ,Pediatrics, Perinatology and Child Health ,Medicine ,Autism ,business ,Gesture ,Clinical psychology - Abstract
Aim The aim of this study is to identify social communication skills in infancy which predict autism spectrum disorder (ASD) diagnosis by 7 years as compared with children with other developmental difficulties or typical development from within a population sample. Methods Children with an ASD (n = 41), developmental delay (n = 28), language impairment (n = 47) and typical development (n = 41) were drawn from a large, longitudinal community sample following children from 8 months to 7 years of age, the Early Language in Victoria Study. At 7 years of age, early social communication skills at 8, 12 and 24 months from the Communication and Symbolic Behavior Scales Infant–Toddler Checklist and the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventory: Words and Gestures were compared between groups and used to predict ASD diagnosis. Results Significant predictors of ASD diagnosis were found from 8 months, predominantly focused on gesture use and communicative behaviours, such as requesting and joint attention. While comparisons between children with ASD and children with language impairment and typical development revealed differences from 8 months of age, the developmental delay group did not differ significantly from ASD on any measure until 24 months of age. At 24 months, children with ASD had lower Communication and Symbolic Behavior Scales Use of Communication scores as compared with all other groups. Conclusions The capacity to identify early markers of ASD should facilitate awareness of the risk of an ASD as compared with other developmental problems and point to the need for further developmental assessment, monitoring and provision of early intervention if indicated.
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
22. Autism in context 2: Assessment, intervention and services in Australia
- Author
-
Jacqueline Roberts, Lawrence Bartak, Margot Prior, Katrina Williams, Susan Woolfenden, and Sylvia Rodger
- Subjects
Service (business) ,medicine.medical_specialty ,business.industry ,Service delivery framework ,Behavioural intervention ,Context (language use) ,medicine.disease ,Clinical Practice ,Nursing ,Intervention (counseling) ,Pediatrics, Perinatology and Child Health ,medicine ,Autism ,Psychiatry ,business - Abstract
Continuing from part 1, part 2 of the autism spectrum disorders review explores clinical practice and service delivery aspects of autism spectrum disorders including current assessment approaches in Australia, family-centred models of care, and key service structure and delivery issues. Treatments including behavioural interventions, established and emergent medication, and complementary and alternative therapies are discussed. The key role of paediatricians as both individual child and family care providers and advocates, as well as agents of service reform in Australia, is evident. Much still needs to be done.
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
23. Autism in context 1: Classification, counting and causes
- Author
-
Lawrence Bartak, Jacqueline Roberts, Margot Prior, Sylvia Rodger, Katrina Williams, and Susan Woolfenden
- Subjects
Causes of autism ,medicine.medical_specialty ,business.industry ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Perspective (graphical) ,Context (language use) ,medicine.disease ,Key issues ,behavioral disciplines and activities ,mental disorders ,Pediatrics, Perinatology and Child Health ,Medicine ,Autism ,Quality (business) ,business ,Psychiatry ,National data ,media_common ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
This review paper describes our current perspective of autism spectrum disorders (ASD), taking into account past, current and future classification systems and the evolving definitions of ASD. International prevalence rates from 1965 to 2012 are presented and key issues, including whether there is an epidemic of autism and what this means in terms of thinking about possible causes of autism, are discussed. Also discussed is the need for high quality national data collection in Australia and the evidence, and lack of evidence, for the many theoretical causes of ASD. The lack of robust classification of autism along with limited high quality evidence base about its prevalence and possible causes leaves ample space for future discoveries.
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
24. Developing relationships between language and behaviour in preschool children from the Early Language in Victoria Study: implications for intervention
- Author
-
Edith L. Bavin, Margot Prior, Sheena Reilly, Eileen Cini, Lesley Bretherton, and Patricia Eadie
- Subjects
Biopsychosocial model ,Longitudinal study ,Vocabulary ,Intelligence quotient ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Cognition ,Developmental psychology ,Psychiatry and Mental health ,Clinical Psychology ,Language development ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,medicine ,Anxiety ,medicine.symptom ,Psychology ,Socioeconomic status ,media_common - Abstract
Following a biopsychosocial model, the study investigated the role of child factors (gender, IQ), maternal factors (psychological distress, maternal education and vocabulary, maternal distress) and environmental factors (SES) in the relationship between language impairment and behaviour problems in preschool children. Participants were drawn from the Early Language in Victoria Study (ELVS) a prospective, longitudinal study examining the epidemiology and natural history of language impairment. The study reports on 1257 participants from the ELVS who completed questionnaires at 2 and 4 years and also completed face-to-face assessments at 4 years of age. Information was collected on child language, non-verbal cognition and social-emotional development; maternal vocabulary, maternal psychological distress and parent–child interaction; and SES. This study showed that language impairment is specifically associated with hyperactivity and peer problems but not anxiety and conduct problems at the preschool stage. ...
- Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
25. Language and literacy challenges for Indigenous children in Australia
- Author
-
Margot Prior
- Subjects
Social Psychology ,media_common.quotation_subject ,English language ,Indigenous ,Literacy ,Education ,Pedagogy ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,Learning to read ,Language proficiency ,Psychology ,Competence (human resources) ,At-risk students ,Reading skills ,media_common - Abstract
Despite considerable effort, progress in ‘closing the gap’ in literacy and in school retention in Indigenous communities is very slow. The reasons for continuing problems are many and various and are generally known at social and environmental levels. In this paper, I identify and emphasize the importance of proximal factors in understanding difficulties in learning to read competently. These are influences that are closer to the actual process of learning to read, since they reside within the child and family, and strongly influence the early pathways into success in reading. These factors are not specific, they apply to all beginning readers, but they have been neglected in the many attempts to improve academic success in Indigenous children. They include particularly a degree of competence in the English language and knowledge of the basic building blocks of successful reading skills. While no-one would deny that maintaining Indigenous cultural languages is important, the hard fact is that all children...
- Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
26. Language Outcomes at 7 Years: Early Predictors and Co-Occurring Difficulties
- Author
-
Cristina McKean, Eileen Cini, Melissa Wake, Fallon Cook, Sheena Reilly, Margot Prior, Laura Conway, Patricia Eadie, Edith L. Bavin, Fiona Mensah, and Lesley Bretherton
- Subjects
Vocabulary ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Mothers ,Child Behavior Disorders ,Language Development ,Literacy ,Cohort Studies ,03 medical and health sciences ,0302 clinical medicine ,030225 pediatrics ,medicine ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Language Development Disorders ,Longitudinal Studies ,Child ,media_common ,Intelligence Tests ,Intelligence quotient ,business.industry ,05 social sciences ,Australia ,medicine.disease ,Birth order ,Language development ,Logistic Models ,Child, Preschool ,Pediatrics, Perinatology and Child Health ,Quality of Life ,Autism ,Educational Status ,Female ,Birth Order ,business ,Psychosocial ,050104 developmental & child psychology ,Clinical psychology ,Cohort study - Abstract
OBJECTIVE: To examine at 7 years the language abilities of children, the salience of early life factors and language scores as predictors of language outcome, and co-occurring difficulties METHODS: A longitudinal cohort study of 1910 infants recruited at age 8 to 10 months. Exposures included early life factors (sex, prematurity, birth weight/order, twin birth, socioeconomic status, non–English speaking background,family history of speech/language difficulties); maternal factors (mental health, vocabulary, education, and age); and child language ability at 2 and 4 years. Outcomes were 7-year standardized receptive or expressive language scores (low language: ≥1.25 SD below the mean), and co-occurring difficulties (autism, literacy, social, emotional, and behavioral adjustment, and health-related quality of life). RESULTS: Almost 19% of children (22/1204;18.9%) met criteria for low language at 7 years. Early life factors explained 9-13% of variation in language scores, increasing to 39-58% when child language scores at ages 2 and 4 were included. Early life factors moderately discriminated between children with and without low language (area under the curve: 0.68–0.72), strengthening to good discrimination with language scores at ages 2 and 4 (area under the curve: 0.85–0.94). Low language at age 7 was associated with concurrent difficulties in literacy, social-emotional and behavioral difficulties, and limitations in school and psychosocial functioning. CONCLUSIONS: Child language ability at 4 years more accurately predicted low language at 7 than a range of early child, family, and environmental factors. Low language at 7 years was associated with a higher prevalence of co-occurring difficulties.
- Published
- 2016
27. Consequences of childhood reading difficulties and behaviour problems for educational achievement and employment in early adulthood
- Author
-
Margot Prior, Craig A. Olsson, Diana Smart, George J. Youssef, Ann Sanson, and John W. Toumbourou
- Subjects
Adult ,Employment ,Male ,Parents ,Longitudinal study ,Victoria ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Occupational prestige ,Standardized test ,Child Behavior Disorders ,Vocabulary ,Education ,Developmental psychology ,Dyslexia ,Young Adult ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,Humans ,0501 psychology and cognitive sciences ,Child ,media_common ,Family Characteristics ,Language Tests ,business.industry ,05 social sciences ,050301 education ,Erikson's stages of psychosocial development ,Infant ,Achievement ,Mental health ,Educational attainment ,Unemployment ,Child, Preschool ,Well-being ,Educational Status ,Temperament ,Female ,Psychology ,business ,0503 education ,050104 developmental & child psychology - Abstract
BACKGROUND: Reading difficulties (RDs) and behaviour problems (BPs) are two common childhood problems that have a high degree of stability and often negatively affect well-being in both the short and longer terms. AIMS: The study aimed to shed light on the unique and joint consequences of these two childhood problems for educational and occupational outcomes in early adulthood. SAMPLE: Data were drawn from a life-course longitudinal study of psychosocial development, the Australian Temperament Project. METHODS: Parent and teacher reports and a standard reading test were used to define four groups of children at 7-8 years: RDs only; BPs only; both problems; and neither problem. These groups were followed forward to ascertain educational attainment and employment status at 19-20 and 23-24 years. RESULTS: Each childhood problem was a unique risk for poorer educational and occupational outcomes, with co-occurring problems significantly increasing the risk of poorer educational outcomes. Further analyses revealed that the effects of childhood BPs on occupational status were mediated by secondary school non-completion, but childhood RDs were not. CONCLUSIONS: The findings point to the importance of screening and early intervention to prevent or minimize the development of these two childhood problems, as well as continuing to support vulnerable children to increase their likelihood of secondary school completion.
- Published
- 2016
28. Predicting Language Development at Age 18 Months: Data From the Norwegian Mother and Child Cohort Study
- Author
-
Patricia Eadie, Anne-Siri Øyen, Margot Prior, Henrik Daae Zachrisson, and Synnve Schjølberg
- Subjects
Male ,Pediatrics ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Longitudinal study ,Language delay ,Birth weight ,Mothers ,Norwegian ,Multiple Birth Offspring ,Language Development ,Risk Factors ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,medicine ,Humans ,Language Development Disorders ,Prospective Studies ,Prospective cohort study ,Language Tests ,Norway ,Infant, Newborn ,Infant ,Infant, Low Birth Weight ,language.human_language ,Psychiatry and Mental health ,Low birth weight ,Socioeconomic Factors ,Pediatrics, Perinatology and Child Health ,Cohort ,language ,Educational Status ,Female ,medicine.symptom ,Factor Analysis, Statistical ,Psychology ,Child Language ,Demography ,Cohort study - Abstract
Objectives: This study investigated predictors of delayed language development at 18 months of age in a large population cohort of Norwegian toddlers. Methods: Data were analyzed on 42,107 toddlers. Language outcome at age 18 months was measured using a standard parent report instrument, the Ages and Stages Questionnaire, communication scale. Confirmatory factor analysis was conducted on the Ages and Stages Questionnaire items. A theoretically derived set of child, family, and environmental risk factors were used to predict delayed language development at age 18 months using Generalized Estimating Equation. Results: A number of child factors, including being a boy, low birth weight or gestational age, or a multiple birth child were all significantly associated with low scores on the language outcome at age 18 months. Maternal distress/depression and low maternal education, having older siblings, or a non-Norwegian lan- guage background also predicted low scores on the language outcome at age 18 months. Overall, estimated variance in language outcome explained by the model was 4% to 7%. Conclusions: A combination of early neurobiological and genetic factors (e.g., male gender, birth weight, and prematurity) and concurrent family variables (e.g., maternal distress/depression) were associated with slower language development at age 18 months. This finding replicated previous research conducted on slightly older language-delayed 2 year olds but also detected the importance of factors related to family resources for the first time in this younger age group. Despite this finding, most of the variability in language performance in this cohort of 18 month olds remained unexplained by the comprehensive set of purported risk factors. (J Dev Behav Pediatr 32:000-000, 2011) Index terms: language development, language delay, longitudinal study, risk factors.
