26 results on '"Hartshorne, Joshua"'
Search Results
2. Developmental psychologists should adopt citizen science to improve generalization and reproducibility.
- Author
-
Li, Wei, Germine, Laura Thi, Mehr, Samuel A., Srinivasan, Mahesh, and Hartshorne, Joshua
- Subjects
RESEARCH evaluation ,DEVELOPMENTAL psychology ,PSYCHOLOGISTS ,ECOLOGY ,LEARNING strategies ,ASTRONOMY ,CITIZEN science ,MEDICAL needs assessment - Abstract
Widespread failures of replication and generalization are, ironically, a scientific triumph, in that they confirm the fundamental metascientific theory that underlies our field. Generalizable and replicable findings require testing large numbers of subjects from a wide range of demographics with a large, randomly‐sampled stimulus set, and using a variety of experimental parameters. Because few studies accomplish any of this, meta‐scientists predict that findings will frequently fail to replicate or generalize. We argue that to be more robust and replicable, developmental psychology needs to find a mechanism for collecting data at a greater scale and from more diverse populations. Luckily, this mechanism already exists as follows: Citizen science, in which large numbers of uncompensated volunteers provide data. While best‐known for its contributions to astronomy and ecology, citizen science has also produced major findings in neuroscience and psychology, and increasingly in developmental psychology. We provide examples, address practical challenges, discuss limitations, and compare to other methods of obtaining large datasets. Ultimately, we argue that the range of studies where it makes sense *not* to use citizen science is steadily dwindling. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2024
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
3. Neither neural networks nor the language-of-thought alone make a complete game.
- Author
-
Oved, Iris, Krishnaswamy, Nikhil, Pustejovsky, James, and Hartshorne, Joshua K.
- Subjects
MENTAL representation ,COGNITIVE science - Abstract
Cognitive science has evolved since early disputes between radical empiricism and radical nativism. The authors are reacting to the revival of radical empiricism spurred by recent successes in deep neural network (NN) models. We agree that language-like mental representations (language-of-thoughts [LoTs]) are part of the best game in town, but they cannot be understood independent of the other players. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2023
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
4. Even Simultaneous Bilinguals Do Not Reach Monolingual Levels of Proficiency in Syntax.
- Author
-
Li, Wei and Hartshorne, Joshua K.
- Subjects
BILINGUALISM ,MONOLINGUALISM ,LANGUAGE ability ,SYNTAX (Grammar) ,VARIATION in language - Abstract
While there is no doubt that children raised bilingual can become extremely proficient in both languages, theorists are divided on whether bilingualism is effectively monolingualism twice (the "Two Monolinguals in One Brain" hypothesis) or differs in some fundamental way from monolingualism. A strong version of the "Two Monolinguals" hypothesis predicts that bilinguals can achieve monolingual-level proficiency in either (or both) of their languages. Recently, Bylund and Abrahamsson argued that evidence of lower syntactic proficiency in simultaneous bilinguals was due to confounds of language dominance; when simultaneous bilinguals are tested in their primary language, any difference disappears. We find no evidence for this hypothesis. Meta-analysis and Monte Carlo simulation show that variation in published results is fully consistent with sampling error, with no evidence that method mattered. Meta-analytic estimates strongly indicate lower syntactic performance by simultaneous bilinguals relative to monolinguals. Re-analysis of a large dataset (N = 115,020) confirms this finding, even controlling for language dominance. Interestingly, the effect is relatively small, challenging current theories. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
5. Contrastive machine learning reveals the structure of neuroanatomical variation within autism.
- Author
-
Aglinskas, Aidas, Hartshorne, Joshua K., and Anzellotti, Stefano
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
6. When Do Children Lose the Language Instinct? A Critical Review of the Critical Periods Literature.
- Author
-
Hartshorne, Joshua K.
- Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
7. Politics is Not a Spectator Sport: On the Role of Psycholinguists in a Global Crisis.
- Author
-
Hartshorne, Joshua K.
- Subjects
PSYCHOLINGUISTICS ,NUCLEAR warfare ,CLIMATE change ,POLITICAL science ,SCIENTISTS - Published
- 2022
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
8. The many blessings of abstraction: A commentary on Ambridge (2020).
- Author
-
Hartshorne, Joshua K.
- Subjects
LANGUAGE acquisition ,ABSTRACTION in children ,EXAMPLE ,BAYESIAN analysis ,SEMANTICS - Abstract
Ambridge argues that the existence of exemplar models for individual phenomena (words, inflection rules, etc.) suggests the feasibility of a unified, exemplars-everywhere model that eschews abstraction. The argument would be strengthened by a description of such a model. However, none is provided. I show that any attempt to do so would immediately run into significant difficulties – difficulties that illustrate the utility of abstractions. I conclude with a brief review of modern symbolic approaches that address the concerns Ambridge raises about abstractions. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
9. Moral Values Reveal the Causality Implicit in Verb Meaning.
- Author
-
Niemi, Laura, Hartshorne, Joshua, Gerstenberg, Tobias, Stanley, Matthew, and Young, Liane
- Subjects
ETHICS ,AGRAMMATISM ,INTUITION ,SUPPORT groups ,VERBS ,SOCIAL perception - Abstract
Prior work has found that moral values that build and bind groups—that is, the binding values of ingroup loyalty, respect for authority, and preservation of purity—are linked to blaming people who have been harmed. The present research investigated whether people's endorsement of binding values predicts their assignment of the causal locus of harmful events to the victims of the events. We used an implicit causality task from psycholinguistics in which participants read a sentence in the form "SUBJECT verbed OBJECT because..." where male and female proper names occupy the SUBJECT and OBJECT position. The participants were asked to predict the pronoun that follows "because"—the referent to the subject or object—which indicates their intuition about the likely cause of the event. We also collected explicit judgments of causal contributions and measured participants' moral values to investigate the relationship between moral values and the causal interpretation of events. Using two verb sets and two independent replications (N = 459, 249, 788), we found that greater endorsement of binding values was associated with a higher likelihood of selecting the object as the cause for harmful events in the implicit causality task, a result consistent with, and supportive of, previous moral psychological work on victim blaming. Endorsement of binding values also predicted explicit causal attributions to victims. Overall, these findings indicate that moral values that support the group rather than the individual reliably predict that people shift the causal locus of harmful events to those affected by the harms. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2020
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
10. A thousand studies for the price of one: Accelerating psychological science with Pushkin.
- Author
-
Hartshorne, Joshua K., de Leeuw, Joshua R., Goodman, Noah D., Jennings, Mariela, and O'Donnell, Timothy J.
- Subjects
INTERNET access ,PRACTICAL reason ,DATA security ,EXPERIMENTAL design ,CITIZEN science - Abstract
Half of the world's population has internet access. In principle, researchers are no longer limited to subjects they can recruit into the laboratory. Any study that can be run on a computer or mobile device can be run with nearly any demographic anywhere in the world, and in large numbers. This has allowed scientists to effectively run hundreds of experiments at once. Despite their transformative power, such studies remain rare for practical reasons: the need for sophisticated software, the difficulty of recruiting so many subjects, and a lack of research paradigms that make effective use of their large amounts of data, due to such realities as that they require sophisticated software in order to run effectively. We present Pushkin: an open-source platform for designing and conducting massive experiments over the internet. Pushkin allows for a wide range of behavioral paradigms, through integration with the intuitive and flexible jsPsych experiment engine. It also addresses the basic technical challenges associated with massive, worldwide studies, including auto-scaling, extensibility, machine-assisted experimental design, multisession studies, and data security. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
11. The effect of working memory maintenance on long-term memory.
- Author
-
Hartshorne, Joshua K. and Makovski, Tal
- Subjects
ABILITY ,LANGUAGE & languages ,META-analysis ,RECOGNITION (Psychology) ,SHORT-term memory ,VERBAL behavior ,TRAINING ,TASK performance - Abstract
Initially inspired by the Atkinson and Shiffrin model, researchers have spent a half century investigating whether actively maintaining an item in working memory (WM) leads to improved subsequent long-term memory (LTM). Empirical results have been inconsistent, and thus the answer to the question remains unclear. We present evidence from 13 new experiments as well as a meta-analysis of 61 published experiments. Both the new experiments and meta-analysis show clear evidence that increased WM maintenance of a stimulus leads to superior recognition for that stimulus in subsequent LTM tests. This effect appears robust across a variety of experimental design parameters, suggesting that the variability in prior results in the literature is probably due to low power and random chance. The results support theories on which there is a close link between WM and LTM mechanisms, while challenging claims that this relationship is specific to verbal memory and evolved to support language acquisition. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
12. The neural computation of scalar implicature.
- Author
-
Hartshorne, Joshua K., Snedeker, Jesse, Stephanie Yen-Mun Liem Azar, and Kim, Albert E.