- Published
- 2011
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
29. Relationships between language impairment, temperament, behavioural adjustment and maternal factors in a community sample of preschool children
- Author
-
Margot Prior, Eileen Cini, Patricia Eadie, Edith L. Bavin, and Sheena Reilly
- Subjects
Persistence (psychology) ,Linguistics and Language ,education.field_of_study ,Longitudinal study ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Population ,Shyness ,Language and Linguistics ,Developmental psychology ,Speech and Hearing ,Language development ,Personality ,Temperament ,education ,Psychology ,Psychosocial ,media_common - Abstract
Background:Relationshipsbetweenchildlanguagedevelopmentandtemperamenthavebeenlittlestudiedinyoung children, although it is known that children with language impairment are at risk in a number of domains of psychosocial development involving temperamental influences. Aims: To investigate the relationships between temperament and language development, along with child behavioural adjustment and maternal psychosocial factors. Methods & Procedures: A sample of 4-year-old children with language impairment was compared with typically developing children, from a large community cohort in a longitudinal study, on three temperament dimensions, behavioural and emotional problems, and maternal factors. Participants were part of a large community cohort involved in a longitudinal study. Outcomes & Results: While the groups did not differ significantly on temperamental shyness/sociability, children with language impairment showed more negative dispositions on the persistence/self-regulation factor, and on overall temperamental difficultness. Behavioural problems were elevated in the language impairment group and were associated with temperament in both groups. Maternal measures of education level, reading and vocabulary skills were significantly lower in the language impairment group. Conclusions & Implications: Generally the language impairment group showed a constellation of developmental disadvantages which add to the existing developmental vulnerability conferred by the presence of language impairment. Poorer child temperament self-regulation and behavioural adjustment are strong risk factors for school learning, while lower mother education and literacy contribute further disadvantage. Clinicians managing language impairment in children need to be aware of the whole package of risk factors which are common in this population.
- Published
- 2011
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
30. Predictors of school readiness in five‐ to six‐year‐old children from an Australian longitudinal community sample
- Author
-
Edith L. Bavin, Ben Ong, and Margot Prior
- Subjects
School readiness ,Longitudinal study ,Phonemic awareness ,Educational psychology ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Cognition ,Sample (statistics) ,Child health ,Education ,Developmental psychology ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,Psychology ,Early language ,Demography - Abstract
This paper reports on school readiness (SR) and its predictors in five‐ to six‐year‐old children from a prospective, longitudinal study of children from eight months to seven years (the Early Language in Victoria Study – ELVS). The ELVS children came from a representative sample of children recruited though the State Government Infant and Child Health Centres at the age of eight months who were studied at yearly intervals with a combination of parent surveys and face‐to‐face assessments. The study had a focus on language, pre‐literacy and behavioural development. In pre‐school and preparatory grade, teachers of the children completed a brief questionnaire rating SR characteristics, including cognitive, language and personal/social competencies. The data bank on these children provided a set of hypothesised child and family predictors of the SR score which were tested via factor analysis and regression analyses. Significant predictors of SR in the equation were evident from two years of age, and were all r...
- Published
- 2011
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
31. Father involvement: the importance of paternal Solo Care
- Author
-
Margot Prior and Katherine R. Wilson
- Subjects
Self-efficacy ,Working hours ,Social Psychology ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Pediatrics ,Pleasure ,Developmental psychology ,Parental Occupation ,Structured interview ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,Psychology ,media_common ,Father-child relations ,Clinical psychology - Abstract
Paternal time spent caring for children alone is qualitatively different from time together mediated by the presence of the mother and may be particularly relevant to father–child relations. Many fathers spend minimal time alone with their children. Indeed, it is still commonly referred to as ‘babysitting’. We explored the concept of Solo Care as a conceptually discrete dimension of father involvement. Fathers and mothers in families (n = 110) with children aged 3–12 years provided qualitative and quantitative data separately in a structured interview. Several variables were considered as potential correlates of Solo Care, with parental occupation and hours of work, perceived maternal support, perceived barriers and paternal efficacy and satisfaction proving significant. Themes describing fathering satisfaction included father–child bond, pleasure in watching children grow and father–child interaction. Important benefits of Solo Care were relationship‐focused along with the opportunity to demonstrate equa...