- Subjects
BRAIN physiology ,CLUSTER analysis (Statistics) ,ELECTRODES ,ELECTROENCEPHALOGRAPHY ,EVOKED potentials (Electrophysiology) ,EXPERIMENTAL design ,COMPARATIVE grammar ,JUDGMENT (Psychology) ,RESEARCH funding ,SEMANTICS ,PHONOLOGICAL awareness ,DESCRIPTIVE statistics - Abstract
Language comprehension involves not only constructing the literal meaning of a sentence but also going beyond the literal meaning to infer what was meant but not said. One widely studied test case is scalar implicature: The inference that, e.g., Sally ate some of the cookies implies she did not eat all of them. Research is mixed on whether this is due to a rote, grammaticalised procedure or instead a complex, contextualised inference. We find that in sentences like If Sally ate some of the cookies, then the rest are on the counter, that the rest triggers a late, sustained positivity relative to Sally ate some of the cookies, and the rest are on the counter. This is consistent with behavioural results and linguistic theory suggesting that the former sentence does not trigger a scalar implicature. This motivates a view on which scalar implicature is contextualised but dependent on grammatical structure. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
13. Love is hard to understand: the relationship between transitivity and caused events in the acquisition of emotion verbs.
- Author
-
HARTSHORNE, JOSHUA K., POGUE, AMANDA, and SNEDEKER, JESSE
- Abstract
Famously, dog bites man is trivia whereas man bites dog is news. This illustrates not just a fact about the world but about language: to know who did what to whom, we must correctly identify the mapping between semantic role and syntactic position. These mappings are typically predictable, and previous work demonstrates that young children are sensitive to these patterns and so could use them in acquisition. However, there is only limited and mixed evidence that children do use this information to guide acquisition outside of the laboratory. We find that children understand emotion verbs which follow the canonical CAUSE–VERB–PATIENT pattern (Mary frightened/delighted John) earlier than those which do not (Mary feared/liked John), despite the latter's higher frequency, suggesting children's generalization of the mapping between causativity and transitivity is broad and active in acquisition. [ABSTRACT FROM PUBLISHER]
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
14. Development of the first-mention bias.
- Author
-
HARTSHORNE, JOSHUA K., NAPPA, REBECCA, and SNEDEKER, JESSE
- Abstract
In many contexts, pronouns are interpreted as referring to the character mentioned first in the previous sentence, an effect called the ‘first-mention bias’. While adults can rapidly use the first-mention bias to guide pronoun interpretation, it is unclear when this bias emerges during development. Curiously, experiments with children between two and three years old show successful use of order of mention, while experiments with older children (four to five years old) do not. While this could suggest U-shaped development, it could also reflect differences in the methodologies employed. We show that children can indeed use first-mention information, but do so too slowly to have been detected in previous work reporting null results. Comparison across the present and previously published studies suggests that the speed at which children deploy first-mention information increases greatly during the preschool years. [ABSTRACT FROM PUBLISHER]
- Published
- 2015
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
15. What is implicit causality?
- Author
-
Hartshorne, Joshua K.
- Abstract
In causal dependent clauses, the preferred referent of a pronoun varies systematically with the verb in the main clause (contrast Sally frightens Mary because she . . . with Sally loves Mary because she . . .). This ''implicit causality'' phenomenon is understood to reflect intuitions about who caused the event. Researchers have debated whether these intuitions are based on linguistic structure or instead a function of high-level, non-linguistic cognition. Two lines of evidence support the latter conclusion: implicit causality is related to a broad social judgement task, and it is affected by general knowledge about the participants in the event. On closer inspection, neither of these claims have been established. Eight new experiments find that (a) the relationship between implicit causality and the social judgement task is tenuous, and (b) previously employed event-participant manipulations have minimal to no effect on implicit causality. These findings support an account on which implicit causality is driven primarily by linguistic structure and only minimally by general knowledge and non-linguistic cognition. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2014