- Published
- 2010
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
32. Tuning in to Kids: improving emotion socialization practices in parents of preschool children - findings from a community trial
- Author
-
Katherine R. Wilson, Ann E. Harley, Christiane E. Kehoe, Margot Prior, and Sophie S. Havighurst
- Subjects
business.industry ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Socialization ,Social environment ,Poison control ,Empathy ,Suicide prevention ,Coaching ,Emotional competence ,Developmental psychology ,Psychiatry and Mental health ,Pediatrics, Perinatology and Child Health ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,Early childhood ,Psychology ,business ,Clinical psychology ,media_common - Abstract
Background: This study evaluated a new prevention and early intervention parenting program: Tuning in to Kids. The program aims to improve emotion socialization practices in parents of preschool children and is based on research evidence that parents’ responses to, and coaching of, their children’s emotions influence emotional and behavioral functioning in children. Methods: Two hundred and sixteen primary caregiver parents of children aged 4.0–5.11 years were randomized into an intervention or waitlist control group. Parents in the intervention condition attended a 6-session group parenting program plus two booster sessions. Assessment occurred pre-intervention, post-intervention and at six-month followup. Questionnaires assessed parent emotion awareness and regulation, parent beliefs and practices of emotion socialization (emotion dismissing, emotion coaching, empathy) and child behavior (parent and teacher report). Observation of emotion socialization practices and child emotional knowledge was conducted pre-intervention and at follow-up with 161 parent–child dyads. Results: Parents in the intervention condition reported significant improvements in their own emotion awareness and regulation, increases in emotion coaching, and decreases in emotionally dismissive beliefs and behaviors. There were increases in parents’ observed use of emotion labels and discussion of causes and consequences of emotions with their children. Child emotional knowledge improved, and reductions in child behavior problems were reported by parents and teachers. Conclusions: This study provides support for the efficacy of a parenting intervention targeting parent emotion socialization practices that lead to improved child emotional knowledge and behavior. This preventative intervention targeting parents’ own emotion awareness and regulation, as well as emotional communication in parent–child relationships, is a promising addition to available parenting programs. Keywords: Tuning in to Kids, emotion coaching, emotion socialization, preschool children, intervention, prevention, behavior problems, parenting. Abbreviations: TIK: Tuning in to Kids. A significant body of research now exists on the socialization of children’s emotional competence and the influence that parent–child relationships have on children’s emotional and social development. However, while evidence now links aspects of parenting around emotions to child outcomes, there are few if any published studies of parenting interventions which draw on this theory and apply it in practice. The current study reports on the outcomes of the Tuning in to Kids parenting program, a program designed to target emotion socialization practices.
- Published
- 2010
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
33. Anxiety is more common in children with asthma
- Author
-
Sharon L. Brennan, Colin F. Robertson, Margot Prior, Bernard Jenner, Peter Vuillermin, Mike South, and John B. Carlin
- Subjects
Male ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Adolescent ,Victoria ,Cross-sectional study ,Anxiety ,Anti-asthmatic Agent ,immune system diseases ,Absenteeism ,medicine ,Humans ,Anti-Asthmatic Agents ,Child ,Psychiatry ,Asthma ,Psychiatric Status Rating Scales ,Response rate (survey) ,business.industry ,medicine.disease ,Comorbidity ,respiratory tract diseases ,Hospitalization ,Cross-Sectional Studies ,El Niño ,Child, Preschool ,Pediatrics, Perinatology and Child Health ,Female ,medicine.symptom ,Emergency Service, Hospital ,business ,Anxiety disorder - Abstract
There are a variety of reasons why there may be an association between asthma and anxiety in children. Research into the relation between asthma and anxiety has been limited by the sole use of parent-reported or self-reported asthma symptoms to define asthma status. The objective of this study was to determine if children with physician-defined asthma are more likely to suffer anxiety than children without asthma.A population-based, cross-sectional assessment, of self-reported anxiety symptoms.Children aged 5-13 years from Barwon region of Victoria, Australia. Asthma status was determined by review with a paediatrician. Controls were a sample of children without asthma symptoms (matched for age, gender and school).The Spence Children's Anxiety Scale (SCAS) written questionnaire. The authors compared the mean SCAS score, and the proportion of children with an SCAS score in the clinical range, between the groups.Questionnaires were issued to 205 children with asthma (158 returned, response rate 77%), and 410 controls (319 returned, response rate 78%). The SCAS scores were higher in asthmatics than controls (p0.001); and were more likely to be in the clinical range (OR=2.5, 95% CI 1.1 to 5.8, p=0.036). There was no evidence that these associations could be explained by known confounding factors.Children with asthma are substantially more likely to suffer anxiety than children without asthma. Future studies are required to determine the sequence of events that leads to this comorbidity, and to test strategies to prevent and treat anxiety among children with asthma.
- Published
- 2010
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
34. Tuning in to kids: an emotion-focused parenting program-initial findings from a community trial
- Author
-
Sophie S. Havighurst, Katherine R. Wilson, Ann E. Harley, and Margot Prior
- Subjects
Social Psychology ,Child rearing ,business.industry ,Emotional intelligence ,education ,Psychological intervention ,Coaching ,Developmental psychology ,Emotional competence ,law.invention ,Randomized controlled trial ,law ,Parent training ,business ,Psychology ,Competence (human resources) ,Clinical psychology - Abstract
This study evaluated a new group parenting program, Tuning in to Kids, which taught emotion coaching skills to parents of preschool children. In a randomized control trial, 218 primary caregiver parents of children aged 4.0–5.11 years completed questionnaires assessing parent emotion socialization (emotion coaching vs. emotion dismissing), parent emotional competence, parent wellbeing and child behavior. Assessment occurred at preintervention and 10 weeks later. Parents randomized to the intervention condition (n=107) attended a 6-session parenting program. Results showed parents in the intervention condition reported significant increases in emotion coaching and significant reductions in emotion dismissing with their children. Child behavior was also reported to improve. Of those with clinical levels of behavior difficulties, more than half were no longer at clinical level postprogram. These findings suggest that an emotion-focused parenting intervention may assist parents to learn emotion-coaching skills that have been linked to improved child behavior. © 2009 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.
- Published
- 2009
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
35. Predicting Stuttering Onset by the Age of 3 Years: A Prospective, Community Cohort Study
- Author
-
Edith L. Bavin, Melissa Wake, Eileen Cini, Margot Prior, Obioha C Ukoumunne, Ann Packman, Mark Onslow, Sheena Reilly, Patricia Eadie, and Catherine Bolzonello
- Subjects
Male ,Pediatrics ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Longitudinal study ,Stuttering ,Language delay ,Birth weight ,Language Development ,Article ,Cohort Studies ,Predictive Value of Tests ,Residence Characteristics ,Risk Factors ,Humans ,Medicine ,Longitudinal Studies ,Prospective Studies ,Age of Onset ,business.industry ,Age Factors ,Infant ,nervous system diseases ,Birth order ,Child, Preschool ,Pediatrics, Perinatology and Child Health ,Cohort ,Female ,medicine.symptom ,Age of onset ,business ,Cohort study - Abstract
OBJECTIVES. Our goals were to document (1) the onset of stuttering and (2) whether specific child, family, or environmental factors predict stuttering onset in children aged up to 3 years. METHODS. Participants included a community-ascertained cohort of 1619 2-year-old Australian children recruited at 8 months of age to study the longitudinal development of early language. The main outcome measure was parental telephone report of stuttering onset, verified by face-to-face expert diagnosis. Preonset continuous measures of the child's temperament (approach/withdrawal) and language development were available. Information on a range of predictor measures hypothesized to be associated with stuttering onset was obtained (maternal mental health and education levels, gender, premature birth status, birth weight, birth order, twinning, socioeconomic status, family history of stuttering). RESULTS. By 3 years of age, the cumulative incidence of stuttering onset was 8.5%. Onset often occurred suddenly over 1 to 3 days (49.6%) and involved the use of word combinations (97.1%). Children who stuttered were not more shy or withdrawn. Male gender, twin birth status, higher vocabulary scores at 2 years of age, and high maternal education were associated with stuttering onset. The multivariable model, however, had low predictive strength; just 3.7% of the total variation in stuttering onset was accounted for. CONCLUSIONS. The cumulative incidence of stuttering onset was much higher than reported previously. The hypothesized risk factors for stuttering onset together explained little of the variation in stuttering onset up to 3 years of age. Early onset was not associated with language delay, social and environmental factors, or preonset shyness/withdrawal. Health professionals can reassure parents that onset is not unusual up to 3 years of age and seems to be associated with rapid growth in language development.
- Published
- 2009
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
36. The Early Language in Victoria Study (ELVS): A prospective, longitudinal study of communication skills and expressive vocabulary development at 8, 12 and 24 months
- Author
-
Edith L. Bavin, Patricia Eadie, Lesley Bretherton, Margot Prior, Sheena Reilly, Melissa Wake, Obioha C Ukoumunne, Eileen Cini, and Laura Conway
- Subjects
Vocabulary ,Longitudinal study ,Research and Theory ,Language delay ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Birth weight ,LPN and LVN ,Language and Linguistics ,Vocabulary development ,Developmental psychology ,Speech and Hearing ,Birth order ,Otorhinolaryngology ,Multiple birth ,Toddler ,Psychology ,media_common - Abstract
The aim of this paper is to provide an overview of the methods and preliminary findings from the Early Language In Victoria Study (ELVS) a prospective, longitudinal study of child language impairment. Specifically, we provide a summary of early communication and vocabulary development and examine the contributions a range of risk factors and predictors make to these outcomes. The sample was a community-ascertained cohort of 1911 infants, recruited at 8 months and followed at ages 12 and 24 months. The main outcomes of interest were parent reported infant and toddler communication (Communication and Symbolic Behavior Scales, CSBS) and expressive vocabulary (MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories, CDI). Predictors included gender, preterm birth, birth weight, multiple birth, birth order, socioeconomic status, maternal mental health, maternal vocabulary and education, maternal age at birth of child, non–English-speaking background, and a family history of speech and/or language difficulties. R...
- Published
- 2009
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
37. REMEDIATION FOR SUBGROUPS OF RETARDED READERS USING A MODIFIED ORAL SPELLING PROCEDURE
- Author
-
Claire M. Fletcher, Sally Frye, and Margot Prior
- Subjects
Male ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Adolescent ,media_common.quotation_subject ,education ,Audiology ,Verbal learning ,Generalization, Psychological ,Dyslexia ,Developmental Neuroscience ,Phonetics ,Reading (process) ,medicine ,Humans ,Remedial Teaching ,Child ,Remedial education ,Referral and Consultation ,media_common ,Verbal Learning ,medicine.disease ,Spelling ,Education, Special ,Pediatrics, Perinatology and Child Health ,Female ,Neurology (clinical) ,Psychology ,After treatment ,Follow-Up Studies - Abstract
The effectiveness of Bradley's modification of the method of Simultaneous Oral Spelling (SOS) remedial training for both spelling and reading was assessed in this study. Subgroups of disabled readers, 'Chinese' (dysphonetic) and 'Phonecians' (dyseidetic), received six remedial sessions and were assessed immediately after treatment and then two months later on spelling of the training words and on general reading measures. Compared with a non-treated group, the two experimental groups showed significant but different general gains in reading. SOS training appears to be more effective for the teaching of regular words, as opposed to exceptional ones.