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
16. Verb argument structure predicts implicit causality: The advantages of finer-grained semantics.
- Author
-
Hartshorne, Joshua K. and Snedeker, Jesse
- Subjects
ATTRIBUTION (Social psychology) ,EXPERIMENTAL design ,COMPARATIVE grammar ,PSYCHOLINGUISTICS ,RESEARCH funding ,SEMANTICS ,T-test (Statistics) ,PHONOLOGICAL awareness ,DESCRIPTIVE statistics - Abstract
While the referent of a nonreflexive pronoun clearly depends on context, the nature of these contextual restrictions is controversial. The present study seeks to characterise one representation that guides pronoun resolution. Our focus is an effect known as “implicit causality”. In causal dependant clauses, the preferred referent of a pronoun varies systematically with the verb in the main clause (contrastSally frightened Mary because she …withSally feared Mary because she…). A number of researchers have tried to explain and predict such biases with reference to semantic classes of verbs. However, such studies have focused on a small number of specially selected verbs. In Experiment 1, we find that existing taxonomies perform near chance at predicting pronoun-resolution bias on a large set of representative verbs. However, a more fine-grained taxonomy recently proposed in the linguistics literature does significantly better. In Experiment 2, we tested all 264 verbs in two of the narrowly defined verb classes from this new taxonomy, finding that pronoun-resolution biases were categorically different. These findings suggest that the semantic structure of verbs tightly constrains the interpretation of pronouns in causal sentences, raising challenges for theories which posit that implicit causality biases reflect world knowledge or arbitrary lexical features. [ABSTRACT FROM PUBLISHER]
- Published
- 2013
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
17. Comment: Acquiring metaphors.
- Author
-
Hartshorne, Joshua K.
- Abstract
Lakoff (2016) describes an account of conceptual representation based in part on metaphor. Though promising, this account faces several challenges with respect to learning and development. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2016
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
18. Visual Working Memory Capacity and Proactive Interference.
- Author
-
Hartshorne, Joshua K.
- Subjects
SHORT-term memory ,MEMORY research ,EXPLICIT memory ,COGNITIVE interference ,TASKS ,ADULTS ,VISUAL acuity ,HUMAN-animal communication ,PHYSIOLOGY - Abstract
Background: Visual working memory capacity is extremely limited and appears to be relatively immune to practice effects or the use of explicit strategies. The recent discovery that visual working memory tasks, like verbal working memory tasks, are subject to proactive interference, coupled with the fact that typical visual working memory tasks are particularly conducive to proactive interference, suggests that visual working memory capacity may be systematically under-estimated. Methodology/Principal Findings: Working memory capacity was probed behaviorally in adult humans both in laboratory settings and via the Internet. Several experiments show that although the effect of proactive interference on visual working memory is significant and can last over several trials, it only changes the capacity estimate by about 15%. Conclusions/Significance: This study further confirms the sharp limitations on visual working memory capacity, both in absolute terms and relative to verbal working memory. It is suggested that future research take these limitations into account in understanding differences across a variety of tasks between human adults, prelinguistic infants and nonlinguistic animals. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2008
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
19. Why girls say ‘holded’ more than boys.
- Author
-
Hartshorne, Joshua K. and Ullman, Michael T.
- Subjects
CHILD psychology ,MEMORY ,GENDER differences (Psychology) ,PSYCHOLOGY ,MEMORIZATION ,RECOGNITION (Psychology) ,LANGUAGE acquisition ,PSYCHOLINGUISTICS ,LANGUAGE awareness in children - Abstract
Women are better than men at verbal memory tasks, such as remembering word lists. These tasks depend on declarative memory. The declarative/procedural model of language, which posits that the lexicon of stored words is part of declarative memory, while grammatical composition of complex forms depends on procedural memory, predicts a female superiority in aspects of lexical memory. Other neurocognitive models of language have not made this prediction. Here we examine the prediction in past-tense over-regularizations (e.g. holded ) produced by children. We expected that girls would remember irregular past-tense forms (held ) better than boys, and thus would over-regularize less. To our surprise, girls over-regularized far more than boys. We investigated potential explanations for this sex difference. Analyses showed that in girls but not boys, over-regularization rates correlated with measures of the number of similar-sounding regulars (folded, molded ). This sex difference in phonological neighborhood effects is taken to suggest that girls tend to produce over-regularizations in associative lexical memory, generalizing over stored neighboring regulars, while boys are more likely to depend upon rule-governed affixation (hold +-ed ). The finding is consistent with the hypothesis that, likely due to their superior lexical abilities, females tend to retrieve from memory complex forms (walked ) that men generally compose with the grammatical system (walk +-ed ). The results suggest that sex may be an important factor in the acquisition and computation of language. [ABSTRACT FROM AUTHOR]
- Published
- 2006
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
20. WHERE ARE THE TALKING ROBOTS?
- Author
-
Hartshorne, Joshua K.