- Published
- 2008
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
38. Children subjected to cardiac surgery for congenital heart disease. Part 2 – Parental emotional experiences☆☆☆
- Author
-
Samuel Menahem, Zeffie Poulakis, and Margot Prior
- Subjects
Heart Defects, Congenital ,Male ,Parents ,Pulmonary and Respiratory Medicine ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Time Factors ,Heart disease ,Emotions ,Anxiety ,Social support ,Surveys and Questionnaires ,Humans ,Medicine ,Prospective Studies ,Cardiac Surgical Procedures ,Parent-Child Relations ,Child ,Psychiatry ,Prospective cohort study ,business.industry ,medicine.disease ,Mental health ,Cardiac surgery ,Distress ,Locus of control ,Child, Preschool ,Female ,Perception ,Surgery ,medicine.symptom ,Cardiology and Cardiovascular Medicine ,business ,Stress, Psychological - Abstract
Parents experience considerable distress when their children are subjected to cardiac surgery. This study investigated their psychological and emotional experiences. As part of a prospective study reviewing the emotional and psychological outcomes of children aged 2-12 years subjected to cardiac surgery, that age group being chosen to allow for objective testing following infancy and before adolescence, their parents were assessed prior to and 12-50 months following the surgery. The measures reviewed their mental health, locus of control, family functioning and social support. There were 39 children. Most of the parental information was obtained from the mothers, who reported increased anxiety, and a tendency to attribute events to luck and/or chance greater than published norms, irrespective of the cardiac anomaly, whether the surgery was 'curative', or if further surgery was required. At follow-up, their ratings approximated to norms, except for a continued perception that life events were a function of fate and beyond one's control. The results confirmed that a substantial increase in the emotional distress of mothers at the time of surgery essentially resolved by 12 months or later. In contrast, they still seemed not to feel in 'control' when reviewed on follow-up.
- Published
- 2008
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
39. Children subjected to cardiac surgery for congenital heart disease. Part 1 – Emotional and psychological outcomes☆☆☆
- Author
-
Samuel Menahem, Margot Prior, and Zeffie Poulakis
- Subjects
Heart Defects, Congenital ,Male ,Reoperation ,Pulmonary and Respiratory Medicine ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Time Factors ,Heart disease ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Emotions ,MEDLINE ,Child Behavior ,Psychology, Child ,Vocabulary ,Quality of life (healthcare) ,Surveys and Questionnaires ,Adaptation, Psychological ,medicine ,Humans ,Prospective Studies ,Cardiac Surgical Procedures ,Child ,Prospective cohort study ,Receptive vocabulary ,media_common ,business.industry ,Case-control study ,medicine.disease ,Cardiac surgery ,Treatment Outcome ,Case-Control Studies ,Child, Preschool ,Physical therapy ,Female ,Surgery ,Temperament ,Cardiology and Cardiovascular Medicine ,business ,Stress, Psychological - Abstract
This study investigated the psychological and emotional functioning of children with congenital heart disease (CHD) subjected to surgery. Children aged 2-12 years with CHD who underwent cardiac surgery were enrolled. Information was collected prior to surgery and 12 months or later following surgery. Measures included assessment of the child's receptive vocabulary, adaptive behaviour skills, emotional and behavioural development, temperament and parent quality of life, as well as surgical data. Similar information was collected from a control group prior to undergoing non-cardiac surgery. Of the 69 children contacted to enrol, completed pre- and post-surgical data were obtained from 39 children, and pre-surgical data from 12 controls. Children with CHD subjected to surgery displayed psychological and emotional functioning indistinguishable from normative populations or the control group. These findings persisted at reassessment 12-50 months after surgery. Psychological functioning at follow-up was most closely related to functioning prior to surgery. Significant residual defects and the need for further surgery were associated with poorer functioning. The results suggest an optimistic psychological and emotional outcome following cardiac surgery. This study may assist in identifying children most at risk of adverse outcomes after cardiac surgery and help guide therapeutic interventional programmes.
- Published
- 2008
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
40. Associations between Early Childhood Temperament Clusters and Later Psychosocial Adjustment
- Author
-
Primrose Letcher, Frank Oberklaid, John W. Toumbourou, Ann Sanson, Margot Prior, and Diana Smart
- Subjects
Aggression ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Attention span ,Education ,Developmental psychology ,Emotionality ,Cohort ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,medicine ,Personality ,Temperament ,Early childhood ,medicine.symptom ,Psychology ,Psychosocial ,Social Sciences (miscellaneous) ,media_common ,Clinical psychology - Abstract
The study adopted a person-centered approach to examine whether clusters of children could be identified on the basis of temperament profiles assessed on four occasions from infancy to early childhood, and if so whether differing temperament clusters were associated with subsequent differences in behavior problems, social skills, and school adjustment in middle and late childhood. Parent, teacher, and self-report data were obtained from a large community-based cohort sample of Australian children, followed prospectively from infancy to late childhood. Four temperament clusters were identified. Children in the clusters labeled as reactive/inhibited and poor attention regulation tended to have higher levels of later behavior problems than children in clusters labeled nonreactive/outgoing and high attention regulation. Results suggested that a person-oriented clustering approach can identify children on the basis of early temperament who are at greater risk for behavioral, academic, and social difficulties four to eight years later.
- Published
- 2008
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
41. Predicting Language at 2 Years of Age: A Prospective Community Study
- Author
-
Sheena Reilly, Edith L. Bavin, Melissa Wake, Patricia Eadie, Lesley Bretherton, Joanne Williams, Yin Barrett, Obioha C Ukoumunne, and Margot Prior
- Subjects
Male ,Vocabulary ,Longitudinal study ,business.industry ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Birth weight ,Logistic regression ,medicine.disease ,Language Development ,Birth order ,Risk Factors ,Premature birth ,Child, Preschool ,Pediatrics, Perinatology and Child Health ,medicine ,Humans ,Female ,Language Development Disorders ,Multiple birth ,Prospective Studies ,business ,Symbolic behavior ,media_common ,Demography - Abstract
OBJECTIVE. This article responds to evidence gaps regarding language impairment identified by the US Preventive Services Task Force in 2006. We examine the contributions of putative child, family, and environmental risk factors to language outcomes at 24 months of age. METHODS. A community-ascertained sample of 1720 infants who were recruited at 8 months of age were followed at ages 12 and 24 months in a prospective, longitudinal study in metropolitan Melbourne, Australia. Outcomes at 24 months were parent-reported infant communication (Communication and Symbolic Behavior Scales and MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories vocabulary production score). Putative risk factors were gender, preterm birth, birth weight, multiple birth, birth order, socioeconomic status, maternal mental health, maternal vocabulary and education, maternal age at birth of child, non–English-speaking background, and family history of speech-language difficulties. Linear regression models were fitted to total standardized Communication and Symbolic Behavior Scales and Communicative Development Inventories vocabulary production scores; a logistic regression model was fitted to late-talking status at 24 months. RESULTS. The regression models accounted for 4.3% and 7.0% of the variation in the 24-month Communication and Symbolic Behavior Scales and Communicative Development Inventories scores, respectively. Male gender and family history were strongly associated with poorer outcomes on both instruments. Lower Communication and Symbolic Behavior Scales scores were also associated with lower maternal vocabulary and older maternal age. Lower vocabulary production scores were associated with birth order and non–English-speaking background. When the 12-month Communication and Symbolic Behavior Scales Total score was added as a covariate in the linear regression of 24-month Communication and Symbolic Behavior Scales Total score, it was by far the strongest predictor. CONCLUSIONS. These early risk factors explained no more than 7% of the variation in language at 24 months. They seem unlikely to be helpful in screening for early language delay.
- Published
- 2007
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
42. No interaction between the serotonin transporter polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) and childhood adversity or recent stressful life events on symptoms of depression: Results from two community surveys
- Author
-
Margot Prior, P. Chipman, Anthony F. Jorm, Simon Easteal, Diana Smart, Ann Sanson, and Xiaoyun Tan
- Subjects
Adult ,Male ,medicine.medical_specialty ,Adolescent ,Genotype ,Life Change Events ,Cellular and Molecular Neuroscience ,Child and adolescent psychiatry ,Humans ,Medicine ,Young adult ,Gene–environment interaction ,Genetics (clinical) ,Depression (differential diagnoses) ,Serotonin transporter ,Serotonin Plasma Membrane Transport Proteins ,Sex Characteristics ,Polymorphism, Genetic ,biology ,Depression ,business.industry ,Data Collection ,Psychiatry and Mental health ,Logistic Models ,El Niño ,5-HTTLPR ,biology.protein ,Female ,business ,Clinical psychology - Abstract
In this study we investigated interactions between the 5-HTTLPR genotype and environmental risk factors (G × E) on symptoms of depression in two large Australian community samples of adolescents and young adults. We postulated that a significant interaction between the 5-HTTLPR genotype and environmental risk factors of childhood adversity or stressful life events on symptoms of depression would be observed in subjects with at least one short allele (s/l or s/s) compared with subjects with no short alleles (l/l). We did not find significant G × E interactions between the 5-HTTLPR genotype and recent stressful life events or childhood adversity on symptoms of depression in our sample populations. However, we did find adolescents aged 17–18 years homozygous for the long allele (l/l) and exposed to persistently high levels of family adversity over a 6-year period were at a greater risk of depression than subjects with the same genotype exposed to no or persistently low levels of family adversity. This interaction should be interpreted cautiously due to the small number of depressed subjects in the sample with persistently high levels of family adversity. © 2007 Wiley-Liss, Inc.
- Published
- 2007
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
43. The impact of temperament factors and family functioning on resilience processes from infancy to school age
- Author
-
Margot Prior and Kristin S. Mathiesen
- Subjects
Longitudinal study ,School age child ,Social Psychology ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Family functioning ,Norwegian ,language.human_language ,Developmental psychology ,Social support ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,language ,Social competence ,Temperament ,Psychological resilience ,Psychology ,media_common ,Clinical psychology - Abstract
A Norwegian sample of 401 mothers was used to investigate the effects of child factors and family risk and supportive factors on both child competencies and behaviour problems at 8 years. The contribution of the various predictors on the differentiation of children into groups of resilient vs. vulnerable across four different time periods was also examined within a prospective longitudinal study design. The study showed that a substantial proportion of the variance in both behaviour problems and social competence at 8 years could be explained by risk and supportive factors in child and family already present at 18 months. Within-child and within environment predictors had selective impact on each outcome domain in addition to common effects. Depending on time of assessment, 73 – 89% of the children from stressed families could be correctly classified as resilient. Pathways to “resilience” and “positive adjustment” are apparent from early in life, with social support factors and temperament characteristics...
- Published
- 2006
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
44. Growth of infant communication between 8 and 12 months: A population study
- Author
-
Edith L. Bavin, Joanne Williams, Patricia Eadie, Margot Prior, Sheena Reilly, Melissa Wake, Lesley Bretherton, Yin Barrett, and Obioha C Ukoumunne
- Subjects
Language delay ,business.industry ,media_common.quotation_subject ,education ,Developmental psychology ,Nonverbal communication ,Language development ,Promotion (rank) ,Variation (linguistics) ,Pediatrics, Perinatology and Child Health ,Medicine ,Risk factor ,business ,Gesture ,media_common ,Cohort study - Abstract
= 37.0). Conclusions: There is a dramatic increase in communication skills between 8 and 12 months, particularly the development of gesture, which (as in previous studies) predates and predicts future language development. Risk factors explained little variation in early communication trajectories and therefore, based on our findings, this developmental course is more likely to be biologically predetermined. Rather than focusing on risk factors, we suggest that language promotion activities in otherwise healthy young infants should either be universal or, if targeted, be based on the level of communication skills displayed.