- Subjects
INDUSTRIAL robots ,TECHNOLOGICAL innovations ,ROBOT programming ,GRAMMAR - Abstract
The article offers information on the development of speaking robots. It states that development of artificial speech is the recent updations in the invention of speaking robots. It mentions that more than technology its has been language which has been challenging in developing speaking robots. It highlights that the earliest attempt to create a speaking robot was by programming the machine with the rules of grammar by International Business Machines Corp. (IBM).
- Published
- 2011
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
21. Why Don't Babies Talk Like Adults?
- Author
-
Hartshorne, Joshua
- Subjects
INFANT development ,SPEECH perception in children ,BRAIN research ,SPEECH research ,HYPOTHESIS ,LEARNING - Abstract
The article presents information on why babies are not able to talk like adults. It is stated that to answer this question, researchers are uncovering clues about brain development and the mysterious process of learning a language. Scientists have settled on two reasonable possibilities, of which first is mental development hypothesis. This states that one-year-olds speak in baby talk because their immature brains cannot handle adult speech.
- Published
- 2009
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
22. Universality and diversity in human song.
- Author
-
Mehr, Samuel A., Singh, Manvir, Knox, Dean, Ketter, Daniel M., Pickens-Jones, Daniel, Atwood, S., Lucas, Christopher, Jacoby, Nori, Egner, Alena A., Hopkins, Erin J., Howard, Rhea M., Hartshorne, Joshua K., Jennings, Mariela V., Simson, Jan, Bainbridge, Constance M., Pinker, Steven, O’Donnell, Timothy J., Krasnow, Max M., and Glowacki, Luke
- Published
- 2019
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
23. Ruled by Birth Order?
- Author
-
Hartshorne, Joshua K.
- Subjects
BIRTH order ,FIRST-born children ,INTELLECT ,PERSONALITY ,FAMILY size ,ETHNICITY - Abstract
The article discusses studies which show that family position may truly affect intelligence and personality. There are many reasons that family size could affect people's predilections and personalities. More children mean that parental resources have to be spread more thinly. Perhaps more telling, family size is associated with many important social factors, such as ethnicity, education and wealth.
- Published
- 2010
- Full Text
- View/download PDF
24. Russia: Lake Baikal Siberia's Vast Treasure.
- Author
-
Berenson, Sheila and Hartshorne, Joshua K.
- Subjects
TRUCKS ,LAKES - Abstract
The article presents information on Lake Baikal, in south-central Russia, in Siberia. During the winter, cars and trucks can take shortcuts across it. Hundreds of cross-country skiers, skaters and hikers enjoy the lake. Lake Baikal is also said to be the oldest lake in the world. Around 25 million years old, it is more than ten times older than the second oldest lake, Lake Tanganyika in Africa.
- Published
- 2007
25. Lake Baikal.
- Author
-
Berenson, Sheila and Hartshorne, Joshua K.
- Subjects
ICE on rivers, lakes, etc. ,WINTER sports ,BOATS & boating ,FRESHWATER animals ,FRESHWATER plants - Abstract
The article informs that Lake Baikal in south-central Russia in Siberia is the oldest and largest freshwater lake in the world. It is 400 miles long, 50 miles wide, and more than a mile deep. During the winter, cars and trucks take shortcuts across it. Hundreds of cross-country skiers, skaters, and hikers spend the day on the lake. During warm, summer months, Russians use this inland playland for swimming and boating. Fishing goes on all year. About two-thirds of the animals and plants found here aren't found anywhere else in the world. These include freshwater seals, salmon, and sea sponges.
- Published
- 2006
26. Calling All Amateur Scientists.
- Author
-
Hartshorne, Joshua K.
- Subjects
SCIENCE projects ,LAUGHTER ,VOCABULARY ,COMPREHENSIVE instruction (Reading) ,LANGUAGE arts - Abstract
The article focuses on three mind-related projects including the Small World of Words, Baby Laughter Project and VerbCorner. Researcher Caspar Addyman hopes to discover when babies laugh and why through the Baby Laughter Project. Determination of what means is the main aim of the project VerbCorner undertaken by the Computational Cognitive Science Group at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology.
- Published
- 2014
Catalog
Discovery Service for Jio Institute Digital Library
For full access to our library's resources, please sign in.