- Published
- 2006
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
45. Book Reviews
- Author
-
Meeri Hellstén, Alison Madelaine, Slava Kalyuga, John T. E. Richardson, Margot Prior, and Gordon Lyons
- Subjects
Developmental and Educational Psychology ,Media studies ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Psychology ,Classics ,Education - Abstract
Book Review : William O’Grady. How Children Learn Language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 2005. ISBN 0-521-53192-6
- Published
- 2006
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
46. The Cognitive and Academic Benefits of Music to Children: Facts and fiction
- Author
-
Rudi Črnčec, Sarah J. Wilson, and Margot Prior
- Subjects
Music psychology ,Music and emotion ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,Cognitive development ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,Academic achievement ,Mozart effect ,Appreciative listening ,Psychology ,Special education ,Music education ,Education ,Cognitive psychology - Abstract
There is considerable interest in the potential non‐musical cognitive and academic benefits of music listening and instruction to children. This report describes three lines of research relevant to this issue, namely, the effects of: (1) focused music listening on subsequent task performance (the Mozart effect); (2) music instruction; and (3) background music listening. Research suggests that while Mozart effect studies have attracted considerable media attention, the effect cannot be reliably demonstrated in children. In contrast, music instruction confers consistent benefits for spatiotemporal reasoning skills; however, improvements in associated academic domains, such as arithmetic, have not been reliably shown. Finally, background music may calm and focus children with special education needs, thereby enhancing learning. Additional research is required to determine whether this effect is evident in normal populations. Overall, evidence for the non‐musical benefits of music listening and instruction is...
- Published
- 2006
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
47. No Evidence for the Mozart Effect in Children
- Author
-
Rudi Crncec, Margot Prior, and Sarah J. Wilson
- Subjects
Mood ,Popular music ,Spatial ability ,Cognitive development ,Active listening ,MOZART ,Mozart effect ,Psychology ,behavioral disciplines and activities ,humanities ,Music ,Cognitive psychology ,Piano sonata - Abstract
The Mozart Effect refers to claims that listening to Mozart-like music results in a small, short-lived improvement in spatiotemporal performance. Based on predominantly adult research that has shown equivocal findings, there has been speculation that the Mozart effect may have pedagogical benefits for children. The present study aimed to examine the Mozart effect in children and to evaluate two alternative models proposed to account for the effect, namely the trion model and the arousal-mood model. One hundred and thirty-six Grade 5 students (mean age 10.7 years) were exposed to three experimental listening conditions: Mozart piano sonata K. 448, popular music, and silence. Each condition was followed by a spatiotemporal task, and mood and music questionnaires. The results showed no evidence of a Mozart effect. Speculation about applications of the Mozart effect in children needs to be suspended until an effect can be reliably reproduced.
- Published
- 2006
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
48. The past decade
- Author
-
Gary B. Mesibov, Lorna Wing, Margot Prior, and Joaquin Fuentes
- Subjects
Publishing ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,medicine ,Humans ,Autism ,Autistic Disorder ,medicine.disease ,Psychology ,Developmental psychology - Published
- 2006
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
49. A longitudinal study of adolescent adjustment following family transitions
- Author
-
Margot Prior, Diana Smart, Eda Ruschena, and Ann Sanson
- Subjects
Male ,Longitudinal study ,Adolescent ,Remarriage ,media_common.quotation_subject ,Peer Group ,Developmental psychology ,Family disruption ,Interpersonal relationship ,Surveys and Questionnaires ,Adaptation, Psychological ,Developmental and Educational Psychology ,Humans ,Personality ,Family ,Interpersonal Relations ,Child ,Temperament ,media_common ,Peer group ,Group Processes ,Psychiatry and Mental health ,Pediatrics, Perinatology and Child Health ,Female ,Social competence ,Psychology ,Follow-Up Studies - Abstract
Background: This study examined the impact of family transitions, that is, parental separation, divorce, remarriage and death, upon the lives of Australian children and adolescents in a longitudinal study of temperament and development. Methods: Using longitudinal and concurrent questionnaire data, outcomes for young people experiencing transitions were compared with those of a random comparison group whose biological parents remained together. Results: No significant group differences were found with regard to behavioural and emotional adjustment concurrently or across time, nor on academic outcomes and social competence. Significant differences between the groups were revealed in measures of parent–teen conflict and parent–child attachment. A number of gender differences were found, with female participants displaying both greater adaptive and maladaptive behaviours. Particular dimensions of temperament, as well as the parent's overall rating of their child as easy or difficult, were found to be important predictors of adjustment status for both transitions and comparison groups. Conclusions: Despite some methodological limitations, this study illustrated the resilience of children experiencing family disruption across childhood and adolescence.
- Published
- 2005
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
50. Effectiveness of CBT Versus Standard Treatment for Childhood Anxiety Disorders in a Community Clinic Setting
- Author
-
Julie Barrington, Margaret Richardson, Kathleen Allen, and Margot Prior
- Subjects
Family therapy ,medicine.medical_specialty ,business.industry ,Child psychopathology ,Standard treatment ,Treatment as usual ,Experimental and Cognitive Psychology ,behavioral disciplines and activities ,Child psychotherapy ,Clinical Psychology ,mental disorders ,Childhood anxiety ,Medicine ,Community setting ,Anxiety ,medicine.symptom ,business ,Psychiatry ,Clinical psychology - Abstract
The efficacy of cognitive—behavioural therapy (CBT) for the treatment of childhood anxiety has mainly been demonstrated in university-clinic settings. This study aimed to evaluate the effectiveness of CBT for the treatment of childhood anxiety in a community mental health service, compared with standard treatments (‘Treatment as Usual’ [TAU]) 'child psychotherapy, family therapy and eclectic treatments. Fifty-four children with anxiety disorders, aged from 7 to 14 years, were randomly assigned to either a CBT or TAU group. CBT and the standard treatments were provided by 18 experienced therapists, and the mean number of treatment sessions was 12. Baseline and follow-up measures at 3, 6 and 12 months included an interview based on criteria in the fourth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV) that was conducted by a clinical psychologist who was blind to the treatment conditions, and standardised anxiety measures (parent, child and teacher reports). Significant improvements were found on all anxiety measures over time, but no significant differences were found between CBT and TAU. The challenge of conducting such research in community settings is discussed.
- Published
- 2005
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.